• No results found

Neural mirroring and social interaction: motor system involvement during action observation relates to early peer cooperation

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Share "Neural mirroring and social interaction: motor system involvement during action observation relates to early peer cooperation"

Copied!
9
0
0

Bezig met laden.... (Bekijk nu de volledige tekst)

Hele tekst

(1)

ContentslistsavailableatScienceDirect

Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience

jou rn a l h om ep ag e : h t t p : / / w w w . e l s e v i e r . c o m / l o c a t e / d c n

Neural mirroring and social interaction: Motor system involvement during action observation relates to early peer cooperation

H.M. Endedijk

a,b,∗

, M. Meyer

a

, H. Bekkering

a

, A.H.N. Cillessen

b

, S. Hunnius

a

aDondersInstituteforBrain,CognitionandBehaviour,RadboudUniversity,Nijmegen,TheNetherlands

bBehaviouralScienceInstitute,RadboudUniversity,Nijmegen,TheNetherlands

a r t i c l e i n f o

Articlehistory:

Received23June2016 Receivedinrevisedform 24November2016 Accepted3January2017 Availableonline7January2017

Keywords:

Neuralmirroring Interpersonalcoordination Cooperation

Socialinteraction Peers

Earlychildhood

a b s t r a c t

Whetherwehandoverobjectstosomeone,playateamsport,ormakemusictogether,socialinteraction ofteninvolvesinterpersonalactioncoordination,bothduringinstancesofcooperationandentrainment.

Neuralmirroringisthoughttoplayacrucialroleinprocessingother’sactionsandisthereforeconsid- eredimportantforsocialinteraction.Still,todate,itisunknownwhetherinterindividualdifferencesin neuralmirroringplayaroleininterpersonalcoordinationduringdifferentinstancesofsocialinteraction.

Arelationbetweenneuralmirroringandinterpersonalcoordinationhasparticularlyrelevantimplica- tionsforearlychildhood,sincesuccessfulearlyinteractionwithpeersispredictiveofamorefavorable socialdevelopment.Weexaminedtherelationbetweenneuralmirroringandchildren’sinterpersonal coordinationduringpeerinteractionusingEEGandlongitudinalbehavioraldata.Resultsshowedthat 4-year-oldchildrenwithhigherlevelsofmotorsysteminvolvementduringactionobservation(asindi- catedbylowerbeta-power)weremoresuccessfulinearlypeercooperation.Thisisthefirstevidencefor arelationbetweenmotorsysteminvolvementduringactionobservationandinterpersonalcoordination duringotherinstancesofsocialinteraction.Thefindingssuggestthatinterindividualdifferencesinneural mirroringarerelatedtointerpersonalcoordinationandthussuccessfulsocialinteraction.

©2017TheAuthors.PublishedbyElsevierLtd.ThisisanopenaccessarticleundertheCCBY-NC-ND license(http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/).

1. Introduction

Ourdailylifecontainsamultitudeofsocialinteractionsinwhich we coordinateouractions withothers. Theinvolvement ofthe mirrorsystemin action perception, monitoring,and prediction (e.g.,Bekkeringetal.,2009;Kilneretal.,2007;Southgateetal., 2009;Stapeletal.,2010)isthoughttohelpusprepareandexecute ourownactionsincoordinationwithothers(Kourtisetal.,2013;

Sebanzetal.,2006).Convergingneuroimagingevidencehasshown thatourmotorsystembecomesactivatedbothwhenperforming anaction,andwhenobservinganaction(MarshallandMeltzoff, 2011;RizzolattiandCraighero,2004;RizzolattiandFogassi,2014).

Thisneuraloverlapbetweenactionproductionandperceptionhas beencalledneuralmirroring(e.g.,HariandKujala,2009).Ithasbeen suggestedthatneuralmirroringprovidestheneurocognitivebasis forprocessing others’ actionsand thereforeplaysa crucial role

∗ Correspondingauthor.Presentaddress:UtrechtUniversity,Heidelberglaan1, 3584CSUtrecht,TheNetherlands.

E-mailaddresses:h.m.endedijk@uu.nl(H.M.Endedijk),m.meyer@donders.ru.nl (M.Meyer),h.bekkering@donders.ru.nl(H.Bekkering),a.cillessen@psych.ru.nl (A.H.N.Cillessen),S.Hunnius@donders.ru.nl(S.Hunnius).

insuccessfulinterpersonalcoordinationduringsocialinteraction (Bekkeringetal.,2009;HariandKujala,2009).

Previous findings supportthis hypothesis ofa close relation between neural mirroring and interpersonal coordination. For instance, adults who showed more motor system involvement whenobservinga partner’smovementsinafingertappingtask alsocoordinatedtheirmovementsbetterwiththepartner(Naeem et al., 2012).While imitative actionsoccur during social inter- action, especially complementaryactions arerelevant in which individualsperformdifferentactions(Bekkeringetal.,2009),for examplewhenpassingandcatchingaball.Complementaryactions werealsorelatedtomotorinvolvementoftheneuralmotorareas duringactionobservation(Ménoretetal.,2014).Comparablefind- ings are present for children, as youngchildren who mirrored anadultactionpartnermorethananotheradultinaturn-taking gamemadefewererrorsininterpersonalcoordinationduringthat game(Meyeretal.,2011).Similarly,recently,Filippietal.(2016) foundthatelevatedlevelsofmirroringin7-month-oldinfantspre- dictedtheirimitationofothers’toychoices.Thesefindingssupport a linkbetweenneuralmirroringandinterpersonalcoordination withinthesamelaboratorytask. However,thedegreetowhich interindividualdifferencesinneuralmirroringsupportthesuccess invariousinstancesofsocialinteractionisunknown.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2017.01.001

1878-9293/©2017TheAuthors.PublishedbyElsevierLtd.ThisisanopenaccessarticleundertheCCBY-NC-NDlicense(http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.

0/).

(2)

34 H.M.Endedijketal./DevelopmentalCognitiveNeuroscience24(2017)33–41

Whiletheroleofinterindividualdifferencesinneuralmirroring forinterpersonalcoordinationisunclear,studiesofsocialcogni- tion(e.g.,empathy,perspectivetaking)highlightaroleofmirroring forsocial skillsthat arenot task-specific.In adults,neuralmir- roringisrelatedtohigherlevelsofperspectivetaking(Woodruff etal.,2011),empathy(Gazzolaetal.,2006;Hookeretal.,2010;

KaplanandIacoboni,2006),andsocialcompetenceasassessedwith questionnaires(Pfeiferetal.,2008).Inthisstudy,weinvestigated whetherinterindividualdifferencesinneuralmirroringalsomight playaroleininterpersonalcoordinationduringsocialinteractions outsidethespecifictask.

Insocialinteraction,two typesofinterpersonalcoordination occuroften:cooperationandentrainment.Whileincooperation, coordinationisplannedandtypicallyinvolvesagoal-directedtask, in entrainment, coordination emerges spontaneously without a jointgoal(Knoblichetal.,2011).Forinstance,soccerplayerscoop- eratebykeepingtrackofeachotherandadjustingtheirpositions accordingly to obtainthe ball and shoot it at the goal.During applause,ontheotherhand,peopleentrainbycoordinatingtheir clappingbehaviorspontaneously.In cooperation,itisimportant tomonitorothers’actionswithrespecttotheachievementofthe commongoal.Inentrainmentthefocusratherisonthemonitor- ingoftheothers’movements.Importantly,boththeobservationof movementsandgoal-directedactionswerefoundtoactivatethe humanmirrorsystem(RizzolattiandCraighero,2004;Rizzolatti andFogassi,2014).Therefore,weexpectedthathigherlevelsof mirroringwouldberelatedtobothhigherlevelsofcooperation andentrainmentsituationsoutsidethespecificmirroringtask.

Activation of the mirror system during action observation alreadyhasbeendemonstratedininfancy(MarshallandMeltzoff, 2011). Investigating therelationbetween neuralmirroring and interpersonalcoordinationisespeciallyimportantinearlychild- hood,since proficiency in social interactionat this age,mainly withpeers,predictssocialcompetencelaterinlife(e.g.,Hayetal., 2009;Rubinetal.,2006).Childrenalreadydemonstrateactioncoor- dinationwithpeersintoddlerhood (e.g.,Ashley and Tomasello, 1998;Brownell,2011;Endedijketal.,2015a;Hunniusetal.,2009).

Duringthepreschoolyears,children’sinterpersonalcoordination continuestodevelop,astheybegintorespondmorequicklytothe behaviorofothersandbecomemorestableincoordination,both incooperation(AshleyandTomasello,1998;Endedijketal.,2015a;

Fletcheret al., 2012)and in entrainmenttasks (Endedijk et al., 2015b).Throughoutearlychildhood,childrengainampleexperi- encewithinterpersonalcoordination.Childrenwhofacedifficulties withsocialinteractionsearlyinlifemoreoftenexperiencerejec- tionbypeerslateron(Friedlmeier,2009;NICHDEarlyChildCare ResearchNetwork,2008)withsubsequentnegativeconsequences fortheirsocialfunctioninginadolescenceandadulthood(Bagwell etal.,1998).Clarifyingtheprocessesinvolvedinearlyinterpersonal coordinationwithpeersisveryimportantforunderstandingsocial development.

Thecurrent study examinedtherelation betweeninterindi- vidualdifferencesinneuralmirroringandyoungchildren’ssocial interactionskills.Children’sneuralmirroringwasassessedbymea- suringoscillatorybrainactivity(bymeansofEEG)duringaction observation.Inparticular,themu-andbeta-frequencybandsover motor areas have been associated with motor system involve- mentduringactionobservation(cf.Meyeretal.,2011;Pfurtscheller andLopesdaSilva,1999;Pineda,2008;SabyandMarshall,2012;

Vanderwertetal.,2013).Toinvestigatetherelationbetweenneural mirroringandinterpersonalcoordinationwithpeers,motorsystem involvementduringactionobservationwasassessedin4-year-old children.Aspartofalongitudinalstudytheirinterpersonalcoor- dinationhadbeenassessedearlierat28,36,and44months,ina cooperationtaskandinanentrainmenttaskwithdifferentpeers.

Basedonpreviousresearchsuggestingthefunctionalinvolvement

ofneuralmirroringduringinterpersonalcoordination(Meyeretal., 2011;Naeemetal.,2012),wehypothesizedthatinterindividualdif- ferencesinchildren’sneuralmirroringofothers’actionswouldbe associatedwithbothformsofinterpersonalcoordination(cooper- ationandentrainment).

2. Method 2.1. Participants

Thesampleconsistedof29children(10boys)whoparticipated inanEEGexperimentat52monthsofage(M=52.48,SD=1.94).

Interpersonalcoordinationwithpeershadbeenassessedinplay sessionsat28months(M=27.96,SD=0.33),36months(M=35.98, SD=0.34),and44months(M=43.83,SD=0.34).Theparticipants werepartofalargersampleof181childrenwhosesocialdevelop- mentwasstudiedlongitudinallyfromtoddlerhoodtoearlyschool age.Childrenwere selectedfromthelargersample iftheyhad participatedinthreeplaysessions(i.e.hadnotmissedasession) andwerewillingtoparticipateinEEGresearch.Theplaysessions tookplaceinthelabwithanunfamiliarsame-genderpeer(also ofthelongitudinalstudysample),eachplaysessionwithadiffer- entpeer.AllchildrenwereDutchandfrommixedsocio-economic backgrounds.Allwerehealthyandhadnoindicationsofatypical development.Parentswereinformedofthestudyandgavewrit- tenconsent.Aftereachtestingsession,childrenreceivedabookor asmallamountofmoney“fortheirpiggybank”asathankyoufor participation.

2.2. Procedure

TheEEGsessiontookapproximately60minincludingfamiliar- izationwiththeexperimenters,preparing theEEGcap,andthe measurementitself(seeSection2.3).Duringtesting,childrenwere videotapedfromtwovisualangles(withonecameradirectedat thechild’supperbody andtheotheroneat thechild’slegs)in ordertoremovetrialsinwhichthechildwasmovingordidnot payattention.

Previously,childrenhadparticipatedinthreeplaysessionsto assesstheirinterpersonalcoordination(seeSections2.4and2.5).

Theplaysessionsstartedwith10–30minoffreeplayduringwhich childrengotfamiliarizedwitheachotherandtheexperimenters.

The introductory phase was followed by the cooperation task, whichtookabout5min.Theentrainmenttaskfollowedwithamax- imumdurationof5min.Parentswereinstructedtominimizetheir interactionswiththeirchildandifthechildwasclingingtothem, respondinwaystostimulateinvolvementinthesessionwithout helpingwiththetasks.Eachsessionlastedabout45minandwas videotapedfromtwovisualanglesusingtwovideocameras.

2.3. Actionobservationtask

Toassesschildren’sindividuallevelsofneuralmirroring,EEG was measuredwhile they watched videos of actions. The task hadtwo conditions:actionobservationand abstractmovement observation.Intheactionobservationcondition(Fig.1,toprow), childrenobservedavideoofanadultperformingdifferentactions onobjects(e.g.,stackingcupsormovingatoycarintoagarage).

In theabstract movement condition(Fig.1,bottom row), chil- drenobservedabstractshapesmovingacrossthescreen,similar toascreensaver.Thisabstractmovementconditionwasincluded tocontrolfornon-humanmovementperception.Thereweresix action videos and six abstract movement videos, each lasting approximately7s. During both action observationand abstract movementobservationcondition,eachvideowasrepeatedthree timesandprecededbya1000msfixationcrossthatfunctioned

(3)

Fig.1.Exampleoftheactionobservation(toprow)andtheabstractmovementobservation(bottomrow)stimuliprecededbythebaseline(fixationcross).

asbaseline(seeFig.1).Theactionobservationconditionwasrun twicewithtwodifferenttaskinstructions(toimitatetheactionor tonamethecoloroftheobjectaftertheobservationofthevideos;

blockedandcounterbalancedbetweenchildren)aspartofadif- ferentstudy.Thus,eachactionobservationvideowasshownsix timesintotalandeachabstractmovementvideothreetimes.After twoactionobservationvideos,oneabstractmovementobserva- tionvideowasshown.Toassesschildren’sneuralactivityduring actionexecution,EEGalsowasrecordedwhilechildrenimitated theactionsafterhavingobservedthem.EEGdataduringtheverbal responsewerenotusedintheanalysis.

Behavioralresponseswerecodedforboththeimitationinstruc- tionandcolornaming.Childrenwereproficientinthecolornaming task,withonechildnaming3outof6,twochildrennaming5out of6,andtheremainingchildrennamingallcolorscorrectly.Forthe imitationtask,theactionsweredividedinthreeparts,e.g.driving thecarto,into,andoutofthegarage.Childrenreceived1pointfor eachpartoftheactiontheyimitated,resultinginamaximumscore of3foreachimitationtrial.Childrenwereatceilinglevelwithan averageimitationperformanceof2.59(range1–3).

EEGrecordingswereconductedusingchild-sizedEEGcapswith 32electrodesitesonthescalp.TheAg/AgClactiveelectrodeswere placedinanactiCap,arrangedaccordingtothe10–20system,and referencedtoelectrodeFCzoverthecentralmidline.Thesignalwas amplifiedusinga32-channelBrainAmpDCEEGamplifier,band- passfiltered(0.1–125Hz),anddigitizedat500Hz.Westrivedto keepallimpedancesbelow60k.

Analogoustopreviousstudies(seeMarshallandMeltzoff,2011;

for a review), we analyzed motor systemactivity by meansof mu-andbeta-oscillatorypoweroversensorimotor areas.Motor system involvement was analyzed during action observation, abstractmovementobservation,andactionexecution.Dataanaly- siswasperformedusingFieldTrip,anopensourceMatlabtoolbox (Oostenveldetal.,2011).Alldatawasdividedinto1-ssegments andre-referencedtotheaverageofallelectrodes.Segmentsdur- ingwhichchildrenmovedordidnotlookatthestimulusdisplay wereremoved.Wevisuallyinspectedtheremainingsegmentsto excludeEEGartifacts(suchasnoisychannelsoreyeblinks).One

child was removedfrom theanalyses due tothe lack of base- linetrialsduringtheabstractmovementobservationcondition.On average,per child120segmentsremainedfortheactionobser- vation(range 33–246),38 segmentsfortheabstractmovement observation(range4–81),12segmentsforthebaselinepreceding theactionobservationstimuli(range3–24),and5segmentsforthe baselineprecedingtheabstractmovementstimuli(range1–12).A DFTfilterwasusedtoremovelinenoisefromthedata,andforeach segmentwetookoutpotentialoffsetdifferencesbysubtractingthe meansignaloftheentiretrialfromthesignalateachtimepoint.

WethencalculatedspectralpowerestimatesusingtheFastFourier transformonthe1-ssegmentsincombinationwithaHanningtaper asappliedonthesegmentswithoutoverlap.Finally,wecalculated anaveragepowerforeachconditionforeachchild,touseinthe analysis.

Basedonpreviousresearch(seePfurtschellerandLopesdaSilva, 1999),wefocusedouranalysesonelectrodesovermotorcortices (C3,C4).Tocontrolforinterindividualdifferencesinabsolutepower duetodifferencesinscalpthicknessandelectrodeimpedance,the ratioofpowerduringtheconditionrelativetobaseline(fixation cross)wascomputedforeachcondition.Sincetheseratioswere notnormallydistributed,alogtransformationwasapplied.These scoreswereusedtoindicatechildren’smotorsysteminvolvement ineachcondition(actionobservation,abstractmovementobser- vation)andduringactionexecution.Asmallerlogratioindicated moresuppressioninaconditioncomparedtobaseline.Basedonthe actionexecutionratio,thesample-specificmu-andbeta-frequency rangewasidentified(seeSection3.1).Normalizedpowervalues werepooledoverthecentralelectrodes(C3,C4)perconditionin theidentifiedmu-andbeta-frequencybandsforfurtheranalysis.

2.4. Cooperationtask

Thecooperationtaskwasa peerversion of Warnekenetal.

(2006)double-tubetask.Thesetupconsistedoftwo1-m-longtubes mountedinparallelonaboxwitha45incline(seeFig.2A).The childrenweregivenaPlaymobilfigureinaswimsuitandasmall swimmingpool.Theywereinstructedthatthefigurewantedtogo

(4)

36 H.M.Endedijketal./DevelopmentalCognitiveNeuroscience24(2017)33–41

Fig.2.Childrenperformingthecooperationtask(A)andentrainmenttask(B).

throughtheslidingtubetotheswimmingpool.Becausethetubes weretoolongforonechildtosimultaneouslyholdtheswimming poolandinsertthefigureintothetube,thetwochildrenhadto cooperatetoperformthetasksuccessfully.Adetaileddescription ofthetaskcanbefoundinEndedijketal.,(2015a).

Eachchild’sbehaviorwascodedoff-linefromthevideorecord- ings.Foreachtrial(definedasaslideofthefigurethroughthetube), itwascodedwhethercooperationwassuccessfulornot.Coopera- tiontrialswerecodedassuccessfulifboththechildwhoinserted thefigureintothetubeandthechildwhoheldtheswimmingpool chosethesametube.Cooperationtrialswerecodedasunsuccessful ifchildrenchosedifferenttubesorifonechildperformedthetask alone,resultinginthefigurefallingonthefloor.Tocontrolforthe totalnumberoftrials,thedataweretransformedintoaproportion ofsuccessonthetaskforeachdyad.Forthelongitudinalstudy,the recordingsof20%ofthedyadsateachtimepointwerecodedby twoobservers.Cohen’skappawas0.94onaverage(SD=0.11).

2.5. Entrainmenttask

Fortheentrainmenttasktwo10-inchdrumsofaHaymanchil- dren’sdrumsetandtwoplasticstickswereused(seeFig.2B).The drumswereplacedonastandthatcouldbeadjustedtotheheight ofeachchildsothattheycouldcomfortablydruminstandingposi- tion.Thedrumswereconnectedviapiezocontactmicrophones placedonthedrumheadstocollectMIDIdataviaanAlesisD4drum module.PerformanceswererecordedwithLogicExpress.Children wereinstructedseparatelytostartdrumminganddidnotreceive anyinstructionsaboutdrummingtogetherorcoordinatingtheir drummingwiththeirdyadpartner.

Cross-correlationscommonlyareusedin interpersonalcoor- dination studies to investigate entrainment (Repp, 2005). We calculated maximum cross-correlations that indicated how a child’shitsbestrelatedtotheirpartner’shitsrhythmicallyacross time.Forthispurpose,thetimebetweenthehitsproducedbyeach childweremeasured.Timeseriesoftheseinter-tap-intervalsofthe twochildrenwereshiftedalongsideeachothertofindthehighest correlationbetweenthetwotimeseries.Thereby,themaximum cross-correlationmeasuredescribesthecoordinationofchildren’s rhythmicbehaviors.

2.6. Analyses

To examine whether interpersonal coordination predicted motor system involvement during action observation(a proxy

forneuralmirroring),twohierarchicalregressionswererun,one predictingnormalizedmu-powerandonepredictingnormalized beta-bandpowerduringactionobservation.Tocontrolformotor systeminvolvementduetonon-humanmovement,thenormal- izedpowerduringobservationofabstractmovementwasentered inStep1ofeachregression.InStep2ofeachregression,themea- suresofinterpersonalcoordinationwereentered:theproportion ofcoordinatedtrialsduringcooperation,andthemaximumcross- correlationduringentrainment.Thescoresforthesetwovariables werestandardizedforeachplaysessionandaveragedacrossthe threesessions, resultinginmeasuresof interpersonalcoordina- tionaggregatedoversessionsandinteractionpartners.Thesethree averagedz-scoreswereenteredinStep2oftheregressionanalysis.

3. Results

3.1. Neuralmirroring

Basedontheobservedsuppressionofpowerduringactionexe- cution(seeFig.3,top),thefrequencybandswereidentifiedonthe basisofthegrandaverageasfollows:mufrom7to12Hzandbeta from16to20Hz.Thetopographicdistributionofthesefrequency bandssupportsthea-prioriselectionofelectrodesovermotorcor- tices(seeFig.3,bottom).

Theanalysisofthesespecifiedfrequencybandsyieldedposi- tivenormalizedpowervaluesforbothmuandbetaduringaction observation,M=0.23,SD=0.28,andM=0.20,SD=0.31,andabstract movementobservation,M=0.25,SD=0.44,andM=0.22,SD=0.44, indicatingrelativelymorepowerduringexperimentalconditions thanatbaseline.Similartoactionexecution,thetopographicdis- tributionofnormalizedpowerinmu-andbeta-frequencybands showedarelativelyconfinedpatternofactivationoverlayingmotor cortices(especially at electrode sites C3 and C4) during action observation(Fig.4,toprow).Thetopographicdistributionduring abstractmovementobservationappearedlessconfinedbutmore widespreadalongthemidline(Fig.4,bottomrow).

3.2. Relationbetweenneuralmirroringandinterpersonal coordination

Table1summarizestheresultsofthehierarchicalregressions.

Instep1,motorsysteminvolvementduringabstractmovement observationwasrelatedtoactionobservationvaluesforthemu- frequencyband,butnotforthebeta-frequencyband.Addingthe measuresofcooperationandentrainmentinStep2resultedina

(5)

Fig.3.Top:Normalizedpowerrepresentedasafunctionoffrequency(Hz)withtheleftblue-shadedareaindicatingtheselectedmu-frequencyband(7–12Hz),andtheright yellow-shadedareaindicatingtheselectedbeta-frequencyband(16–20Hz).Negativenormalizedpowervaluesrepresentsuppressionduringactionexecutionwithrespect tobaseline.Bottom:Thetopographicdistributionofthenormalizedpowerinmu-andbeta-frequencybandsduringactionexecution,withwarmcolorsrepresentinghigher normalizedpower(enhancement)andcoolercolorsrepresentinglowerpower(suppression).

significantlybettermodelforthebeta-frequencyband,Fchange(2, 21)=5.14,p=0.02,R2=0.39,butnotforthemu-frequencyband, Fchange(2,21)=0.31,p=0.74,R2=0.02.Forbeta,whilecontrolling fornon-humanmovement,powerreductionwasstronglyrelated to children’s performance on the cooperation task (␤=−0.52, p=0.01).Childrenwhoweremoresuccessfulincooperationwith peersalsoshowedmoreinvolvementofthemotorsystemdur- ingactionobservation.Therewasnosignificantrelationbetween entrainmentandbeta-bandpower.

4. Discussion

Inthisstudy,weexaminedtherelationbetweeninterindivid- ualdifferences inneural mirroringin youngchildren and their

socialinteractionwithpeersinacooperationandanentrainment task.Wefoundthatyoungchildrenwhoshowedmoremotorsys- teminvolvementwhenobservingothers’actions(asindicatedby a relative reduction in betapower), showedbettercooperation skillswithpeers.Theexplainedvariancewashigh,suggestingthat interindividualdifferencesinmirroringarerelevantforinterper- sonalcoordinationwithpeersinearlychildhood.

Therelationbetweenmotorsysteminvolvementduringaction observationandchildren’speercoordinationisconsistentwithpre- viousfindingsthatmirroringisrelatedtomorereliableimitation (Bernieretal.,2007;Filippietal.,2016;Warreynetal.,2013),bet- terinterpersonalcoordinationoffingermovements(Naeemetal., 2012),andfewerturn-takingerrors(Meyeretal.,2011).However, thesepreviousstudiesmeasuredneuralmirroringandbehavioral

(6)

38 H.M.Endedijketal./DevelopmentalCognitiveNeuroscience24(2017)33–41

Fig.4.Topographicdistributionofnormalizedpowerinmu-(left)andbeta-frequencybands(right)duringactionobservation(toprow)andabstractmovementobservation (bottomrow).Warmcolorsrepresenthighernormalizedpower(enhancement)fortheconditionascomparedtothebaselineandcoolercolorsrepresentlowerpower (suppression)fortheconditionascomparedtothebaseline.

Table1

StepwiseRegressionAnalysisWithNormalizedMuandBetaPowerValuesDur- ingActionObservationasDependentVariables,andNormalized MuandBeta PowerValuesDuringAbstractMovementObservation,CooperationPerformance andEntrainmentPerformanceasIndependentVariables.

Mu(7–12Hz) Beta(16–20Hz)

R2 p R2 p

Step1

Abstractmovement observation

0.46 0.02* 0.29 0.15

Total 0.22 0.02* 0.09 0.15

Step2

Abstractmovement observation

0.48 0.03* 0.17 0.38

Proportioncoordinated trials

0.14 0.47 −0.52 0.01*

Maximum cross-correlation

0.05 0.79 0.19 0.31

Total 0.24 0.12 0.39 0.02*

Note.

R2indicatestheamountofexplainedvariancebythepredictors,andarestan- dardizedregressioncoefficients.

* p(probability)<0.05.

performanceduringthesameinstanceofsocialinteraction(i.e.one laboratorytask)andthusdidnotaddresswhetherthisrelationis task-specificorreflectsinterindividualdifferencesthatgeneralize tosocialinteractionsoutsidethespecifictask.

Tocapturevariousformsofpeerinteraction,weinvestigated twotypesof interpersonalcoordination:goal-directedcoopera- tion,andentrainmentwithoutanovertcommongoal.Wefound thatneuralmirroringwasrelatedtochildren’sperformanceinthe cooperationtaskbutnotintheentrainmenttask.Thisisconsistent

withpreviousresearchthathighlightedtheimportanceofgoals foraction mirroring(Koskiet al.,2002).Bekkeringetal.(2009) arguedthatmonitoringandpredictinganotherperson’sgoalrather thantheirmovementsisimportantforinterpersonalcoordination becauseitoftenrequiresco-actorstoperformdifferentmovements toachieveacommongoal.Inthecurrentcooperationtaskalso,chil- drenhadtoassumecomplementaryrolesthatrequiredmonitoring ofeachother’sactions.

Theobservedlinkbetweenneuralmirroringandcooperation wasevidentforbetapower(16–20Hz).Formupower(7–12Hz), however,no indicationfor sucha relationwasfound.Previous researchhasshownthatbothmuandbetapoweraremodulated duringactionobservation,althoughtheyhavebeenassociatedwith slightlydifferentfunctions(Caetanoetal.,2007;Meyeretal.,2011;

QuandtandMarshall,2014;Schuchetal.,2010).Mu-bandactiv- ityissuggestedtobeinvolvedintranslatingsensoryinputinto motorprocesses(Naeemetal.,2012;Pineda,2005;Vanderwert etal.,2013),whichmatcheswithitsmoreposteriorlocalization over sensorimotor regions of the brain (Ritter et al., 2009). In contrast,thelocationofbetaoscillatoryactivityistypicallymore anterioranditisassociatedwithactivityinthemotorandpre- motorcortex(Ritteretal.,2009).Ithasbeensuggestedthatboth mu-andbeta-bandoscillationsareinvolvedinactionpredictions (Southgateetal.,2009;Stapeletal.,2010),whilebeta-bandactivity isassociatedspecificallywithpredictionupdatinganderrormon- itoring(Arnaletal.,2011;Koelewijnetal.,2008).Exactlythese processes–monitoringothers’actionsandintegrating informa- tioninordertoupdateactionpredictions–areimportantduring cooperation(Kourtisetal.,2013;Sebanz etal.,2006).Updating actionpredictionsandmonitoringwereessentialforthecurrent peercooperationtask.Predictingwhich tubethepartnerwould

(7)

choose,monitoringthepartner’sbehaviortocheckwhetherthe predictionwascorrect,andupdatingone’spredictionswerenec- essarytosucceedonthetask.Thismightalsoexplainwhyarelation betweencooperationperformanceandoscillatorymodulationwas observedinthebeta-band,ascontinuouspredictionandupdating ofpredictionsareinherenttoactionobservation(Falck-Ytteretal., 2006;GredebäckandMelinder,2010;Kilneretal.,2007).Still,the exactfunctionaldifferencesbetweenmu-andbeta-bandoscilla- tionsandtheirrespectiverolesduringactionobservationhaveto bedeterminedinfutureresearch.

For bothmu- andbeta-power,we observedthat powerval- ueswerehigherduringactionobservationthanbaselineindicating enhancementratherthan suppression.Atfirstsight,this issur- prisingsincepreviousresearchsuggeststhatsuppressionofmu- andbeta-powerindicateincreasedinvolvementofthemotorsys- tem(MarshallandMeltzoff,2011;RizzolattiandFogassi,2014).

However,severalrecentstudiesalsofoundthatpowerinthese frequency bands is not significantly suppressed during action observation(Cannonetal.,2016;Nyström,2008;PerryandBentin, 2010)orevenenhanced(Marshalletal.,2013).Althoughinour studytheneuralresponseshowedanunexpecteddirectionality withregardtobaseline,weareconfidentthatthemu-andbeta- band activityreflects a response of the motor systemfor two reasons.First,theeffectwasrelativelyconfinedtoelectrodesites overlayingovermotorareas(C3,C4),whichsuggestsamodulation ofthemotorsystem.Second,childrenwereaskedtositmotion- lessandwatchthreerepetitionsofeachactionvideoonthescreen beforetheywereallowedtorespond.Thus,childrenlikelytriedto activelyinhibitanovertmotorresponseduringtheactionobser- vation,and this wasassociatedwithanincrease inbeta-power (Gilbertsonetal.,2005).Notably,thismotorinhibitiondidnotaffect thedirectionoftherelationwefound.Thatis,lessbetapowerwith respecttobaseline(indicatingrelativelymoremotoractivity)was relatedtomoresuccessduringpeercooperation.Althoughchil- drenthuslikelysuppressedtheirmotoractivityingeneraltositas motionlessaspossible,interindividualdifferenceswithrespectto howsensitivetheirmotorsystemwastoactionobservationwere stillrelatedtotheircooperationbehavior.Totestwhetherinhibi- tionindeedplaysarole,itisimportanttostructurallyinvestigate thisinfuturestudiesbyincludingaconditionwithoutanyinstruc- tioninwhichchildrenobserveexactlythesameactions.

Ourresultssuggest thatinterindividual differencesinneural mirroringare related to successfulcooperation. Yet,the causal directionunderlyingthisrelationremainsanopenquestion.Bet- terinterpersonalcoordinationlikelyistheresultofhighergeneral levelsofneuralmirroring.Previousresearchhasshownthatneural mirroringsupportsprediction(e.g.,Southgateetal.,2009;Stapel etal.,2010)andmonitoringofothers’actions(Becchioetal.,2012;

Bekkeringetal.,2009)aswecanuseourownactionsystemto predicttheactionsofapartner(Kilneretal.,2007).Enhancedpre- dictionand monitoring,in turn,might helpus preparefor and executeourownactionsaccordingly(Kourtisetal.,2013;Sebanz etal.,2006).Basedonthisreasoning,individualswithhigherlevels ofneuralmirroringmightbebetteratcoordinatingtheiractions withothers. However, neuralmirroring and cooperationmight alsobothbetheresultofathirdfactor,suchassocialmotivation.

Childrendifferintheirmotivationtobeinvolvedinsocialinterac- tions(BrownellandHazen,1999),whichcouldimpactboththeir level ofmirroringand theircooperationsuccess. Neuroimaging studiesinadultshaveshownaroleofsocialmotivationformir- roringastheyfoundenhancedmirroringwhenparticipantswere sociallyprimed(HogeveenandObhi,2012;Obermanetal.,2007), andenhancedmirroringforin-groupmembersthanforout-group members(Gutselland Inzlicht,2010; Molenberghset al.,2013;

Rauchbaueretal.,2015).Studieswithadultsalsosupporttheroleof socialmotivationininterpersonalcoordination:Adultswithapro-

socialorientationcoordinatedtheiractionsbetterthanadultswith apro-selforientation(Lumdsenetal.,2012).Whetherchildren’s neuralmirroringisreallyatthebaseoftheirinterpersonalcoor- dinationorwhetherbotharetheresultoftheirsocialmotivation hastobeaddressedinfutureresearch.Hereby,itwouldespecially beinformativetodevelop stimulusvideosactedbychildrenfor children,asthesevideoswouldbemoresociallyrelevantforthem.

Thequestionarisestowhatextentinterindividualdifferences inneuralmirroringplayaroleinchildren’ssocialdevelopment.

Friedlmeier (2009) suggested that adapting behavior might be an indicatorofsocial competence.And Cirelliet al. (2014)and KirschnerandTomasello(2010)foundmorehelpingbehaviorin children aftertheyexperiencedsmooth interpersonalcoordina- tion.Thisincreasedprosocialitycouldbeanindicatoroflikeability, therebysuggestingthathigherlevelsofmirroringresultinbetter peerrelationsviasuccessfulinterpersonalcoordination.However, a relation betweeninterpersonalcoordination and peerprefer- encewasnotpresentinarecentlongitudinalstudyweconducted (Endedijketal.,submitted).Ontheotherhand,theincreasedhelp- ingbehaviorasresponsetointerpersonalcoordinationalsocould suggestthatmirroringsupportsestimationoftheneedsofpeers.

Baimeletal.(2015)arguedthatcoordinatinginterpersonallyhelps reasoningaboutothers’mind,therebyfosteringperspectivetaking andempathicconcern.Althoughtheexactsocialconsequencesof peercoordinationareunclear,theselinesofreasoningsuggestthat interindividualdifferencesinneuralmirroringmayhaveseveral implicationsforchildren’ssocialdevelopment.

In summary,ourfindings suggestthat interindividualdiffer- encesinthedegreetowhich childrenmirrorothers’actions(as indexedbyrelativebeta-powerdecrease)arecloselyrelatedtohow welltheycoordinatetheirownactionsduringcooperationwith peers.Toourknowledge,thesefindingsprovidethefirstevidence thatinterindividualdifferencesinmotoractivationduringaction observationarerelatedtointerpersonalcoordinationoutsidethe specificmirroringtaskandthussuccessfulsocialinteraction.

Conflictofinterest None.

References

ActiCap.Munich,Germany:BrainProductsGmbH.

AlesisD4drummodule.Cumberland,RI:AlesisInnovations.

Arnal,L.H.,Wyart,V.,Giraud,A.,2011.Transitionsinneuraloscillationsreflect predictionerrorsgeneratedinaudiovisualspeech.Nat.Neurosci.14,797–803, http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nn.2810.

Ashley,J.,Tomasello,M.,1998.Cooperativeproblem-solvingandteachingin preschoolers.Soc.Dev.7,143–163,http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-9507.

00059.

Bagwell,C.L.,Newcomb,A.F.,Bukowski,W.M.,1998.Preadolescentfriendshipand peerrejectionaspredictorsofadultadjustment.ChildDev.69,140–153, http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/1132076.

Baimel,A.,Severson,R.L.,Baron,A.S.,Birch,S.A.J.,2015.Enhancing”Theoryof Mind”throughbehaviouralsynchrony.Front.Psychol.6,http://dx.doi.org/10.

3389/fpsyg.2015.00870.

Becchio,C.,Cavallo,A.,Begliomini,C.,Sartori,L.,Feltrin,G.,Castiello,U.,2012.

Socialgrasping:frommirroringtomentalizing.Neuroimage61,240–248, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.03.013.

Bekkering,H.,DeBruijn,E.R.A.,Cuijpers,R.H.,Newman-Norlund,R.,vanSchie,H.T., Meulenbroek,R.,2009.Jointaction:neurocognitivemechanismssupporting humaninteraction.Top.Cogn.Sci.1,340–352,http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.

1756-8765.2009.01023.x.

Bernier,R.,Dawson,G.,Webb,S.,Murias,M.,2007.EEGmurhythmandimitation impairmentsinindividualswithAutismSpectrumDisorder.BrainCogn.64, 228–237,http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2007.03.004.

Brownell,C.A.,Hazen,N.,1999.Earlypeerinteraction:aresearchagenda.Early Educ.Dev.10,403–413,http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15566935eed10039.

Brownell,C.A.,2011.Earlydevelopmentsinjointaction.Rev.Philos.Psychol.2, 193,http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s13164-011-0056-1.

Caetano,G.,Jousmãki,V.,Hari,R.,2007.Actor’sandobserver’sprimarymotor corticesstabilizesimilarlyafterseenorhearmotoractions.PNAS22, 9058–9062,http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0702453104.

(8)

40 H.M.Endedijketal./DevelopmentalCognitiveNeuroscience24(2017)33–41

Cannon,E.N.,Simpson,E.A.,Fox,N.A.,Vanderwert,R.E.,Woodward,A.L.,Ferrari, P.F.,2016.Relationsbetweeninfants’emergingreach-graspcompetenceand event-relateddesynchronizationinEEG.Dev.Sci.19,50–62,http://dx.doi.org/

10.1111/desc.12295.

Cirelli,L.K.,Einarson,K.M.,Trainor,L.J.,2014.Interpersonalsynchronyincreases prosocialbehaviorininfants.Dev.Sci.17,1003–1011,http://dx.doi.org/10.

1111/desc.12193.

Falck-Ytter,T.,Gredebäck,G.,vonHofsten,C.,2006.Infantspredictotherpeople’s actiongoals.Nat.Neurosci.9,878–879,http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nn1729.

Endedijk,H.M.,Cillessen,A.H.N.,Bekkering,H.,Hunnius,S.,(submitted).Girls’

peerinteractionintoddlerhoodpredictstheirpeerpreferenceatschoolage:A longitudinalstudyonpeerinteractioninearlychildhood.

Endedijk,H.M.,Cillessen,A.H.N.,Cox,R.F.A.,Bekkering,H.,Hunnius,S.,2015a.The roleofchildcharacteristicsandpeerexperiencesinthedevelopmentofpeer cooperation.SocialDevelopment24,521–540,http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/sode.

12106.

Endedijk,H.M.,Ramenzoni,V.C.O.,Cox,R.F.A.,Cillessen,A.H.N.,Bekkering,H., Hunnius,S.,2015b.Developmentofinterpersonalcoordinationbetweenpeers duringadrummingtask.DevelopmentalPsychology51,714–721,http://dx.

doi.org/10.1037/a0038980.

Filippi,C.A.,Cannon,E.N.,Fox,N.A.,Thorpe,S.G.,Ferrari,P.F.,Woodward,A.L.,2016.

Motorsystemactivationpredictsgoalimitationin7-month-oldinfants.

Psychol.Sci.27,675–684,http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0956797616632231.

Fletcher,G.E.,Warneken,F.,Tomasello,M.,2012.Differencesincognitiveprocesses underlyingthecollaborativeactivitiesofchildrenandchimpanzees.Cogn.Dev.

27,136–153,http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2012.02.003.

Friedlmeier,W.,2009.Reciprocityandrelationshipschemesinearlypeer interactions.Eur.J.Dev.Sci.3,347–367.

Gazzola,V.,Aziz-Zadeh,L.,Keysers,C.,2006.Empathyandthesomatotopic auditorymirrorsysteminhumans.Curr.Biol.16,1824–1829,http://dx.doi.

org/10.1016/j.cub.2006.07.072.

Gilbertson,T.,Doyle,L.,DiLazzaro,V.,Cioni,B.,Brown,P.,2005.Existingmotor stateisfavoredattheexpenseofnewmovementduring13–35hzoscillatory synchornyinthehumancorticospinalsystem.J.Neurosci.25,7771–7779, http://dx.doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1762-05.2005.

Gredebäck,G.,Melinder,A.,2010.Infants’understandingofeverydaysocial interactions:adualprocessaccount.Cognition114,197–206,http://dx.doi.

org/10.1016/j.cognition.2009.09.004.

Gutsell,J.N.,Inzlicht,M.,2010.Empathyconstrained:prejudicepredictsreduced mentalsimulationofactionsduringobservationofoutgroups.J.Exp.Soc.

Psychol.46,841–845,http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2010.03.011.

Hari,R.,Kujala,V.,2009.Brainbasisofhumansocialinteraction:fromconceptsto brainimaging.Physiol.Rev.89,453–479,http://dx.doi.org/10.1152/physrev.

00041.2007.

Hay,D.F.,Caplan,M.,Nash,A.,2009.Thebeginningsofpeerrelations.In:Rubin, K.H.,Bukowski,W.M.,Laursen,B.(Eds.),HandbookofPeerInteractions, Relationships,andGroups.Guilford,NewYork,pp.121–142.

HaymanJSD-010-MR.London:Hayman.

Hogeveen,J.,Obhi,S.S.,2012.Socialinteractionsenhancesmotorresonancefor observedhumanactions.J.Neurosci.32,5984–5989,http://dx.doi.org/10.

1523/JNEUROSCI.5938-11.2012.

Hooker,C.I.,Verosky,S.C.,Germine,L.T.,Knight,R.T.,D’Esposito,M.,2010.Neural activityduringsocialsignalperceptioncorrelateswithself-reportedempathy.

BrainRes.1308,100–113,http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2009.10.006.

Hunnius,S.,Bekkering,H.,Cillessen,A.H.N.,2009.Theassociationbetween intentionunderstandingandpeercooperationintoddlers.Eur.J.Dev.Sci.3, 368–388.

Kaplan,J.T.,Iacoboni,M.,2006.Gettingagriponotherminds:mirrorneurons, intentionunderstanding,andcognitiveempathy.Soc.Neurosci.1,175–183, http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17470910600985605.

Kilner,J.M.,Friston,K.J.,Frith,C.D.,2007.Predictivecoding:anaccountofthe mirrorneuronsystem.Cogn.Process.8,159–166,http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/

s10339-007-0170-2.

Kirschner,S.,Tomasello,M.,2010.Jointmusicmakingpromotesprosocialbehavior in4-year-oldchildren.Evol.Hum.Behav.31,354–364,http://dx.doi.org/10.

1016/j.evolhumbehav.2010.04.004.

Knoblich,G.,Butterfill,S.,Sebanz,N.,2011.Psychologicalresearchonjointaction:

theoryanddata.In:Ross,B.H.(Ed.),PsychologyofLearningandMotivation:

AdvancesinResearchandTheory,vol.54.ElsevierAcademicPressInc,San Diego,pp.59–101.

Koelewijn,T.,vanSchie,H.T.,Bekkering,H.,Oostenveld,R.,Jensen,O.,2008.

Motor-corticalbetaoscillationsaremodulatedbycorrectnessofobserved action.Neuroimage40,767–775,http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.

2007.12.018.

Koski,L.,Wohlschläger,A.,Bekkering,H.,Woods,R.P.,Dubeau,M.,Mazziotta,J.C., etal.,2002.Modulationofmotorandpremotoractivityduringimitationof target-directedactions.Cereb.Cortex12,847–855,http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/

cercor/12.8.847.

Kourtis,D.,Sebanz,N.,Knoblich,G.,2013.Predictiverepresentationofother people’sactionsinjointactionplanning:anEEGstudy.Soc.Neurosci.8,31–42, http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17470919.2012.694823.

LogicExpress(Version8).Cupertino,CA:AppleInc.

Lumdsen,J.,Miles,L.K.,Richardson,D.C.,Smith,C.A.,Macrae,C.N.,2012.Who syncs?Socialmotivesandinterpersonalcoordination.J.Exp.Soc.Psychol.48, 746–751,http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2011.12.007.

Ménoret,M.,Varnet,L.,Fargier,R.,Cheylus,A.,Curie,A.,desPortes,V.,etal.,2014.

Neuralcorrelatesofnon-verbalsocialinteractions:adual-EEGstudy.

Neuropsychologia55,85–97,http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.

2013.10.001.

Marshall,P.J.,Meltzoff,A.N.,2011.Neuralmirroringsystems:exploringtheEEG murhythminhumaninfancy.Dev.Cogn.Neurosci.1,110–123,http://dx.doi.

org/10.1016/j.dcn.2010.09.001.

Marshall,P.J.,Saby,J.N.,Meltzoff,A.N.,2013.Infantbrainresponsestoobject weight:exploringgoal-directedactionsandself-experience.Infancy18, 942–960,http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/infa.12012.

Matlab,version7.7.0.Natick,Minnesota,USA:TheMathWorks,Inc.

Meyer,M.,Hunnius,S.,vanElk,M.,vanEde,F.,Bekkering,H.,2011.Jointaction modulatesmotorsysteminvolvementduringactionobservationin 3-year-olds.Exp.BrainRes.211,581–592,http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00221- 011-2658-3.

Molenberghs,P.,Halász,V.,Mattingly,J.B.,Vanman,E.J.,Cunningtong,R.,2013.

Seeingisbelieving:neuralmechanismsofaction-perceptionarebiasedby teammembership.Hum.BrainMapp.34,2055–2068,http://dx.doi.org/10.

1002/hbm.22044.

NICHDEarlyChildCareResearchNetworkhNetwork,2008.Socialcompetence withpeersinthirdgrade:associationswithearlierpeerexperiencesin childcare.Soc.Dev.17,419–453,http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9507.2007.

00446.x.

Naeem,M.,Prasad,G.,Watson,D.R.,Kelso,J.A.S.,2012.Functionaldissociationof brainrhythmsinsocialcoordination.Clin.Neurophysiol.123,1789–1797, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.clinph.2012.02.065.

Nyström,P.,2008.TheinfantmirrorneuronsystemstudiedwithhighdensityEEG.

Soc.Neurosci.3,334–347,http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17470910701563665.

Oberman,L.M.,Pineda,J.A.,Ramachandran,S.,2007.Thehumanmirrorneuron system:alinkbetweenactionobservationandsocialskills.SCAN2,62–66, http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsl022.

Oostenveld,R.,Fries,P.,Maris,E.,Schoffelen,J.,2011.FieldTrip:opensource softwareforadvancedanalysisofMEG,EEG,andinvasiveelectrophysiological data.Comput.Intell.Neurosci.2011,http://dx.doi.org/10.1155/2011/156869, ArticleID156869.

Perry,A.,Bentin,S.,2010.Doesfocusingonhand-graspingintentionsmodulate electroencephalogrammuandalphasuppressions?Neuroreport21, 1050–1054,http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/wnr.0b013e32833fcb71.

Pfeifer,J.H.,Iacoboni,M.,Mazziotta,J.C.,Dapretto,M.,2008.Mirroringothers’

emotionsrelatestoempathyandinterpersonalcompetenceinchildren.

Neuroimage39,2076–2085,http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.10.

032.

Pfurtscheller,G.,LopesdaSilva,F.H.,1999.Event-relatedeeg/megsynchronization anddesynchronization:basicprinciples.Clin.Neurophysiol.110,1842–1857, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s1388-2457(99)00141-8.

Pineda,J.A.,2005.Thefunctionalsignificanceofmurhythms:translatingseeing andhearningintodoing.BrainRes.Rev.50,57–68,http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.

brainresrev.2005.04.005.

Pineda,J.A.,2008.Sensorimotorcortexasacriticalcomponentofan’extended’

mirrorneuronsystem:doesitsolvethedevelopment,correspondence,and controlproblemsinmirroring?Behav.BrainFunct.4,47,http://dx.doi.org/10.

1186/1744-9081-4-47.

Playmobil,GeobraBrandstätterGmbH&Co.KG.Zirndorf,Germany.

Quandt,L.C.,Marshall,P.J.,2014.Theeffectofactionexperienceonsensorimotor EEGrhythmsduringactionobservation.Neuropsychologia56,401–408, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.02.015.

Rauchbauer,B.,Majdandˇzi ´c,J.,Hummer,A.,Windischberger,C.,Lamm,C.,2015.

Distinctneuralprocessesareengagedinthemodulationofmimicrybysocial group-membershipandemotionalexpressions.Cortex70,49–67,http://dx.

doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2015.03.007.

Repp,B.H.,2005.Sensorimotorsynchronization:areviewofthetappingliterature.

Psychon.Bull.Rev.12,969–992,http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03206433.

Ritter,P.,Moosmann,M.,Villringer,A.,2009.RolandicalphaandbetaEEG rhythms’strengthsareinverselyrelatedtofMRI-BOLDsignalinprimary somatosensoryandmotorcortex.Hum.BrainMapp.30,1168–1187,http://dx.

doi.org/10.1002/hbm.20585.

Rizzolatti,G.,Craighero,L.,2004.Themirror-neuronsystem.Annu.Rev.Neurosci.

27,169–192,http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev.neuro.27.070203.144230.

Rizzolatti,G.,Fogassi,L.,2014.Themirrormechanism:recentfindingsand perspectives.Philos.Trans.R.Soc.B369,21030420,http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/

rstb.2013.0420.

Rubin,K.H.,Bukowski,W.M.,Parker,G.,2006.Peerinteractions,relationships,and groups.In:Eisenberg,N.(Ed.),HandbookofChildPsychology(Vol.3:Social, Emotional,andPersonalityDevelopment).Wiley,Hoboken,NJ,pp.571–645.

Saby,J.N.,Marshall,S.J.,2012.TheutilityofEEGbandpoweranalysisinthestudy ofinfancyandearlychildhood.Dev.Neuropsychol.37,253–273,http://dx.doi.

org/10.1080/87565641.2011.614663.

Schuch,S.,Bayliss,A.P.,Klein,C.,Tipper,S.P.,2010.Attentionmodulatesmotor systemactivationduringactionobservation:evidenceforinhibitoryrebound.

Exp.BrainRes.205,235–249,http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00221-010-2358-4.

Sebanz,N.,Bekkering,H.,Knoblich,G.,2006.Jointaction:bodiesandminds movingtogether.TrendsCogn.Sci.10,70–75,http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.

2005.12.009.

Southgate,V.,Johnson,M.H.,Osborne,T.,Csibra,G.,2009.Predictivemotor activationduringactionobservationinhumaninfants.Biol.Lett.5,769–772, http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rsbl.2009.0474.

Referenties

GERELATEERDE DOCUMENTEN

Uit het onderzoek dat in het voorjaar op de drie praktijkbedrijven gedaan is blijkt, dat over het algemeen de beste geelverkleuring van het hart wordt verkregen in­ dien de

To investigate whether there are differential effects for response hand in pre-response and post-response action monitoring, average peak amplitudes of the ERN and the N2

- Voor waardevolle archeologische vindplaatsen die bedreigd worden door de geplande ruimtelijke ontwikkeling en die niet in situ bewaard kunnen blijven:.. o Wat is

In light of the newly established SDG 6, and the UNESCO UNHIDE research project, this bears the following research question: How are legally mandated responsibilities and practiced

Both groups of adolescents reported similar increases in distress after being excluded in a virtual ball-tossing game (Cyberball), but ado- lescents with a history of chronic

In particular, we are trying to make an analogy between state-building theory and drug cartels who can be said to become equivalents of states by contesting Mexican state

To summarize, shoal margin collapse scars and associated sediment deposits in the channel affect the local dynamics of the Western Scheldt, such as the sediment transport direction