• No results found

This study has made a contribution to the sentence processing literature by investigating the influence of lexical frequency on the subject object asymmetry in Dutch. Using the self-paced reading paradigm, the present study found overall processing difficulties for object RCs as compared to subject RCs which fits well in the current literature on RC processing. However,

unlike Johnson et al. (2011), no facilitating effect was found for lexical frequency. Object RCs with an infrequent NP1 were not read faster than those with a frequent NP1. It has been suggested that this may have been due to the absence of a difference in reading time between frequent and infrequent NPs, the imageability of the NPs, or syntactic differences between Dutch and English. The results are therefore best explained by a syntactic model based on memory limitations since object RCs require higher integration costs in Dutch as compared to subject RCs (Gibson, 1998).

A WM effect was found such that people with higher WM capacity processed object RCs slower than people with lower WM capacity. After controlling for accuracy, no effect of WM was found which supports this suggestion. It has been argued that this is due to a risky processing strategy employed by people with lower WM capacity. The suggestion was further strengthened by the fact that a triple interaction was found such that people with higher WM capacity processed object RCs with an infrequent NP1 more efficiently.

Two directions for future inquiries were noted in the discussion in order to investigate where the facilitating effect of frequency originated in Johnson et al. (2011). First, it was suggested that infrequent NPs are encoded better as they were read slower. Future studies could manipulate the presentation time for each word. If this suggestion is true, then the subject-object asymmetry would be reduced if the NP1 is presented longer. Second, it was suggested that there may have been a difference in imageability between the NPs in the present study and those in Johnson et al. (2011). Future studies could contrast NPs with low versus high imageability scores. Based on this suggestion, it is predicted that NPs with a low imageability result in better encoding and therefore reduce the subject-object asymmetry.

In terms of the usefulness of the sentences for the therapy programme, it was concluded that changes need to be made in order to represent different difficulty levels. It was proposed that the therapy material could exploit the well-tested animacy and pronoun cues in constructing RC exercises. Moreover, if any of the suggested future directions bear any truth, then these may be used as well. In any case, it is imperative that therapy programmes for PWA have a strong foundation in both theoretical and empirical works. The present study and its suggested future directions work to strengthen our understanding of RC processing which, in turn, allows us to develop efficient and well-structured therapy programmes that will benefit PWA as much as possible.

References

Baayen, R. H., Piepenbrock, R., & Gulikers, L. (1996). The CELEX lexical database (cd-rom).

Baguley, T. (2012). Serious stats. Basingstoke, UK: Palgrave Macmillan

Baluch, B., & Besner, D. (2001). Basic processes in reading: Semantics affects speeded naming of high-frequency words in an alphabetic script. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 55(1), 63.

Barbieri, E., Mack, J., Chiappetta, B., Europa, E., & Thompson, C. K. (2019). Recovery of offline and online sentence processing in aphasia: Language and domain general network neuroplasticity. Cortex, 120, 394–418.

Bastiaanse, R., & Edwards, S. (2004). Word order and finiteness in Dutch and English Broca’s and Wernicke’s aphasia. Brain and Language, 89, 91–107.

Bastiaanse, R., & Prins, R. S. (2013). Aphasia. In L. Cummings (Ed.), The Cambridge Handbook of Communication Disorders. Pp. 224-246. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Bastiaanse, R., & van Zonneveld, R. M. (2004). Broca's aphasia, verbs and the mental lexicon.

Brain and Language, 90(1-3), 198–202.

Bos, L.S., Van der Keur-Van Driel, A., & Visch-Brink, E.G. (2021). De ontwikkeling van een syntactisch therapieprogramma. Oral presentation at Aphasia Clinics 2021, Rotterdam, The Netherlands, March 12th.

Brown, C. M., Hagoort, P., & Vonk, W. (2000). Processing relative clauses in Dutch: An analysis with event-related potentials. Manuscript, Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics, Nijmegen.

Brysbaert, M. (2019). How many participants do we have to include in properly powered experiments? A tutorial of power analysis with reference tables. Journal of cognition, 2(1).

Buchsbaum, B. R., Baldo, J., Okada, K., Berman, K. F., Dronkers, N., D’Esposito, M., &

Hickok, G. (2011). Conduction aphasia, sensory-motor integration, and phonological short-term memory: An aggregate analysis of lesion and fMRI data. Brain and Language, 119(3), 119–128. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. Bandl.2010.12.001

Burkhardt, P., Avrutin, S., Piñango, M. M., & Ruigendijk, E. (2008). Slower-than-normal syntactic processing in agrammatic Broca's aphasia: Evidence from Dutch. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 21(2), 120-137.

Caplan, D., & Futter, C. (1986). Assignment of thematic roles to nouns in sentence comprehension by an agrammatic patient. Brain and Language 27(1). 117–134.

Caplan, D. D., Michaud, G., Michaud, G., Tripodis, J., & Tripodis, Y. (2011). Effects of age, speed of processing, and working memory on comprehension of sentences with relative clauses. Psychology and Aging, 26(2), 439–450. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0021837 Caplan, D., & Waters, G. S. (1995). Aphasic disorders of syntactic comprehension and working

memory capacity. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 12, 637–649.

Caplan, D., & Waters, G. S. (1999). Verbal working memory and sentence comprehension.

The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22(1), 77–94.

Caramazza, A., & Zurif, E. B. (1976). Dissociation of algorithmic and heuristic processes in language comprehension: Evidence from aphasia. Brain and language, 3(4), 572-582.

Cho-Reyes, S., & Thompson, C. K. (2012). Verb and sentence production and comprehension in aphasia: Northwestern Assessment of Verbs and Sentences (NAVS). Aphasiology, 26(10), 1250-1277.

Christianson, K. (2016). When language comprehension goes wrong for the right reasons:

Good-enough, underspecified, or shallow language processing. Quarterly journal of experimental psychology, 69(5), 817-828.

Christianson, K., Williams, C. C., Zacks, R. T., & Ferreira, F. (2006). Younger and older adults'" good-enough" interpretations of garden-path sentences. Discourse processes, 42(2), 205-238.

Clough, S., & Gordon, J. K. (2020). Fluent or nonfluent? Part A. Underlying contributors to categorical classifications of fluency in aphasia. Aphasiology, 34(5), 515-539.

Crutch, S. J., & Warrington, E. K. (2005). Abstract and concrete concepts have structurally different representational frameworks. Brain, 128(3), 615-627.

den Besten, H. (1990). Studies in West-Germanic syntax. PhD thesis, University of Tilburg.

den Ouden, D.-B., Malyutina, S., Basilakos, A., Bonilha, L., Gleichgerrcht, E., Yourganov, G.,

… Fridriksson, J. (2019). Cortical and structural connectivity damage correlated with impaired syntactic processing in aphasia. Human Brain Mapping, 40(7), 2153–2173.

https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.24514

Christiansen, M. H., & MacDonald, M. C. (2009). A usage‐based approach to recursion in sentence processing. Language Learning, 59, 126-161.

Engelmann, F., & Vasishth, S. (2009). Processing grammatical and ungrammatical center embeddings in English and German: A computational model. In Proceedings of the Ninth International Conference on Cognitive Modeling, Manchester, UK (pp. 240-45).

Faroqi-Shah, Y., & Thomson, C. K. (2003). Effect of lexical cues on the production of active and passive sentences in Broca’s and Wernicke’s aphasia. Brain and Language, 85, 409–426.

Findelsberger, E., Hutzler, F., & Hawelka, S. (2019). Spill the load: Mixed evidence for a foveal load effect, reliable evidence for a spillover effect in eye-movement control during reading. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 81(5), 1442-1453.

Frazier, L. (1987). Syntactic processing: evidence from Dutch. Natural Language & Linguistic Theory, 5(4), 519-559.

Friedmann, N. (2000). Moving verbs in agrammatic production. In R. Bastiaanse & Y.

Grodzinsky (Eds.), Grammatical disorders in aphasia: a neurolinguistic perspective, pp. 152-70. London: Whurr.

Friedmann, N., & Shapiro, L. P. (2003). Agrammatic comprehension of simple active sentences with moved constituents: hebrew osv and ovs structures. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 46(2), 288–97.

Gennari, S.P, and MacDonald, M.C. (2008). Semantic indeterminacy in object relative clauses.

Journal of Memory and Language, 58(2). 161–87.

Gennari, S.P, and MacDonald, M.C. (2009). Linking production and comprehension processes:

the case of relative clauses. Cognition 111(1). 1–23.

Gibson, E. (1998). Linguistic complexity: Locality of syntactic dependencies. Cognition, 68(1), 1-76.

Goodglass, H., & Kaplan, E. (1983). The Assessment of Aphasia and Related Disorders.

Philadelphia, PA: Lea & Febiger

Gordon, P. C., Hendrick, R., & Johnson, M. (2004). Effects of noun phrase type on sentence complexity. Journal of memory and Language, 51(1), 97-114.

Gordon, P. C., Hendrick, R., & Johnson, M. (2001). Memory interference during language processing. Journal of experimental psychology: learning, memory, and cognition, 27(6), 1411.

Gordon, P. C., Hendrick, R., Johnson, M., & Lee, Y. (2006). Similarity-based interference during language comprehension: Evidence from eye tracking during reading. Journal of experimental psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 32(6), 1304.

Gordon, P. C., & Lowder, M. W. (2012). Complex sentence processing: A review of theoretical perspectives on the comprehension of relative clauses. Language and Linguistics Compass, 6(7), 403-415.

Gordon, P. C., Lowder, M. W., & Hoedemaker, R. S. (2016). Reading in normally aging adults.

In H. H. Wright (Ed.). Cognition, Language and Aging. (pp. 165–191). Amsterdam:

John Benjamins Publishing.

Griffin, Z.M., & Bock, K. (1998). Constraint, word frequency, and the relationship between lexical processing levels in spoken word production. Journal of Memory and Language, 38, 313–338.

Grodzinsky, Y. (1989). Agrammatic comprehension of relative clauses. Brain and Language, 37(3), 480-499.

Grodzinsky, Y. (2000). The neurology of syntax: Language use without Broca's area.

Behavioral and brain sciences, 23(1), 1-21.

Grodzinsky, Y., Pinango, M. M., Zurif, E., & Drai, D. (1999). The critical role of group studies in neuropsychology: Comprehension regularities in Broca's aphasia. Brain and language, 67(2), 134-147.

Gundel, J. K., Hedberg, N., & Zacharski, R. (1993). Cognitive status and the form of referring expressions in discourse. Language, 69(2), 274-307.

Hale, J. (2001). A probabilistic Earley parser as a psycholinguistic model. In Proceedings of NAACL (Vol. 2, pp. 159–166).

Hoffman, P., Jefferies, E., & Ralph, M. A. L. (2011). Remembering ‘zeal’but not ‘thing’:

Reverse frequency effects as a consequence of deregulated semantic processing.

Neuropsychologia, 49(3), 580-584.

Jescheniak, J.D., & Levelt, W.J.M. (1994). Word frequency effects in speech production:

Retrieval of syntactic information and of phonological form. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 20, 824–843.

Johnson, M. L., Lowder, M. W., & Gordon, P. C. (2011). The sentence-composition effect:

Processing of complex sentences depends on the configuration of common noun phrases versus unusual noun phrases. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 140(4), 707-724.

Kaan, E., 1997. Processing Subject–Object Ambiguities in Dutch. PhD thesis, University of Groningen.

Kertesz, A. (2006). Western Aphasia Battery-Revised. San Antonio, TX: Pearson.

Kliegl, R., Grabner, E., Rolfs, M., & Engbert, R. (2004). Length, frequency, and predictability effects of words on eye movements in reading. European journal of cognitive psychology, 16(1-2), 262-284.

Koster, J. (1975). Dutch as an SOV language. Linguistic Analysis, 1, 111–136.

Kroll, J. F., & Merves, J. S. (1986). Lexical access for concrete and abstract words. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 12(1), 92.

Kuznetsova, A., Brockhoff, P.B., & Christensen, R.H. (2017). lmerTest package: tests in linear mixed effects models. Journal of Statistical Software, 82(13), 1–26.

Levelt, W.J.M., Praamstra, P., Meyer, A.S., & Salmelin, R. (1998). An MEG study of picture naming. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 10, 553–567.

Levy, R. (2008). Expectation-based syntactic comprehension. Cognition, 106(3), 1126-1177.

Lukatela, K., Shankweiler, D., & Crain, S. (1995). Syntactic processing in agrammatic aphasia by speakers of a Slavic language. Brain and Language, 49(1), 50-76.

MacWhinney, B. (1977). Starting points. Language, 152-168.

Mak, W. M., Vonk, W., & Schriefers, H. (2002). The influence of animacy on relative clause processing. Journal of memory and language, 47(1), 50-68.

Mak, W. M., Vonk, W., & Schriefers, H. (2006). Animacy in processing relative clauses: The hikers that rocks crush. Journal of memory and language, 54(4), 466-490.

Mak, W. M., Vonk, W., & Schriefers, H. (2008). Discourse structure and relative clause processing. Memory & Cognition, 36(1), 170-181.

Mecklinger, A., Schriefers, H., Steinhauer, K., & Friederici, A. D. (1995). Processing relative clauses varying on syntactic and semantic dimensions: An analysis with event-related potentials. Memory & Cognition, 23, 477–494.

Mitchell, D. C. (1984). An evaluation of subject-paced reading tasks and other methods of investigating immediate processes in reading. In D. E. Kieras and M.A.Just (Eds.), New Methods in Reading Comprehension Research, pp. 69-90. New York: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Oldfield, R. C., & Wingfield, A. (1965). Response latencies in naming objects. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 17(4), 273-281.

Powell, M. J. D. (2009). The BOBYQA algorithm for bound constrained optimization without derivatives. Technical Report. Cambridge: Department of Applied Mathematics and Theoretical Physics, University of Cambridge

Purdy, M. (2002). Executive function ability in persons with aphasia. Aphasiology, 16(4-6), 549-557.

Qualtrics. (2019). Version 20.05. Utah, USA. URL https://www.qualtrics.com.

Rayner, K., Reichle, E. D., Stroud, M. J., Williams, C. C., & Pollatsek, A. (2006). The effect of word frequency, word predictability, and font difficulty on the eye movements of young and older readers. Psychology and aging, 21(3), 448.

Roberts, R., & Gibson, E. (2002). Individual differences in sentence memory. Journal of psycholinguistic research, 31(6), 573-598.

Schad, D. J., Vasishth, S., Hohenstein, S., & Kliegl, R. (2020). How to capitalize on a priori contrasts in linear (mixed) models: A tutorial. Journal of Memory and Language, 110, 1-40.

Schriefers, H., Friederici, A. D., & Kühn, K. (1995). The processing of locally ambiguous relative clauses in German. Journal of Memory and Language, 34, 499–520.

Stine-Morrow, E. A. L., Ryan, S., & Leonard, J. S. (2000). Age differences in on-line syntactic processing. Experimental Aging Research, 26, 315–322.

doi:10.1080/036107300750015714

Thompson, C. K. (2019). Neurocognitive recovery of sentence processing in aphasia. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 62(11), 3947-3972.

https://doi.org/10.1044/2019_JSLHR-L-RSNP-19-0219

Thompson, C. K., Shapiro, L. P., Kiran, S., & Sobecks, J. (2003). The role of syntactic complexity in treatment of sentence deficits in agrammatic aphasia. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research, 46(3), 591-607

Traxler, Matthew J., Robin K. Morris, and Rachel E. Seely. 2002. Processing subject and object relative clauses: evidence from eye movements. Journal of Memory and Language 47.

69–90.

Traxler, Matthew J., Rihana S. Williams, Shelley A. Blozis, and Robin K. Morris. 2005.

Working memory, animacy, and verb class in the processing of relative clauses. Journal of Memory and Language 53. 204–24.

Vaartjes, I., Reitsma, J. B., De Bruin, A., Berger‐van Sijl, M., Bos, M. J., Breteler, M. M. B., ... & Bots, M. L. (2008). Nationwide incidence of first stroke and TIA in the Netherlands. European journal of neurology, 15(12), 1315-1323.

van Hell, J. G., & de Groot, A. M. D. (1998). Disentangling context availability and concreteness in lexical decision and word translation. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 51(1), 41-63.

Vet, D.J. (2021). ED- Experiment Designer. URL https://www.fon.hum.uva.nl/dirk/ed.php Visch-Brink, E.G. (2019). BOX, een lexicaal therapieprogramma. Houten: Bohn-Stafleu.

Visch-Brink, E., Vandenborre, D., Jung de Smet, H, Mariën, P. (2014). Comprehensive Aphasia Test, CAT-NL. Amsterdam: Pearson

Visch-Brink, E.G., & De Waard-van Rijn, M. (2018). FIKS, een fonologisch therapieprogramma. Houten: Bohn-Stafleu.

Vonk, W., Brown, C. M., & Hagoort, P. (2000). Processing relative clauses in Dutch: An analysis with self-paced moving window and eye-movement reading times. Manuscript, Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics, Nijmegen.

Warren, T., & Gibson, E. (2002). The influence of referential processing on sentence complexity. Cognition, 85(1), 79-112.

Wilson, S. M., Henry, M. L., Besbris, M., Ogar, J. M., Dronkers, N. F., Jarrold, W., … Gorno-Tempini, M. L. (2010). Connected speech production in three variants of primary progressive aphasia. Brain, 133(7), 2069–2088. https://doi.org/10.1093/ brain/awq129 Van Witteloostuijn, M., Lammertink, I., Boersma, P., Wijnen, F., & Rispens, J. (2019).

Assessing visual statistical learning in early-school-aged children: The usefulness of an online reaction time measure. Frontiers in psychology, 10, 2051.

Yourganov, G., Smith, K. G., Fridriksson, J., & Rorden, C. (2015). Predicting aphasia type from brain damage measured with structural MRI. Cortex, 73, 203–215.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j. Cortex.2015.09.005

Appendix A: Lexical items and their log frequencies

Green shading denote high frequency items and red shading low frequency items

Word Letters Syllables Log frequency

student 7 2 1.6902

dokter 6 2 2.1614

kapitein 8 3 1.9956

gevangene 9 4 1.5441

schrijver 9 2 2.0755

directeur 9 3 1.7559

leraar 6 2 1.8633

agent 5 2 1.6628

politicus 9 4 1.5315

leerling 8 2 1.7634

huisarts 8 2 1.6128

minister 8 3 2.0453

arbeider 8 3 1.7853

bewoner 7 3 1.5441

eigenaar 8 3 1.6128

rechter 7 2 1.8633

chauffeur 9 2 1.6628

president 9 3 1.7076

vriendin 8 2 1.8573

Engelsman 9 3 1.5315

professor 9 3 1.716

ambtenaar 9 3 1.7634

werknemer 9 3 1.6812

kennis 6 2 1.5682

zuster 6 2 2.017

burgemeester 12 4 1.6232

luitenant 9 3 1.6435

kunstenaar 10 3 1.6232

medewerker 10 4 1.5798

officier 8 3 1.8451

dichter 7 2 1.9138

client 6 2 1.7404

reisleider 10 3 0.301

raadgever 9 3 0.4771

verloofde 9 3 0.4771

stewardess 10 3 0.4771

decaan 6 2 0.301

telefoniste 11 5 0.4771

juwelier 8 3 0.301

wandelaar 9 3 0.6021

wielrenner 10 3 0.301

molenaar 8 3 0.301

rekruut 7 2 0.301

pianist 7 3 0.6021

makelaar 8 3 0.6021

detective 9 4 0.6021

cursist 7 2 0.301

verpleger 9 3 0.6021

barman 6 2 0.4771

bokser 6 2 0.4771

duiker 6 2 0.4771

wachter 7 2 0.4771

vertaler 8 3 0.4771

metselaar 9 3 0.4771

conrector 9 3 0.4771

smokkelaar 10 3 0.4771

commentator 11 4 0.4771

mentor 6 2 0.301

turner 6 2 0.301

activist 8 3 0.301

aannemer 8 3 0.6021

bouwvakker 10 3 0.301

loodgieter 10 3 0.301

maniak 6 3 0.301

Appendix B: Experimental sentences used per condition

HF - LF Subject

1. De nieuwe studenten, die de knappe barman geroepen hadden, bestelden een lekker biertje.

2. De handige dokter, die de boksers behandeld had, keek vol spanning naar de rest van de wedstrijd.

3. De ervaren kapitein, die de duikers begroet heeft, loopt over het mooie schip.

4. De boze gevangenen, die de wachter genegeerd hebben, protesteren op de binnenplaats.

5. De beroemde schrijver, die de vertalers gebeld heeft, brengt een nieuw boek uit.

6. De klunzige directeur, die de metselaars aangenomen had, stootte de koffie om.

7. De jonge leraren, die de conrector gesproken hebben, waren net afgestudeerd.

8. De dappere agenten, die de beruchte smokkelaar betrapt hadden, kregen een medaille.

LF - HF Subject

1. De leuke reisleiders, die de chauffeur bedankt hadden, organiseerden een mooie trip.

2. De slimme raadgevers, die de president bijgestaan hebben, geven altijd goed advies.

3. De jonge verloofde, die de vriendinnen uitgenodigd had, ging binnenkort trouwen.

4. De aardige stewardess, die de Engelsmannen begeleid heeft, ruimt de koffers op 5. De vriendelijke decaan, die de professoren rondgeleid heeft, loopt naar de receptie 6. De goede telefonistes, die de ambtenaar ondersteund hebben, nemen een

welverdiende pauze

7. De verdrietige juweliers, die de werknemer ontslagen hadden, waren failliet gegaan 8. De sportieve wandelaar, die de kennissen bezocht had, liep ook weer naar huis

HF - LF Object

1. De belangrijke politici, die de commentator geïnterviewd had, verschenen op tv.

2. De brutale leerlingen, die de mentor toegesproken heeft, spijbelen te veel.

3. De oude huisarts, die de turners bezocht hebben, staat erg goed bekend.

4. De corrupte minister, die de activisten beschuldigd hadden, nam uiteindelijk ontslag.

5. De goede arbeiders, die de aannemer ingehuurd had, bouwden het deftige huis.

6. De ongeruste bewoner, die de bouwvakkers geholpen hadden, liet het lekke dak repareren.

GERELATEERDE DOCUMENTEN