• No results found

Also, there have been investigations about the formants and the duration of the

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Share "Also, there have been investigations about the formants and the duration of the "

Copied!
11
0
0

Bezig met laden.... (Bekijk nu de volledige tekst)

Hele tekst

(1)

Summary

1. Introduction

In this paper we will investigate the influence of the weakening of a sibilant on the preceding vowel in Andalusian Spanish, spoken in the south of Spain, in Andalusia. It is one of the most complicated varieties of the Spanish language, and the use of the sibilants can be seen as one of the most interesting characteristics. Until now, there have been numerous studies about the Andalusian vowels and the weakening of /s/ and /θ/, but it is quite difficult to find studies about the influence the weakening has on the quality of the vowels. One of the main problems linguists have, when they analyze aspiration, is its detection. The majority simply listens to categorize the sounds, but since this method is very subjective, mistakes are easy to make. Therefore, in this paper we will offer a more objective method that can facilitate the categorization.

Alarcos Llorach (1958: 193) is one of the few linguists that have studied the influence the weakening of the sibilant has on the preceding vowel. He found that especially the vowels of the last syllable of a word change, mostly because of the aspiration and the complete loss of the final /s/. The consequence is that these vowels become more open and have a longer duration in general.

Also, there have been investigations about the formants and the duration of the

Andalusian vowels. In Alonso, Zamora Vicente & Canelloa de Zamora (1950), the authors

discovered that vowels in general tend to be more open in plural, but more closed in

singular. In other words, in plural they have a higher F1, but it is lower in singular. Also

Martínez Melgar (1986; 1992), who investigated the speech of Almería, Córdoba, Jaen and

Granada, got more or less the same results. In both of her studies, Martínez Melgar also

investigates the duration of the vowels in different positions within the word. The results of

the first article show that the vowels of the initial syllable are usually longer in singular than

in plural. In stressed syllables, there is not a very clear tendency, whereas the vowels in final

syllables actually have a longer duration in plural, the only exception being [ɑ] (Martínez

Melgar, 1986). In the other article, the results show quite some differences. Now, the vowels

in final position are not longer in plural anymore, but they are consistently shorter in plural

than in singular.

(2)

Concerning the weakening of sibilants, three types can be distinguished. These are aspiration, (partial or total) assimilation and the total loss of /s/ or /θ/. In this paper, the assimilation will not be investigated, but we will concentrate on the retention, the aspiration and the elimination of the sibilant. Sibilants are frequently weakened when followed by /p/, /t/ or /k/. Besides that, weakening usually takes place within a word (as in “este”), at the end of a word before a pause (as in “son estos”) and at the end of a word before the consonant of the following word (as in “estas casas”). The weakened sibilant always forms the coda of a syllable and is always preceded by a vowel (Alvar, 1996: 243; Penny, 2000: 122).

Aspiration can be found not only in Andalusia, but also in some parts of Extremadura, Castilla la Mancha, Murcia and various regions of South America. This form of weakening is socially well accepted, which is why it can be encountered in all social classes. Many linguists interpret the aspiration as a continuation of the preceding vowel (Terrell, 1979: 602; Alarcos Llorach, 1958: 203). One of the main reasons to do so, is that it is rather hard to detect the [h]. Depending on the interpretation linguists choose, the results of their analysis can show many differences. Therefore, a universal objective method would offer great advantages. In order to detect aspiration, different methods are used. Gerfen (2002), for example, uses spectrograms as well as sound waves to distinguish aspirated sibilants from eliminated ones.

Torreira (2006: 115) only uses the sound waves to measure the voice onset time (VOT). To locate the beginning and the end of the aspiration, he uses the energy of the second formant.

As mentioned before, one of the main problems in studies about aspiration, is the lack of objectivity. The voiceless [h] can often be hard to perceive, which is why linguists who do not use an objective method can easily make mistakes. Often, they try to hear the

difference between [s], [h] and [Ø], on which they based their categorization. According to Caravedo (2006: 113-114), a speaker of a certain language uses pre-established referential models, that guide their observations. In the case of native speakers, the influence of these models is even bigger.

2. Corpus and method

For the collection of data, we used eleven videos of YouTube, generally containing

interviews with famous artists. The videos have been converted to MP3 files, followed by an

(3)

optimalization of the fragments, using Adobe Audition 3.0. First, the background noise has been reduced by 90%. Then, we selected all the words that contained a coda formed by a (weakened) sibilant. These selected words have been saved as WAV files. After that, all the data has been normalized to 100%.

For the phonetic analysis, we used Praat (Boersma & Weenink, 2013). First of all, the beginning and the end of each word, (weakened) sibilant and preceding vowel were localized, using a spectrogram and oscillogram. In order to determine the formants, we used the point in which the intensity of the vowel was highest. In some cases, Praat was not able to analyze the formants well, so we tried to determine the average formant values of the whole vowel. Unfortunately, this also did not work, which is why these data have been deleted. In figure 1, an example of the annotation of the word “artistas” (realized as [ɑrtiØtɑs]) can be seen.

Figure 1: The annotation of the word “artistas”: [ɑrtiØtɑs].

For the normalization of the vowel duration, the duration of the vowel was divided by the duration of the entire word, and the outcome was multiplied by hundred. This way, the result is a percentage that indicates the proportion of the vowel with respect to the word. In this study, we did not add the duration of the aspiration to the total vowel duration, which is something many linguists do. Also some of the formants had to be normalized, since

a r t i t a s

66 ms 100 ms

375 Hz 611 Hz

2036 Hz 1571 Hz

Time (s)

0 0.6193

0 0.619311166

s32+33_nuestros

(4)

some of the informants were female. For the normalization, we used the coefficient of 1.22 that can be used to divide the female formants with, according to Martínez Celdrán (1996:

68).

In order to distinguish elimination from a (weakened) sibilant, the spectrogram and oscillogram can be used, by looking whether there is any realization of the sibilant or not.

Concerning [s] and [h], their categorization can be relatively hard. It is known, however, that sibilants have more energy in the high frequency area, in the region between 4 kHz and 6 kHz (Yu, 1999: 1). The [h], on the contrary, does not show that much energy in this area, but has its energy spread across the spectrogram (Rothenberg, 1974: 2; Stevens, 2007: 429). To facilitate the categorization of the preserved and aspirated sibilants, we tried to design a model. First, the phone in question was selected and extracted with Praat (Boersma &

Weenink, 2013). Then, we measured the average intensity of the sound. Since [s] has more energy in the high frequency area, we decided to use a filter of 5 kHz to 6 kHz, using the pass Hann band option in Praat. This filter only filters the energy of the chosen area, so that analysis can be made in this specific part of the sound. We expect that, after the filtering process, the average intensity of [s] in the region of 5 kHz to 6 kHz is still high, while the average intensity of [h] should be relatively lower, due to the fact that this phone has its energy spread across the spectrogram, mainly below 5 kHz.

Both the values before filtering as well as the values after filtering have been used, in order to calculate the decrease of intensity in percentages, using the following formula: ((y- x)/x)*100%. The value ‘x’ stands for the intensity before filtering, while ‘y’ stands for the intensity after filtering. After having calculated the decrease of intensity of every phone, we calculated the average decrease of intensity of [s] and [h]. For [s], the average is 22.5% and for [h], it is 43.6%, almost two times higher. However, we need a point of reference so that it can be determined whether a phone is [s] or [h]. In 83.1% of the cases of [s], the decrease of intensity is less than 34%. The decrease is more than 34% in 85.5% of the cases of [h].

So, when we want to distinguish [h] from [s], this new method can be used. First, put

the phone in Praat and measure the average intensity. Then, apply a filter of 5 kHz to 6 kHz,

using the pass Hann band option. After that, measure the average intensity of the sound in

this filtered area, so that the two values can be compared. Finally, see whether the decrease

of intensity is more or less than 34%. If it is less, it is very likely to be an [s], but when the

percentage is higher than 34%, it is much more likely to be an [h].

(5)

3. The results

For this paper, we have categorized the functions of all the sibilants (implosive as well as final ones) into two categories: /s/ morfema and /s/ no morfema. The sibilants that are /s/

morfema, have a certain morphological relevance, which is why the total loss of the sibilant could cause trouble concerning the comprehension. In the category /s/ no morfema, the sibilants are not necessary for a good understanding. For this reason, the /s or /θ/ could easily be eliminated without causing any problems. In table 1, the distribution of the sibilants can be found, according to our categorization.

Table 1: Distribution of the sibilants into two groups: /s/ no morfema and /s/ morfema.

X

2

=13.1; p<0.001 /s/ no morfema /s/ morfema

sibilant 47 / 32% 24 / 27%

aspiration 31 / 21% 38 / 43%

elimination 69 / 47% 27 / 30%

Two oppositions can be made: on the one hand, there is the opposition between the retention of the sibilant and the weakened variant, and on the other hand, the opposition between a perceptible variant ([s] or [h]) and an imperceptible one (elimination). It is to be expected that in the cases of /s/ no morfema, more relaxed forms of the sibilant are used. In the cases of /s/ morfema, however, the use of elimination will be disfavored. In table 2 and 3, the data of the two different oppositions have been organized.

Table 2: The opposition between the retention of the sibilant and the weakened variant.

X

2

=0.7; p<0.5 /s/ no morfema /s/ morfema

sibilant 47 / 32% 24 / 27%

aspiration +

elimination 100 / 68% 65 / 73%

Table 3: The opposition between a perceptible and an imperceptible variant.

X

2

=6.3; p<0.05 /s/ no morfema /s/ morfema sibilant +

aspiration 78 / 53% 62 / 70%

elimination 69 / 47% 27 / 30%

(6)

Table 3 represents our expectations better, as well as the hypothesis of Terrell (1979:

602), who supposes that the sibilant will be eliminated more frequently when it is redundant. On the other hand, the sibilant will be conserved (entirely or aspirated) especially when the sibilant is an /s/ morfema. Since the last table represents our expectations better, table 2 is not needed. For this reason, we will use the division between a perceptible and an imperceptible variant throughout this paper.

Concerning the phonological context, sibilants often are retained in a prevocalic context, due to the fact they are usually /s/ morfema. Terrell (1979: 607) also mentions that retention of the sibilant often occurs before a pause and before a vowel. In a preconsonantic context, however, it is more likely to encounter an /s/ no morfema. Silva-Corvalán (2001: 23) affirms that the retention of a sibilant in this context is very rare. (Alvar, 1996: 243) says that sibilants are weakened even more frequently when followed by /p/, /t/ or /k/. In table 4, the data of this analysis have been organized.

Table 4: Distribution of sibilant/aspiration versus elimination according to the phonological context

1

.

X

2

=60.7;

p<0.001 SV SP SC Sptk

sibilant +

aspiration 73 / 94% 17 / 59% 9 / 43% 41 / 38%

elimination 5 / 6% 12 / 41% 12 / 57% 67 / 62%

As can be seen in the table, the Andalusian speakers indeed use a perceptible variant of the /s/ or /θ/ in a prevocalic or prepausal context. When these phonemes are followed by a consonant, they are more likely to be eliminated. An explanation for these results could be found in the Optimality Theory. In this theory, it is supposed that there exists a universal syllable that has a consonant-vowel structure (Prince & Smolensky, 2004: 105-106). This could be why native speakers of the Andalusian variant pronounce the underlined /s/ in “las alas” as [s] or [h], so that the following structure would be obtained: CVCVCVC. If the sibilant would have been eliminated, the structure would have been CVVCVC. Of course, the morphological relevance of the sibilant is another reason to retain the sound.

1 SV = sibilant before a vowel; SP = sibilant before a pause; SC = sibilant before a consonant that is not /p/, /t/ nor /k/; Sptk = sibilant before the phoneme /p/, /t/ or /k/.

(7)

It is also very interesting to investigate the position of the sibilant, to see whether this factor has any influence on the use of /s/ and /θ/. In this paper, we will only analyze the initial and final position

2

, since there were not many data of sibilants in other positions in our corpus. We expect the sibilant to be eliminated more frequently in initial syllables, because they are usually /s/ no morfema. On the other hand, we expect that there is a preference for retaining or aspirating the sibilant in final syllables, when it generally does have a morphematical function. In table 5 can be seen to which degree the sibilants are (im)perceptible in initial and final syllable. As the table shows, our expectations are confirmed.

Table 5: Distribution of sibilant/aspiration versus elimination, according to the position.

X

2

=23.5, p<0.001

/s/ in initial syllable

/s/ in final syllable sibilant +

aspiration 28 / 37% 112 / 70%

elimination 48 / 63% 48 / 30%

In studies about the influence weakening of the sibilant has on the preceding vowel, it is said that the vowel becomes opener and longer (Alarcos Llorach, 1958: 196). In Martínez Melgar (1986: 231), the author found that initial vowels tend to be longer than final ones

3

. We expect that the vowels in final position will be longer than in initial position. Also, we expect the vowels before elimination to be longer in comparison with those before sibilant or aspiration. In table 6, the results of this analysis can be seen. We have added a column with monosyllabic words, since in this type of words there is relatively much more retention and aspiration of the sibilant.

2 In this paper we will use the terms initial position and final position to indicate a sibilant that appears in the first or last syllable of a word, respectively.

3 In this paper we will use the terms initial vowel and final vowel to indicate a vowel that appears in the first or last syllable of a word, respectively.

(8)

Table 6: The vowel duration in initial position, final position and monosyllabic words.

initial position

final position

mono- syllabic before

sibilant + aspiration

18.5% 22.5% 42.2%

before

elimination 21.2% 26.6% 63.6%

In the first place, it can be seen that the vowels before [s] or [h], as well as those before elimination, are longer in final position than in initial position, as expected. In second place, the vowels before elimination have a longer duration in comparison with those that appear in the other phonetic context.

According to the linguists, not only the vowel duration changes because of the weakening of the sibilants, but also the way of articulation. For this reason, we have analyzed the first two formants of each vowel. We expect the vowel to be more open before elimination, just like it is described in the articles on this subject. The results have been put in table 7, which presents percentages that indicate the increase or decrease of the formants before elimination with respect to the vowels before sibilant or aspiration. In other words, if an [ɔ] before [s] or [h] has an F1 of 577 Hz, but before elimination it has an F1 of 478 Hz, the percentual change is ((478-577)/577)*100%= -17%.

Table 7: Percentual changes of the formants of the vowels before elimination with respect to the formants of the vowels before sibilant or aspiration

4

.

initial position final position

[ɑ] [ɛ] [ɔ] [i] [u] [ɑ] [ɛ] [ɔ] [i] [u]

F1 * -2% -17% * -16% -6% -7% -4% +11% /

F2 * +5% -18% * +18% -7% -16% -4% +4% /

Regarding the first formant, it can be seen that generally the vowels before elimination are more closed than before a perceptible variant. This means that our hypothesis has not been confirmed. One explanation could be that the vowels before [Ø]

tend to centralize, or in other words, their first formants move towards a central point

4 The symbol * indicates that there was no comparison possible, since there were no data for one of the groups. The symbol / indicates that there were no data of none of the two groups.

(9)

around 400 Hz. Another explanation has to do with the relevance of the sibilant. Eliminated sibilants are usually /s/ no morfema, which is why they are communicatively less important.

For this reason, the preceding vowel could also be less open without causing any problems.

This explanation can be related to the principle of economy, which defines itself as the desire to invest the least amount of effort to obtain the maximum of clarity (De Jonge, 2000:

9). The second formant seems to show another tendency. The F2 of the (semi)open vowels, which are [ɑ], [ɛ] and [ɔ], is lower when the vowel stands before an eliminated sibilant. In the case of the closed vowels, which are [i] y [u], the F2 tends to be higher in the phonetic context we just mentioned.

As mentioned before, the Andalusian speakers use to retain the sibilant when it appears in final position. However, there is a difference between the final /s/ of “estamos”

and “estás”: in the first word, the last syllable is not stressed, whereas in the last word it actually is. Since the unstressed syllable is weaker and often contains an /s/ no morfema, we expect to see more elimination in this type of syllable. On the other hand, the expectation is that a perceptible variant will be used more frequently when it appears in a stressed syllable, because it is stronger and the sibilant often has a morphematic function. See table 8 for the results. As can be seen, our hypotheses have been confirmed.

Table 8: Distribution of sibilant/aspiration versus elimination, according to the stress

5

. X

2

=13.8,

p<0.001

stressed syllable

unstressed syllable sibilant +

aspiration 84 / 71% 56 / 47%

elimination 34 / 29% 62 / 53%

In this part of the analysis, we also analyzed the vowel duration in the different types of syllable. As we have seen in the analysis of the position, the vowels tend to have a longer duration when they precede the total loss of a sibilant. This is confirmed by Alarcos Llorach (1958: 196). For this reason, we will use that as our hypothesis here as well. Besides that, it is to be expected that the vowels have a longer duration when they are in a stressed syllable, because this is a stronger syllable than the unstressed one.

5 In the category stressed syllable the monosyllabic words have also been included.

(10)

Table 9: The vowel duration in unstressed syllable, stressed syllable and monosyllabic words.

unstressed syllable

stressed syllable

mono- syllabic sibilant +

aspiration 20.0% 23.1% 42.2%

elimination 23.1% 25.0% 63.6%

In the first place, the vowels in both phonetic contexts are longer in stressed syllables than in unstressed syllables, as expected. Also, the vowels before [Ø] have a longer duration if they are compared with those that appear before sibilant or aspiration.

From the analysis of the position resulted that the vowel before elimination were more closed. The F2 of the (semi)open vowels was lower when they appeared before elimination, while the second formant of the closed vowels was higher in this context. For the analysis of the formants of the vowels in (un)stressed syllables, we will use these results as our hypothesis. The percentages can be found in table 10.

Table 10: Percentual changes of the formants of the vowels before elimination with respect to the formants of the vowels before sibilant or aspiration.

stressed syllable unstressed syllable [ɑ] [ɛ] [ɔ] [i] [u] [ɑ] [ɛ] [ɔ] [i] [u]

F1 -2% -12% -3% +1% -14% -6% +4% -5% * *

F2 -13% -1% -1% +5% +11% -19% -4% -4% * *

In general, the results are more or less the same as those of the analysis of the position. The vowels tend to close when they appear before the total loss of a sibilant. The absolute data, that can be found in the tables 20 and 21 of the appendix 4, correspond with our theory of the centralization of the F1. Also the other explanations, based on the morphologic relevance and the theory of economy, could be valid. The second formant also follows the same tendency as seen before. The F2 of the (semi)open vowels is lower when they appear before elimination. The closed vowels, on the contrary, have a higher F2 when they appear in this phonetic context.

In conclusion, it could be said that neither phonology nor phonetics can exist without

the morphology. In the majority of the cases, we have been able to explain the observed

phenomena with the categorization of the sibilants in /s/ morfema and /s/ no morfema. The

(11)

reasons to pronounce a certain phoneme in a certain way, always have to do with the communicative relevance. Besides that, the principle of economy also has a lot of influence in the realization of a language. On the one hand, this is what stimulates the elimination of a sibilant, while the communicative relevance, on the other hand, reduces the use of the total loss of /s/ or /θ/.

During the process of writing this paper, it was clear that there are still many things that need to be investigated, when it comes to the subject of the weakening of the sibilants and the effect it has on the preceding vowels. In a future paper, the formants, for example, could be studied better. Also, the explanations given in this paper could be tested. Besides that, it would be interesting to investigate the perception of native and non-native speakers profoundly. Another possibility would be to dedicate more time to the Optimality Theory.

Until now, a lot has been written about vowels and the use of sibilants in the Andalusian

variant. For this reason, this paper will not be the last, since a great quantity of linguists will

remained interested in these fascinating phenomena.

Referenties

GERELATEERDE DOCUMENTEN

Again, in the case of adjusting the speed of the filling machine to the speed of the box packaging machine, this means that a lack of tins causes downtime of the box packaging machine

Objective The objective of the project was to accompany and support 250 victims of crime during meetings with the perpetrators in the fifteen-month pilot period, spread over

Given that my profile in Groningen is the linguistics profile, in France I followed courses from the Sciences du langage program at the Université de Lille, campus pont de

This book on regulatory compliance in the logging sector in Ghana, has attempted to understand how and why the key logging actors in the Ghana’s timber industry respond to

Privacy risks can consist of being harassed by drones (nuisance and annoyance), but also the large- scale (legal or illegal) collection of personal data, inadequate transparency

7 a: For both the SC- and KS-informed hybrid ground-truth data, the number of hybrid single-unit spike trains that are recovered by the different spike sorting algorithms is shown..

Table 3 Comparisons of C-SVM and the proposed coordinate descent algorithm with linear kernel in average test accuracy (Acc.), number of support vectors (SV.) and training

Lasse Lindekilde, Stefan Malthaner, and Francis O’Connor, “Embedded and Peripheral: Rela- tional Patterns of Lone Actor Radicalization” (Forthcoming); Stefan Malthaner et al.,