• No results found

University of Groningen Affect and physical health Schenk, Maria

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Share "University of Groningen Affect and physical health Schenk, Maria"

Copied!
15
0
0

Bezig met laden.... (Bekijk nu de volledige tekst)

Hele tekst

(1)

Affect and physical health

Schenk, Maria

IMPORTANT NOTE: You are advised to consult the publisher's version (publisher's PDF) if you wish to cite from it. Please check the document version below.

Document Version

Publisher's PDF, also known as Version of record

Publication date: 2017

Link to publication in University of Groningen/UMCG research database

Citation for published version (APA):

Schenk, M. (2017). Affect and physical health: Studies on the link between affect and physiological processes. Rijksuniversiteit Groningen.

Copyright

Other than for strictly personal use, it is not permitted to download or to forward/distribute the text or part of it without the consent of the author(s) and/or copyright holder(s), unless the work is under an open content license (like Creative Commons).

Take-down policy

If you believe that this document breaches copyright please contact us providing details, and we will remove access to the work immediately and investigate your claim.

Downloaded from the University of Groningen/UMCG research database (Pure): http://www.rug.nl/research/portal. For technical reasons the number of authors shown on this cover page is limited to 10 maximum.

(2)

6

Differential association between Affect and

Somatic Symptoms at the Between- and

Within-individual Level

HM Schenk, EH Bos, JPJ Slaets, P de Jonge, JGM Rosmalen

(3)

Abstract

Objectives

The established between-subjects associations between affect and somatic symptoms have often been interpreted as indicating a causal effect of affect on somatic symptoms, but it is doubtful whether this is valid. In this study we evaluate the association between positive affect (PA), negative affect (NA) and somatic symptoms at both the between- and within-subject level.

Design and Methods

Diary data were collected in the context of an online study called “HowNutsAreThe-Dutch”. Participants filled out an online questionnaire, 3 times a day for 30 consecutive days. A mixed linear model was used to test the contemporaneous and lagged associa-tions between affect and somatic symptoms.

Results

586 participants (481 females, median age 39.6 years (range 18.1 to 71.4)) were in-cluded with a total number of 28,264 completed questionnaires. At the between-subjects level, a positive association between NA and somatic symptoms was found (B=0.60, P<.001), whereas the negative association between PA and somatic symptoms was much smaller (B=-0.14, P=.062). At the within-subject level, PA (B=-0.33, P<.001) was more strongly associated with somatic symptoms than NA (B=0.13, P<.001). The lagged ana-lyses showed a negative association between previous-day PA and somatic symptoms (B=-0.05, P=0.001).

Conclusions

The results suggest that NA is more important for differences in symptom levels between subjects, whereas PA is more important for variations in symptom levels within subjects. Moreover, our results suggest that an increase in PA is followed by a decrease in somatic symptoms after 24 hours, which suggests a causal effect.

(4)

Introduction

A close relationship exists between mental and physical health1,2. Mental disorders can

function as precipitating and perpetuating factors in the development or presence of somatic symptoms3–5. A positive association between negative affect (NA) and somatic

symptoms has been found in healthy individuals. Individuals who report more stressful events, daily hassles or more negative affect, report more somatic symptoms6,7. Likewise,

individuals who score high on personality traits associated with more NA, e.g. neuroticism, report more somatic symptoms. This association is notably stronger for symptoms in the psychosomatic cluster (e.g. nausea, stomach ache, fatigue) than for symptoms associated with infectious disease8. Also in patients with diseases with a clear organic origin, affect is

associated with the level of somatic symptoms. Patients who report higher levels of NA re-port higher, and patients with higher positive affect (PA) rere-port lower severity of somatic symptoms9–11. Little is known about how PA and NA dynamically influence each other, and

it is more or less assumed that a decrease in NA means that PA increases. However, stud-ies show that this is not true for all individuals12,13.

In somatoform disorders and functional somatic symptoms (FSS), the association be-tween mental and physical health is particularly evident. Cross-sectional, epidemiological studies show that individuals who suffer from FSS report more stress, stressful life events and daily hassles14–17. PA has been shown to be negatively associated with FSS18. Current

treatments of somatoform disorders and FSS are often focused on reducing precipitating and perpetuating factors of somatic symptoms19,20. Such factors include NA, stress,

de-pression, and associated dysfunctional cognitions and behaviors21–23. Although successful

to a certain extent, effect sizes of treatments are generally small20,24,25. This might be due

to an overestimation of the role of NA and an underestimation of the role of PA in the etiology and persistence of somatoform disorders or FSS.

Reviews by Pressman et al.9 and Steptoe et al.26 discuss the findings with regard to the

effect of PA on health. A limitation of most of the reported studies is that the effects of NA are not taken into account and most studies examine long term health effects, e.g. mortality or coronary heart disease. This makes it hard to identify the exact independent contribution of changes in PA on short-term health-parameters. Therefore, we would like to explore the association between day-to-day fluctuations in NA, PA and somatic symp-toms.

There are existing prospective longitudinal studies on PA, NA and somatic symptoms. However, most of them have long time windows between assessments, hampering the study of causality of such dynamic processes27,28. Studies who do have relatively short

time windows between assessments (e.g. a day), have smaller samples sizes, comprise a certain subgroup, e.g. college students29, or a shorter study period30,31. The goal of this

study is to evaluate the associations between positive affect (PA), negative affect (NA) and levels of somatic symptoms at both the between-subjects and within-subject level in longitudinal diary data. Since data at the between-subjects level are not directly

(5)

trans-latable to the within-subject level, the within-subject association may be different in size or even sign from the association between subjects32. For example, an earlier longitudinal

study showed a direct link between stress and the increase of FSS in several patients33,

however, the associations were inconsistent between individuals and not present, or even reversed in some. In addition, longitudinal diary data with shorter intervals allows lagged analyses34. Accordingly, the dynamic effects of affect on somatic symptoms can be

ex-amined within individuals.

In this study, our objective was to evaluate the contemporaneous and lagged associ-ations between PA, NA and somatic symptoms using diary data. First, we hypothesize a negative association between PA and somatic symptoms and a positive association between NA and somatic symptoms, at both the between- and within-subject level. Sec-ond, we hypothesize that the between-subjects associations between PA, NA and somatic symptoms differ from the associations at the within-subject level. Third, we hypothesize that affect has a lagged association with somatic symptoms from one moment to the next. We used data from a diary study in which PA, NA and somatic symptoms have been assessed35.

Materials and methods

Participants

This study was performed on diary data collected in the context of the study ‘HowNutsAreTheDutch’. ‘HowNutsAreTheDutch’ is an online platform and an ongoing study on the mental state of the Dutch population35,36. Participants were recruited through

the ‘HowNutsAreTheDutch’ website, www.hoegekis.nl (Dutch). The release of the study on the website was announced in magazines and newspapers. Participants received online information about the diary study. Informed consent was obtained online before entering the study. Eligible participants were age 18 or older and consented to their data being used for research. The protocol was approved by the local Medical Ethical Committee. In total, 629 individuals enrolled in the diary study.

Diary Study

During the study, participants received a text message on their smart phone 3 times a day during awakening time, every 6 hours, for 30 consecutive days. This text message contained a link to an online questionnaire, which was available for one hour. The tionnaire contained 43 items about emotions, activities, sleep, et cetera. Omitting ques-tions in the questionnaire was not possible, since the questionnaire could not be submitted if a question was skipped or unanswered. The questionnaire also contained twelve items about affect, based on the circumplex model of affect35,37. The following items were included to assess PA: ‘I feel relaxed, I feel energetic, I feel enthusiastic, I feel content, I feel calm, I feel cheerful’ (Cronbach’s α=.89 for t=1). The items used to assess NA were:

(6)

‘I feel gloomy, I feel anxious, I feel nervous, I feel irritable, I feel dull’ (Cronbach’s α=.79 for t=1)38,39. For this study the item ‘I feel tired’ was excluded, since it can be somatic as well as psychological. Participants had to report how they felt at that particular moment. Responding to the items was done by moving a slider on a visual analogue scale ranging from ‘not at all’ (0) to ‘very much’ (100). The PA and NA scores were calculated by taking the mean of the six and five items, respectively.

Validation outcome measure

The item ‘I experience somatic symptoms (e.g. headache, diarrhea, heavy legs, etc)’ was used to determine the level of somatic symptoms. This item was validated using data from the cross-sectional part of HowNutsAreTheDutch. HowNutsAreTheDutch included ques-tions on the occurrence of 28 somatic symptoms over the preceding 24 hours35, which

was assessed in a subgroup of participants. The means of the single diary item and the sum score of this cross-sectional symptom questionnaire correlated significantly (N=63, Spearman’s rho = .46; p < .001).

Statistical analyses

To test the between- and within-subject association between affect and somatic symptoms, a mixed linear model was constructed. The PA and NA scores were used as predictors in the model. The advantage of the hierarchical multilevel model is that the technique deals with missing values automatically. Therefore it was not necessary to impute the data. All participants with at least one completed questionnaire are thus included in the analyses. Although participants who filled out only one questionnaire do no contribute to the with-in-individual associations, they do contribute to the between-individual associations.

Contemporaneous associations

To remove long-term time trends, the data were detrended, for each individual sep-arately 40,41. To disaggregate the between- and within-subject effects in the analyses,

the predictor variables were person-mean centered (Curran & Bauer, 2011). Both the person-mean centered values and the person means were entered as predictors in the models, to assess the within- and the between-subjects association, respectively. Models with random intercepts and random slopes were used for the within-subjects effects, to provide insight into the heterogeneity of these effects. An autoregressive covariance structure at level 1 was found to be optimal according to the Bayesian Information Cri-terion (BIC).

Lagged associations

The appropriate time lag to analyze the effect of PA and NA on somatic symptoms has not been described. In addition, the optimal lag length can differ between individuals33.

Analyses about the optimal lag length exceeds the scope of the study, therefore we stud-ied the effects of affect up to three previous time points (t-1, t-2, t-3), which reflects a

(7)

period of 24 hours, which seemed a suitable time frame. To test the lagged within-subject association between affect and somatic symptoms, the three lagged values of PA and NA were used as predictors in a second mixed linear model, to correct for the influence of each of them. To correct for auto-correlation, the lagged (t-1) variable of somatic symp-toms was entered as a predictor too. A scaled identity structure at level 1 was found to be optimal according to the BIC.

A p-value of 0.05 was used to determine statistical significance. All statistical analyses were done using SPSS 22.0 (IBM Corp, Armonk, NY). The assumption of homoscedastic-ity of the residuals was checked by plotting the standardized predictor values against the standardized residuals. Normality of the residuals was checked by a Q-Q plot, and both assumptions were met. In addition, to ensure the robustness of our results, we boot-strapped our models (k = 1000), which did not lead to an alteration of our conclusions, and the normal, non-bootstrapped results were presented. Since the association of PA and physical symptoms can depend on the level of NA, or vice versa, the interaction PA x NA was explored at both the between-individual (B=0.002, SE =0.003, p = 0.612) and the within-individual level (B=0.001, SE =0.001, p = 0.077) in the contemporaneous model. However, none of the interaction terms was significant and therefore we excluded them from the final models.

Results

Sample characteristics

In total, 629 participants (517 females) enrolled in the diary study. 586 participants (481 females) filled out at least one questionnaire and were included in the analyses. In this subgroup, the median of the age of the participants was 39.6 years (range 18.1 to 71.4) and participants were highly educated (high 84%, middle 12%; and low 4%). The total number of completed questionnaires was 28,264; the median number of completed questionnaires per participant was 59 (range 1 to 90). The median response time was within 11.6 minutes (range 1 to 60) after receiving the text message. Median time to fill out the questionnaire was 3 minutes. Reporting the same score on a certain item did occur, but only in a small number of participants and on specific items. However, in those cases participants usually reported not having any physical complaints, and did show variabili-ty on other items. Overall, the median score for PA was 57.4 (range 0 to 100), the medi-an score for NA was 20.2 (rmedi-ange 0 to 100), medi-and the medimedi-an score for somatic symptoms was 16.1 (range 0 to 100). More details about the diary study can be found elsewhere42.

Contemporaneous associations

At the between-subjects level, the mixed linear model showed a significant positive asso-ciation between NA and somatic symptoms (B = 0.60, p < .001). The negative associa-tion between PA and somatic symptoms approached significance (B = -0.14, p = .062)

(8)

Table 1: Mixed linear model of somatic symptoms as a function of positive affect and negative affect

Estimate CI95 Lower CI95 Upper P Between-subjects Intercept 19.30 8.66 29.94 <.001 NA 0.60 0.47 0.72 <.001 PA -0.14 -0.29 0.01 .062 Within-subject NA 0.13 0.09 0.16 <.001 PA -0.33 -0.36 -0.29 <.001 Random-effects variances Intercept 242.77 213.93 275.51 <.001 NA 0.08 0.06 0.10 <.001 PA 0.07 0.05 0.09 <.001

Note: N = 586; duration of the study = 30 days, 3 measurements per day. Total number of completed

ques-tionnaires =28,264. PA = positive affect, NA = negative affect. Dependent variable: somatic symptoms (scale 0-100), CI95 = 95% confidence interval.

(Table 1). At the within-subject level, the analysis showed a significant positive association between NA and somatic symptoms (B = 0.13, p < .001) (Table 1). In addition, a signifi-cant negative association was found between PA and somatic symptoms within individuals (B = -0.33, p < .001).

The random-effects variances, presented in the lower panel of table 2, showed sub-stantial between-subjects variability in the contemporaneous associations between affect and somatic symptoms. The variation around the NA slope was 0.08, corresponding with an SD of √0.08= 0.28. On the basis of this SD and an estimate of 0.13, it can be esti-mated that 95% of the population has coefficients between -0.42 and 0.68 for negative affect. The variation around the PA slope, with an estimate of -0.33, was 0.07, corre-sponding with an SD of 0.26, and slopes ranging between -0.85 and 0.19 for positive affect in approximately 95% of the population.

Lagged associations

The lagged associations at the within-subject level with lags up to one day during awak-ening time (t-1, t-2, t-3) showed the following results. At the within-subjects level the anal-ysis showed a significant effect of PA at t-3 (B = -0.05, P = .001). For NA at t¬-1, t-2 and t-3 and PA at t¬-1 and t-2, no significant effects were found (Table 2). The random effects in the model on the lagged associations also showed large variability between subjects in the effect of affect on somatic symptoms, with slopes ranging between -0.20

(9)

Table 2: Mixed linear model of somatic symptoms as a function of lagged values of positive affect and negative affect

Estimate CI95 Lower CI95 Upper P Within-subject Intercept 25.53 23.84 27.22 <.001 Somatic Symptoms (t-1) 0.26 0.23 0.29 <.001 NA (t-1) 0.00 -0.04 0.04 .915 PA (t-1) -0.02 -0.05 0.02 .325 NA (t-2) 0.01 -0.03 0.05 .593 PA (t-2) 0.00 -0.03 0.03 .932 NA (t-3) -0.03 -0.07 0.01 .093 PA (t-3) -0.05 -0.08 -0.02 .001 Random-effect variances Intercept 330.88 289.10 378.69 <.001 Somatic Symptoms (t-1) 0.04 0.03 0.06 <.001 NA (t-1) 0.01 0.00 0.03 .030 PA (t-1) 0.01 0.01 0.02 .004 NA (t-2) 0.00 0.00 0.03 .236 PA (t-2) 0.01 0.00 0.02 .081 NA (t-3) 0.02 0.01 0.03 .001 PA (t-3) 0.00 0.00 0.02 .277

Note: N = 525; Total number of completed questionnaires =28,146; PA = positive affect, NA = negative affect. Dependent variable: somatic symptoms (scale 0-100). CI95 = 95% confidence interval; t-1, t-2, t-3 = three preceding measuring points.

and 0.20 for lag 1 of NA, between -0.31 and 0.25 for lag 3 of NA, and between -0.22 and 0.18 for lag 1 of PA in approximately 95% of the population. The random slopes of lag 2 of NA, and 2 and 3 of PA were non-significant, indicating there was no significant variability in the effect.

Since the median level of the somatic symptoms was low, we performed additional sensitivity analyses, with subgroups using 1) only the values of somatic symptoms above the 50th percentile, increasing this threshold each time with 10%. In addition, we repeat-ed the analysis in 2) individuals who reportrepeat-ed on average higher levels of somatic symp-toms, starting from the median, increasing this threshold each time with 10%. Overall, the results remained approximately the same in size and sign. In both subgroups 1 and 2, the association between PA and somatic symptoms at the individual level remained inversely significant in each severity class. The association between NA and somatic symptoms at the individual level did not remain significant in subgroup 1. Neither did the association between PA, NA and somatic symptoms at the group level in both subgroups, due to a decrease in power (data not shown).

(10)

Discussion

In this study a striking difference was found in the between- and within-subject asso-ciations between affect and somatic symptoms. The results showed that NA was more strongly associated with somatic symptoms at the between-subject level, in contrast to the within-subjects level, where PA was more strongly associated. Moreover, we showed that within subjects, an increase in PA is followed by a small but significant decrease in somatic symptoms the following day, whereas no significant lagged association was found for NA. This study suggests that NA is more important for differences in symptom levels be-tween subjects, whereas PA is more important for variations in symptom levels within subjects. Our findings imply that between-individual results might camouflage individual processes. Therefore, the consequences of translating between-individual results to an individual should be considered, since results derived at between person level do not always apply to the individual32. Another interesting finding, which emphasizes the

impor-tance of acknowledging the difference between within-individual and between-individ-ual level results, is the considerable heterogeneity observed in the effects. This shows the diversity across individuals in the degree to which affect is related to somatic symptoms. The fixed effect of the mixed linear model showed the average within-subject effect, but the significant random effects imply that the within-individual associations differed in size and even sign between individuals.

The small, but significant association between PA and physical symptoms the next day implies that higher levels of PA are followed by lower levels of somatic symptoms the next day. Moreover, the between-person variability in this effect was non-significant, which indicates that this association was in general present in the participants. It is remarkable that our results show an effect of PA only after 24 hours. It is possible that physiological effects need some time to become apparent. In addition, a lagged association has al-ready been found between daily hassles and somatic symptoms, up to 4 days43.

Our results suggest that current treatments aimed at reducing somatic symptoms might benefit from a shift to a stronger focus on increasing PA. Höhn et al.44 showed already

that individuals with depressive symptoms who had greater PA persistence showed bet-ter prevention and recovery. Positive psychology inbet-terventions are expanding already, and more and more attention is given to the idea that positive psychology interventions can be a useful supplement to traditional treatments45, and in our opinion also in the

treatment of FSS. Furthermore, the heterogeneity between individuals suggests that in-terventions may profit from an approach which is based on a within-subject model. We restricted our analyses to the role of affect on somatic symptoms, but it is without doubt that the effect of somatic symptoms on PA and NA is interesting as well. A vicious circle may exist between NA and somatic symptoms, and PA may be a target to break this.

The strengths of this study include the large sample size, the large number of assess-ments within individuals, the duration of 30 days, and the age diversity of the partici-pants. A handful of studies have examined the association between stress or affect and

(11)

somatic symptoms in a longitudinal setting. However, these studies have a smaller, less diverse population46,47 and/or a less intense study protocol29,30. The duration of this study

and the relatively small interval between the measurements allowed us to examine dy-namic processes within individuals while taking into account individual variance. More-over, the relatively small time windows between the assessments limits recall bias, often present when using self-reports48.

This study has also limitations. The biased sample with an overrepresentation of higher educated women, and thus the perhaps limited generalizability of the study, should be taken into account. However, the findings at the individual level are not influenced by this selection bias, since participants serve as their own controls. It should be taken into account that the data are based on self-report measures, which may have introduced dif-ferential measurement error (an error with different magnitude or direction across groups or subjects) at the between- and within-individual level. The momentary assessments do not eliminate the error and bias associated with retrospection, but do reduce this problem considerably. In addition, there was only one item about somatic symptoms in the diary study.

However, we showed that there is a significant correlation between the daily average of this item and the scores on a more elaborate questionnaire, assessed at a single day, which expands the validity of this single item. The generally low level of symptoms re-ported by participants could be regarded as a limitation of this study. However, sensitivi-ty analysis revealed that in subgroups with higher levels of somatic symptoms, the associ-ations between affect and somatic symptoms remain roughly the same. Another limitation is that the protocol did not include an assessment in the night. This was because we did not want to interfere with participants’ sleep, which would reduce ecological validity. Be-cause of this gap in the night, the intervals between assessments were not entirely evenly spaced. However, it can be argued that affect is not present during sleep and cannot be measured. Therefore, we evenly spaced the measurements during awakening time.

This study provided insights into the associations between affect and somatic symp-toms, demonstrating the strong concurrent and prospective association of PA and somatic symptoms at the within-subject level. Future studies should reveal whether in psychopatho-logical conditions PA and NA contribute to the presence of somatic symptoms.

(12)

References

1 Brod S, Rattazzi L, Piras G, D’Acquisto F. ‘As above, so below’ examining the interplay between emotion and the immune system. Immunology 2014; 143: 311–318.

2 Prince M, Patel V, Saxena S, Maj M, Maselko J, Phillips MR et al. No health without mental health. Lancet 2007;

370: 859–877.

3 Haug TT, Mykletun A, Dahl A a. The Association Between Anxiety, Depression, and Somatic Symptoms in a Large Population: The HUNT-II Study. Psychosom Med 2004; 66: 845–851.

4 Simms LJ, Prisciandaro JJ, Krueger RF, Goldberg DP. The structure of depression, anxiety and somatic symptoms in primary care. Psychol Med 2012; 42: 15–28.

5 Simon GE, VonKorff M, Piccinelli M, Fullerton C, Ormel J. An international study of the relation between somatic symptoms and depression. N Engl J Med 1999; 341: 1329–35.

6 Clark LA, Watson D. Mood and the mundane: Relations between daily life events and self-reported mood. J Pers Soc Psychol 1988; 54: 296–308.

7 Watson D, Pennebaker JW. Health complaints, stress, and distress: exploring the central role of negative affec-tivity. Psychol Rev 1989; 96: 234–54.

8 Rosmalen JGM, Neeleman J, Gans ROB, de Jonge P. The association between neuroticism and self-reported common somatic symptoms in a population cohort. J Psychosom Res 2007; 62: 305–311.

9 Pressman SD, Cohen S. Does positive affect influence health? Psychol Bull 2005; 131: 925–71.

10 Koller M, Heitmann K, Kussmann J, Lorenz W. Symptom reporting in cancer patients II. Cancer 1999; 86: 1609– 1620.

11 Kvaal SA, Patodia S. Relations among positive affect, negative affect, and somatic symptoms in a medically ill patient sample. Psychol Rep 2000; 87: 227–33.

12 Wichers M, Lothmann C, Simons CJP, Nicolson NA, Peeters F. The dynamic interplay between negative and posi-tive emotions in daily life predicts response to treatment in depression: a momentary assessment study. Br J Clin Psychol 2012; 51: 206–22.

13 Diener E, Emmons RA. The independence of positive and negative affect. J Pers Soc Psychol 1984; 47: 1105–17. 14 Christiansen LM, Copeland EP, Stapert EB. Predictors of somatic symptoms in younger rural adolescents.

Adoles-cence 2008; 43: 791–806.

15 Greene JW, Walker LS, Hickson G, Thompson J. Stressful life events and somatic complaints in adolescents. Pe-diatrics 1985; 75: 19–22.

16 Kroenke K. Patients presenting with somatic complaints: epidemiology, psychiatric co-morbidity and manage-ment. Int J Methods Psychiatr Res 2003; 12: 34–43.

17 Tak LM, Kingma EM, van Ockenburg SL, Ormel J, Rosmalen JGM. Age- and sex-specific associations between adverse life events and functional bodily symptoms in the general population. J Psychosom Res 2015; 79: 112–116.

18 De Gucht V, Fischler B, Heiser W. Neuroticism, alexithymia, negative affect, and positive affect as determinants of medically unexplained symptoms. Pers Individ Dif 2004; 36: 1655–1667.

19 Edwards TM, Stern A, Clarke DD, Ivbijaro G, Kasney LM. The treatment of patients with medically unexplained symptoms in primary care: a review of the literature. Ment Health Fam Med 2010; 7: 209–21.

20 Kroenke K. Efficacy of Treatment for Somatoform Disorders: A Review of Randomized Controlled Trials. Psycho-som Med 2007; 69: 881–888.

21 Edwards RR, Dworkin RH, Sullivan MD, Turk DC, Wasan AD. The Role of Psychosocial Processes in the Develop-ment and Maintenance of Chronic Pain. J Pain 2016; 17: T70-92.

22 Gupta M a. Review of somatic symptoms in post-traumatic stress disorder. Int Rev Psychiatry 2013; 25: 86–99. 23 Henningsen P, Zipfel S, Herzog W. Management of functional somatic syndromes. Lancet (London, England)

2007; 369: 946–55.

24 Kleinstäuber M, Witthöft M, Steffanowski A, van Marwijk H, Hiller W, Lambert MJ. Pharmacological interventions for somatoform disorders in adults. In: Kleinstäuber M (ed). Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews. John Wiley & Sons, Ltd: Chichester, UK, 2014 doi:10.1002/14651858.CD010628.pub2.

25 van Dessel N, den Boeft M, van der Wouden JC, Kleinstäuber M, Leone SS, Terluin B et al. Non-pharmacological interventions for somatoform disorders and medically unexplained physical symptoms (MUPS) in adults. Cochra-ne database Syst Rev 2014; 11: CD011142.

(13)

26 Steptoe A, Dockray S, Wardle J. Positive Affect and Psychobiological Processes Relevant to Health. J Pers 2009;

77: 1747–1776.

27 de Jonge P, Bos EH. Tobacco smoking and depression: time to move on to a new research paradigm in medicine? BMC Med 2013; 11: 138.

28 Tennen H, Affleck G. Daily processes in coping with chronic pain: Methods and analytic strategies. In: Handbook of coping: Theory, research, applications. John Wiley & Sons: Oxford, England, 1996, pp 151–177.

29 Watson D. Intraindividual and interindividual analyses of positive and negative affect: their relation to health complaints, perceived stress, and daily activities. J Pers Soc Psychol 1988; 54: 1020–30.

30 Charles ST, Almeida DM. Daily reports of symptoms and negative affect: Not all symptoms are the same. Psychol Health 2006; 21: 1–17.

31 Gartland N, O’Connor DB, Lawton R, Ferguson E. Investigating the effects of conscientiousness on daily stress, affect and physical symptom processes: a daily diary study. Br J Health Psychol 2014; 19: 311–28.

32 Hamaker EL. Why researchers should think ‘within-person’: A paradigmatic rationale. In: Handbook of research methods for studying daily life. Guilford Press: New York, 2012, pp 43–61.

33 van Gils A, Burton C, Bos EH, Janssens K a M, Schoevers R a, Rosmalen JGM. Individual variation in temporal relationships between stress and functional somatic symptoms. J Psychosom Res 2014; 77: 34–9.

34 Bolger N, Davis A, Rafaeli E. Diary Methods: Capturing Life as it is Lived. Annu Rev Psychol 2003; 54: 579–616. 35 van der Krieke L, Jeronimus BF, Blaauw FJ, Wanders RBK, Emerencia AC, Schenk HM et al. HowNutsAreTheDutch

(HoeGekIsNL): A crowdsourcing study of mental symptoms and strengths. Int J Methods Psychiatr Res 2016; 25: 123–144.

36 Blaauw F, Krieke L Van Der, Bos E, Emerencia A, Jeronimus BF, Schenk M et al. HowNutsAreTheDutch : Personali-zed Feedback on a National Scale. Expand Boundaries Heal Informatics Using AI Mak Pers Particip Med A Real 2014; : 6–10.

37 Feldman Barrett L, Russell JA. Independence and bipolarity in the structure of current affect. J Pers Soc Psychol 1998; 74: 967–984.

38 Russell JA. A circumplex model of affect. J Pers Soc Psychol 1980; 39: 1161.

39 Yik MSM, Russell J a., Barrett LF. Structure of self-reported current affect: Integration and beyond. J Pers Soc Psychol 1999; 77: 600–619.

40 Chatfield C. The Analysis of Time Series. 5th ed. Chapman and Hall: London, 1996.

41 Curran PJ, Bauer DJ. The Disaggregation of Within-Person and Between-Person Effects in Longitudinal Models of Change. Annu Rev Psychol 2011; 62: 583–619.

42 van der Krieke L, Blaauw FJ, Emerencia AC, Schenk HM, Slaets JPJ, Bos EH et al. Temporal Dynamics of Health and Well-Being: A Crowdsourcing Approach to Momentary Assessments and Automated Generation of Perso-nalized Feedback. Psychosom Med 2016; 79: 213–223.

43 Dancey CP, Taghavi M, Fox RJ. The relationship between daily stress and symptoms of irritable bowel: a time-se-ries approach. J Psychosom Res 1998; 44: 537–545.

44 Höhn P, Menne-Lothmann C, Peeters F, Nicolson NA, Jacobs N, Derom C et al. Moment-to-moment transfer of po-sitive emotions in daily life predicts future course of depression in both general population and patient samples. PLoS One 2013; 8: e75655.

45 Linley PA, Joseph S, Maltby J, Harrington S, Wood AM. Positive Psychology Applications. Oxford University Press, 2009 doi:10.1093/oxfordhb/9780195187243.013.0005.

46 DeLongis A, Folkman S, Lazarus RS. The impact of daily stress on health and mood: Psychological and social resources as mediators. J Pers Soc Psychol 1988; 54: 486–495.

47 Larsen RJ, Kasimatis M. Day-to-Day Physical Symptoms: Individual Differences in the Occurrence, Duration, and Emotional Concomitants of Minor Daily Illnesses. J Pers 1991; 59: 387–423.

48 Houtveen JH, Oei NYL. Recall bias in reporting medically unexplained symptoms comes from semantic memory. J Psychosom Res 2007; 62: 277–282.

(14)
(15)

Referenties

GERELATEERDE DOCUMENTEN

Physiqual enables the use of sensor data from commercially available wearable devices in EMA mental health research by interfacing with service providers to export data in

The aim of this paper is to study the association between PA and established biological markers for an AL profile, based on inflammatory, cardiovascular, and metabolic mar- kers..

In the current study, we analyzed the time series of 10 healthy individuals, with a length of 63 days to investigate the bidirectional contemporaneous and lagged asso- ciations

In all three studies in part two (chapter 6: Affect and somatic symptoms, chapter 7: Affect and allostatic load, and chapter 8: Affect and inflammatory markers), PA has a signifi-

In chapter 2, we studied the presence of inflammatory markers in urine, to investigate whether it is possible to use urine for non-invasive sampling in inten- sive day-to-day

Met statistische technieken, die ook worden gebruikt voor het analyseren van beurskoers- en, werd gekeken naar de associatie tussen positief affect, negatief affect en acht

Other than for strictly personal use, it is not permitted to download or to forward/distribute the text or part of it without the consent of the author(s) and/or copyright

Physical activity in recipients of solid organ transplantation (prof CP van der Schans, prof PU Dijkstra, dr R Dekker).