• No results found

0 Habitat-preference of foraging Brent Geese

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Share "0 Habitat-preference of foraging Brent Geese"

Copied!
36
0
0

Bezig met laden.... (Bekijk nu de volledige tekst)

Hele tekst

(1)

Habitat-preference of foraging Brent Geese (Branta bernicla bernicla) in relation to

management-type

ci

C

Meike Bulten

Department of Behavioural Ecology Rijkuniversiteit Groningen

Biologisch Centrum, Haren October 1999

0

Supervisors: Maarten Loonen Julia Stahl Daan Bos

(2)

U

Habitat-preference of foraging Brent Geese (Branta bernicla bernicla) in relation to

management-type

An experimental test on the polder of Schiermonnikoog.

Meike Bulten

Department of Behavioural Ecology

Rijkuniversiteit Groningen Supervisors: Maarten Loonen Biologisch Centrum, Haren

Julia Stahl October 1999

Daan Bos

'l('r-

EL ish

(3)

Contents:

1. Abstract 2. Introduction 3. Study Area 5

4. Methods 8

4.1. Use of different management-types 9 4.2. The Experiment 9

4.2.1. Experimental design 9

4.2.2. Behavioural parameters 9

4.2.3. Vegetation parameters 10 4.3. Statistical analysis 10

4.3.1. Data expelled from the analysis 1111

4.3.2. Statistics 5. Results 12

5.1 Goose behaviour during the experiment 13

5.2 Vegetation changes 13

5.2.1 Height, biomass and tiller density 1414

5.2.2 Correlation between height and biomass 16

5.2.3 Nitrogen content and digestibility of the grass 17 5.2.4 Intake Rate

5.3. Preference of the goose 1918

5.3.1. Foraging preference from observation 19

5.3.3. Combination of droppingcounts and observation 20

5.4. Foraging preference and vegetation 22

6. Discussion

6.1. Preference of the geese for the combination SG vs. NG 2424

6.1.1. Height and intake rate 24

6.1.2. Nitrogen content and digestibility 25

6.1.3 Optimisation of the nutrient intake 25

6.2. Preference of the geese for the combination SG vs. GG 26

6.2.1 Nutrient intake 26

6.2.2. Height and intake rate 27

6.3. A comparison with Ameland 27

6.4. Geese grazing without facilitation ofsheep 28

6.5. Comparing polder feeding with salt marsh feeding 28 7. Conclusions

8. Appendices 30

Appendix 1: Schiermonnikoog 31

9. Acknowledgements 31

10. References 32

33

(4)

1. Abstract

In this study the preference

of foraging Brent

geese is studied. The study

was performed on the polder of theDutch Waddenisland Schiermonnikoog.

Wintering geese feeding in the polder are expected to prefer grazed areas due to a shorter sward, a higher protein content, a higher tiller density and less bare ground in these areas than in areas with other management types. Therefore, the expectation is that geese prefer sheep grazed grass to cattle grazed grass, which is at that time of year, ungrazed grass.

In a choice-experiment, where the geese could choose between two treatments, two geese were allowed to forage on the treatment of their choice. Foraging time was used as an indication for the foraging preference of the geese. Several vegetation parameters were measured for each treatment.

The geese showed a large preference for the sheep grazed areas compared to ungrazed areas. The sheep grazed area had a higher tiller density, a lower sward height and biomass and the intake rate of sheep grazed grass was higher than in the ungrazed parcels. In the sheep grazed areas nutrient intake was highest.

The geese preferred grazing on sheep grazed compared to goose grazed areas. The goose grazed areas have a lower sward height, a lower biomass and a lower intake rate.

Height and not treatment seems to be the major determinant a goose selects on.

Nevertheless, there is a correlation between sward heightand treatment this is because treatment is a way to achieve a certain grass height. Height is related to intake rate;

they show an optimum curve; the geese have a preferred foraging height of the grass where they can optimise theirintake rate.

(5)

2. Introduction

On Schiermonnikoog, one of the Dutch Friesian Islands, Brent Geese (Brania bernicia bernicla) make a habitat shift in the early spring. Thegeese leave the foraging areas on the polder, pastures that are protected by dikes from the

sea, and go to the salt

marshes (Prins & Ydenberg 1985, McKay el al. 1994, Arends 1998). This shift is related to the digestibility of the eatable grasses (Boudewijn 1984). During winter and early spring, food supplies on the saltmarsh are not sufficient for large goose flocks (Vickery et al. 1995). Hence, the majority of the birds are forced to graze on polder grasses. At the end of March, digestibility of polder grasses decreases. Reason for this is the sudden and immense growth of these grasses. At the same time, the grasses of the salt marsh start to grow and here digestibility rises (Boudewijn 1984). Then, the levels of digestibility are almost equal for both places. In this period, the geese make the habitat shift from polderto salt marsh.

Habitat shifts are well reported for all kinds of herbivores (Baker 1978). The basic theory to explain such movements is that animals shift from one area to another to capitalise on better

or more resources, usually food (Prins

& Ydenberg 1985).

Belovsky (1981) stated that the behaviour of the moose (A ices alces) supports this theory. The diet of the moose changes as the season progressed. In the late spring / beginning of the summer the animal feeds mainly on terrestrial

plants and in

midsummer the moose switch feeding on aquatic plants mainly. By doing so, the moose can profit from the high quality of both terrestrial and aquatic plants.

Though Brent geese on Schiermonnikoog make a habitat shift from the polder to the salt marsh, this process does not occur on the neighbouring island of Ameland. On Ameland the number of Brent geese on the polder do not decrease in the spring (Van der Graaf 1998, Arends 1998). A possible reason for this is that the polder of Ameland is, in contrast with Schiermonnikoog, sheep grazed (Van der Graaf 1998). Here, the assumption is that the grazing of the sheep facilitates the geese. The grazing keeps the foraging areas in a preferable stage for the geese.

This process, whereby the use of the herb layer by one grazing species increases the availability of theseresources for other animals, has been termed 'grazing facilitation' (Gordon 1988). Mostly, it are the larger herbivores who are facilitating the smaller ones, but on Ameland for instance, not only sheep grazing but also grazing of geese early in the season can facilitate geese later on in the season (Van der Graaf 1998).

In nature, the process of facilitation occurs spontaneously. Animals eat the vegetation they prefer and by doing so they facilitate, unconsciously, other animals. Human's can control and/or influence this process by, for instance, grazing by live stock on meadows or salt marshes. Use of management types like grazing or cutting for silage has turned into a possible facilitation process. Small herbivores like hares, ducks or wild geese can benefit from this. Each of these management types has their own specific way of influencing the vegetation.

As grass grows, fibre content increases, whereas nitrogen content decreases. Young tender shoots contain relatively high concentrations of nitrogen (Riddington et a!.

(6)

1997). After the grass has reached a certain height, the tiller density of the grass decreases. This is caused by the competition for light or nutrients. It inhibits the growth of new sprouts.

By cutting the grass, the height and the senescence of thegrass decreases (Grant ci a!.

1981). Growth of young sprouts will enhance and the percentage of living material of the grass will increase under this management regime (Vickery ci al. 1994).

Grazing by large herbivores enhances the growth of young shoots (Gordon & Lindsay 1990), thus increasing the nitrogen-content of the sward. Grazed vegetation has, except for a high quality (Gordon & Lindsay 1990, Vickery ci a!. 1994, Hobbs ci a!.

1996), also a sward, which contains more living material (Coppock ci a!. 1983, Gordon 1988, Summers 1990). Grazed vegetation has a higher tiller density (Grant ci a!. 1981, Jones ei a!. 1982, Parsons & Penning 1988) and a greater diversity of plant- species (Smith & Rushton 1994). Grazing decreases the height of the grazed plants (McNaughton 1976).

Both sheep and cattle influence the vegetation by grazing. However, sheep and cattle have different ways of grazing, which causes a different impact

on the grazed

vegetation. At current agricultural practice in the Netherlands, cattle and sheep are grazing outside at different times in the season. Sheep graze the whole winter and spring, whereas cattle only graze in the fields from April till October (Van der Graaf

1998). Cows pull the grass of the sward, while sheep nibble the grass (Holmes 1980).

This leads to a difference in height of the grass sward. Cattle grazed swards are of greater height than sheep grazed swards (Kralovec ci a!. 1991). Both sheep and cattle prefer the green leaves and stem of the grasses (Crawley 1983), but sheep are more selective on which species they feed (Holmes 1980). Plants in sheep-grazed swards regrow more quickly and accumulate more herbage (Murphy ci a!. 1995). Sheep grazing increases soil bulk density, percentage Poapraiensis and extractable soil P (Abaye cia!. 1997).

A reasonable part of the world population of Brent geese (Brania bcrnicla bcrnicla) spent the winter in the Dutch Waddensea. A small part, about 3000 Brent geese in the winter of 1998-1999, winters on the polder and salt marshes of Schiermonnikoog (pers. comm. J. Stahl).

The maximum number of Brent Geese counted

on Schiermonnikoog increased from around 1500 in the 1960s to over 4000 in the late 1970s. From here onwards, no further increase was observed (van der Wal 1998).

Apparently, the carrying capacity of the island was reached in the late l970s.

During late winter and early spring geese have to accumulate body-reserves fortheir migration and breeding

season in the Arctic. The birds obtain most of their body- reserves in the last seven weeks before migration (Ydenberg & Prins 1981). The geese prefer grazing on the salt marshes (Ebbinge 1992), but by depletion of these salt marshes in late winter, they are forced to graze elsewhere (Vickery ci a!. 1995). From November till late March or early April they forage primarily on the grasslands in the polder (Boudewijn 1984). Here they consume the grasses Lolium percnne L. and Poa pralensis L., cultivated for livestock (Ydenberg & Prins 1981). At the end of March, the geese switch back to the saltmarsh (Boudewijn 1984, Prins & Ydenberg 1985, McKay ci a!. 1994, Vickery ci a!. 1995, Arends 1998).

(7)

Food intake, in herbivores, increases with plant standing crop, but saturates at high standing crop (Holling 1965). When grass reaches a certain height, it is no longer profitable for geese to eat it. Geese are also very selective feeders. They typically select for low fibre and high nitrogen content in the grass (Owen et a!. 1977). The birds prefer short and dense vegetation (Hassall & Riddington in prep.) with many tillers.

Middleton (1992) found that Greylags (Anser anser) preferred sites, which were grazed concurrently by other species of herbivores. He also states that these geese did not show a preference for sites of which the grass had been cut. Thus, it is expected that geese select short, grazed grasslands.

In this study the hypothesis will be tested that foraging Brent geese prefer grazed areas. The geese in this study will be held on different manipulated grass swards.

Differences between the

grass swards, which have been treated

with different management types, will be determined, as well as the differences in the behaviour

of

the geese on the different grass swards (plots).

The geese used in this study came from a flock that was held at the Zoological Laboratory (Haren, Netherlands) for two years. For the experiment, they were offered the choice between these different grass swards.

(8)

3. Study Area

This study was performed in the spring of 1999 on Schiermonnikoog, one of the Friesian Waddensea islands in the Netherlands. The polder of Schiermonnikoog (c. 265

ha)is situated at the western part of the island (figure 1 & appendix 1). The main part of the polder is used as grassland; a smaller part is used for agricultural crops. The grasslands, with two dominant species Poa pratensis and Lolium perenne that form almost 100% of the cover, are usually cut for silage at the end of May. In some parts there is aftermath grazing with young cattle but the most will be cut again later in the year.

The geese prefer particular parts of the polder and return every year at almost the same places. The farmers of those meadows have not consequently chased the geese away for the last ten years, so the birds can forage in a relatively quiet environment. For the last 5 to 10 years, the geese have been foraging on three specific sites. The experiment is situated at one of these specific sites, in the eastern part of the polder. The size of this meadow, rented from a nearby farmer, was c. 2 ha. In the past, this meadow was used as grassland (hay mowing). The farmer treated both his land and the land we rented like usual. Chemical fertiliser was used twice, organic fertiliser only once.

Organic fertiliser was applied in the first half of March. The first application of the chemical fertiliserwas in the beginning of April, the second in the end of May. Cutting for silage of the meadow proceeded after the experiment was over. Fieldwork for this study lasted from the first half ofMarch until the beginning of June.

Figure 1: The map shows the polder (enclosed by a sea-dyke) and salt marsh of Schiermonnikoog (Prins en Ydenberg 1985).

(9)

4. Methods

4.1. Use of different management-types

The geese used in the experiment were held on manipulated grass swards, called plots.

Three different management-types were selected. Different vegetation types were created by the use of the different management types.

The following management-types were used:

Sheep grazing;

• Goose grazing;

• No grazing.

The management-type sheep grazing took place all over the meadow, the other types only in special plots.

On the sheep-grazed meadow (c. 1 ha), 9 sheep and their 6 lambs were grazing from the first of April until the end of June. The grazing pressure was intensified in time:

while the number of sheep was kept constant, the size of the pasture decreased. By doing this, the height of the grass was maintained at almost 4 cm, despite the increase in growth rate of the grasses over the spring season. The sheep remained at the pasture until the end of the experiment.

For the maintenance of the goose grazed pastures, three Barnacle Geese were used.

They were allowed to graze the plot for c. 6 days. Three days before the plots were used the geese were removed of the grass, so it could recover.

All the plots, except for the sheep grazed areas, were protected with netting to prevent natural grazing (hares, rabbits or wild geese) until the moment of experimental use (van der Wal et a!. 1998).

Before the observation started all the droppings of sheep and/or the Barnacle geese were removed from the plot.

Table 1. Management-ty )CS andtheir codes used in this study.

Management type Code

Sheep grazing SG

Goose grazing GG

No grazing NG

4.2. The Experiment

4.2.1. Experimental design

The northern side of the meadow was used for the experiment. It is located close to the road. The distance between the experiment and the road was about 50 m.

The experiment was done with two semi-captive Brent

geese (Branta bernicla

bernicla). The geese were paired. In this experimental design, where the habitat preference of foraging geese was tested, the geese had to make a choice between two plots; each treated with a different management-type. Each testing-day, the geese were

(10)

offered the choice between two different vegetation types. Possible combinations were GG vs. NG, NG vs. SG and SG vs. GG. By observing the behaviour of the geese, the preference for a certain habitat of the foraging geese could be determined. In total there were three experimental

periods, lasting 10 to 12 days

each. Between the experimental periods there was a 12-day resting interval.

Each day a new plot was provided to the birds to prevent depletion of the forage (Van der Wal eta!. 1998). The birds were held in a 4 x 4 m cage, so the actual size of the

two plots was 2 x 4 m. Fresh

water for drinking and bathing was freely available during the day. The water was put in the middle, so it couldn't effect the choice the geese made. The geese were observed from an observation-tent, so the observation itself could not influence the geese. In the resting periods, the birds were held in a 4 x 4-rn. cage on a grass sward. If necessary, supplementary waterfowl foodwas provided.

Before the experiment, the birds were held for two weeks on a grass sward, without supplementary food.

4.2.2. Behavioural parameters

The observation of the geese lasted for 2-3 hours a day, mainly in the morning when

the geese were foraging. During

this observation, several 10-minute bouts of observation were conducted. Also, a camera was recording the cage during the whole observation period. Observation bouts in which a goose was resting or sleeping, thus of which the habitat preference of foraging geese was not possible to measure are excluded from the analysis.

Foraging time was noted. If a goose stopped foraging, time was stopped also. If a goose looked up, time was continuing. It is normal for

geese to look up while

foraging; it's part of the foragingbehaviour.

The geese were allowed to graze in the cage for 24 hours. After 24 h, all goose droppings were collected for each parcel separately as a measure of total grazing pressure and preference for a specific vegetationtype.

4.2.3. Vegetation parameters

Vegetation parameters were measured on both vegetation types (plots) that were used that day. The following parameters were determined before observation:

• Vegetation height;

• Tiller density;

• Biomass;

• Intake Rate.

Height of the vegetation was measured by using a sward stick. A polystyrene disc (24 g, 0 20 cm) is sliding down a PVC-stick with a cm-scale. Height was measured at 7 to 10 randomly selected locations in each plot with an accuracy of 0.25 cm.

(11)

The density of the tillers was measured by counting all the tillerson an area of 25 cm2.

In each plot, tiller density was measured six times at randomly chosen locations.

To determine biomass of the vegetation types, a sod, with an area of 100 cm2, was taken of each plot. The vegetation of the sod was clipped to ground level. Then the vegetation was sorted in living and dead grass. A separation between the two species,

Poa pratensis and Lolium perenne, wasn't necessary; the geese also did not

discriminate between the plants (Prins & Ydenberg 1985). The vegetation was dried for at least 48 hours in a stove at 70-80 °C and weighted afterwards.

Intake rate measurements

were done for

all

the three

different plots. These measurements were performed with two captive geese. The geese were placed in a wooden cage in which a wire net was separating the birds from each other. Grass sods of the different vegetation

types were given to the geese. Then a goose was allowed to take about 50 pecks from a sod. During the testing a camera was recording the sod and the goose. From this recording, the exact number of pecks, the foraging time (time the goose feeds on the sod, including looking up) and the peck-time (time the goose takes bites of the sod) could be determined. By weighing the sod before and after the measurement the amount of weight loss by goose grazing could be determined. A correction for amount of weightloss by evaporation was performed. By using the time the goose grazed, the number of pecks and the change of weight of the sod, the intake rate could be determined (Haar, R., van der 1999). A fellow student performed the intake rate measurements.

The digestibility of the grass in each vegetation type was measured. This was done in the periods the geese did not take part in the observation experiment. A cage was placed upon one of the vegetation types. After the geese had spent 24 hours in the cage, goose droppings and grass samples were collected. These samples were air-dried in a stove at 70-80 °C and an ADF-analysis (Acid Detergent Fibre) was performed (Stahl 1997).

The changes in nitrogen-content of the grass over time and the differences between sheep grazed and ungrazed areas were measured. Samples of the grass were collected once every two weeks at six randomly chosen spots in each of the different vegetation types. These samples were dried in a stove of 70-80 °C for 48 hours and an automated CNHS analysis was performed. The nitrogen-content can be used as a measurement for the protein-content ofgrass.

4.3. Statistical analysis

4.3.1. Data ecpelledfrom theanalysis

When a goose did not forage at all during the whole I 0-mm observation bout, this observation bout was expelled from the analysis.

All observation days in which a goose had spent less than 10 % of the observation period on foraging were expelled from the analysis. In all off these expelled cases a

(12)

reason for the behaviour of the geese can be given, mostly in the form of heavily disturbance of wild geese or nosy lambs.

Photograph 1: Little lambs playing bo-peep in the observation tent. In the back is the cage of the maintenance geese visible. The difference between grazed and ungrazed grass is clearly visible by the colour of the grass.

4.3.2. Statistics

Foraging preference of the geese is calculated by comparing the percentage of time spent on foraging on both sides of the cage. This calculated fraction of foraging time is called the foraging preference.

All foraging percentages have been arc sinus transformed before performing an analysis on these data. This has not been done with the data shown in the figures. All the analyses were performed with SPSS 9.0.

(13)

5. Results

5.1 Goose behaviour during the experiment

Both geese seem to spend equal time on foraging (see table 2). A Paired Samples T- test shows that there is no significant difference between the foraging time of the two individuals (t = -1.013, df= 20,p = 0.323).

Table 2: Percentage foraging (mean with std. error).

-)'I-0

C0

C.) Cu

4-

Cu

E 0)C C) U-0

Goose Percentage foraging (mean

± std.

JB()

32.77±4.91

JI (c3') 37.13 ± 5.19

Foraging time (fraction) of JB

Figure 2: Foraging time (fraction) of JB and JI of the observation period.

Because the two geese behave so similar in amount of foraging and preference for a treatment, the data of the geese will be combined.

A preference for a western or an eastern part of the cage independent of treatment is tested by comparing eastern preference within all cages with treatment SO vs. NO. In the data set 5 cages had the SG treatment on the eastern side and 5 on the western side. An ANOVA test shows that there is no preference for east or west (ANOVA; F1,9

= 0.035, p = 0.855).

The preference of the geese for the different treatments, significantly the same (Spearman's rho Correlation Test;

0.000, n = 21).

as shown in Figure 2, is

Con. Coeff =

0.791, p =

1.0

.8

.6

.4

.2

0.0

-.2

Treatment

Sheep grazing

O No grazing

o Goosegrazing Total Population

(14)

Weight of the geese was measured during the experiment (figure 3).

1800

1600

1400

.C 1200

CI 1000

800

600

14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22

Week number(1999)

Figure 3:Changes of weight of JB and JI duringthetesting period.

Weight of JI (male) has a significant negative relation with week number (ANOVA;

F1,11 = 19.183,

p =

0.002), while the weight of JB (female) a significant positive relation has with week number (ANOVA; F1,4= 50.54, p = .019).

5.2 Vegetation changes

5.2.1 Height, biomass and tiller density

Height and biomass show vegetation changes related to treatment and to week number (see figure 4 for height).

30—

20

EC)

0)

I

4) 10

e a

a a

a

treatment

no grazing

g H c sheep grazing

0,

________________________________________

0 goosegrazing

12 14 16 18 20 22

Week number (1999)

Figure4: Effect of week number and treatmenton height of the grass.

(15)

The graph of the effect of week number and treatment on biomass is almost the same as figure 4. The effect of treatment and week number and their interaction on height and biomass is significant (seetable 3 a-b).

Grass height and biomass of the no grazing (beginning of May) (see figure 4). There is with sheep and goose grazing.

Figure 5 shows the effect of treatment and week number on the tiller density of the grass swards. Tiller density is significantly related with week number and treatment (ANOVA; respectively F1,51 = 28.37, p = 0.000; F25, = 11.09, p = 0.000), however the interaction between week number and treatment approaches significance (F2,45 = 1.91,

pO.161).

CJ E

C)

to

a,0

a,

0

.04) E

z

Table 3a-b: Effect of treatment, week number and their interaction on height and biomass on SG, NG and GG grass swards.

Th

areas increase no increase in

rapidly after the 1 8th height or biomass in

week areas

80

0

60 H

00

A

50

I

40

I

H

a

A 0

20

A A

A b

10

14 16

treatment

A no grazing o sheep grazing o goose grazing

18 20 22

Week number (1999)

Figure5: The effect of treatment and week number on tiller density on different treatments.

There is a difference in tiller density for the different treatments. For the goose grazed areas there seems to be an increase in tiller density over the whole time period. For the

(16)

treatment sheep grazing and no grazing there is an increase followed by a decrease in tiller density.

This becomes clear when the treatments are analysed separately. For the treatment sheep grazing and no grazing there is a non-linear relation. The addition of a quadratic term to the regression is significant (see table 4). For treatment goose grazing the quadratic relation is significant, but because there are only a few data points the R(sq.) is low.

Table 4: ANOVA of tiller density against week number (and (week no)2) for different treatments.

Treatment Linear

/

Quadratic

F p

Goose grazing Linear Week no F1,7 = 35.32 = 0.001

Linear Week no F1,7= 35.32

p

= Quadratic £Week no)2 F1,6= 20.81

p 1

Sheep grazing Linear Week no F1,21 = 13.78 p== Quadratic Week no F1,21 = 13.78

p

= 0.00

jWeek no)2 F1,20 =27.61 p

No grazing Linear Week no F1,17= 2.87 =

p 0.000

= Quadratic Week no F1,17= 2.87

p 0.109

= 0.109

j'eek no)2

9.32 — p

5.2.2 Correlation between height and biomass

EC)

U)U) CD

C) a)

0

I

C)

Biomass (gr dryweight/I00 cm2)

Figure 6: Correlation between height and biomass with different treatments.

An ANOVA was performed to test the influence of treatment on the correlation of height and biomass. Treatment and the interaction of treatment is significant, biomass

Height and biomass of the grass (see rho; Correlation Coefficient = 0.928,

30

20

10

0

£ £

£

Figure 6) are significantly correlated (Spearman's

n=49, p=O.000).

Treatment

no grazing

O sheep grazing o goose grazing

£4 *

0 1 2

j

4

5 o

(17)

approaches significance (see table 5). An ANOVA performed without interaction results in significance for both treatment and biomass (ANOVA, respectively F49 =

7.75, p = 0.001;F1,49= 410.30, p = 0.000).

Table 5: Test results from ANOVA; height by treatment with biomass.

Height F

Biomass F1,49 = 2.98

p

=0.092 Treatment F2,49 = 3.47

p

= 0.040

Interaction F2,49 = 19.00

p

=0.000

The three treatments have different regression lines. Table 6 shows the coefficients of these lines based on the test of table 5.

Table 6: Different lines for the three treatments: Hei2ht = Constant+B *Biomass.

L

Constant ± std. error)

B std.

LNo grazing 1.467±0.433

±

error) 0.343±0.120 (Sheep grazing 1.115 ± 1.146 -4.112± 1.378 LGoose grazing 0.366 ± 0.369 -2.497 ± 0.400

1

5.2.3 Nitrogen content and digestibility of the grass

Figure 7 shows the differences of nitrogen content of sheep grazed areas over time.

Differences in nitrogen content between SG and NG are calculated and NG is set to zero. The figure shows that nitrogen content of sheep grazed areas significantly increases with week number (ANOVA;F1,39 = 14.29, p =0.001).

0.4

0.3

C C0

o 0

C 0.2

0.1

-0.2

-03 -0.4

-05

Figure 7: Differences in nitrogen content (%) over time for sheep grazed (y =0.140x 0.387).

There is not a significant effect of treatment on nitrogen (ANOVA; F1,78 = 0.582, p =

0.448). However the treatments sheep grazing and no grazing do not significantly differ in nitrogen content, there is a change over time. In week 15 and 16 nitrogen content of SG is lower than the nitrogen content of grass of NG, this changes in week 17, from here the nitrogen content of SG is higher than that of NG.

Week number (1999)

(18)

Analysis on the ADF-data was performed. No difference in ADF percentage or in digestibility between the two treatments NG and SG was found (see table 7). There appeared to be no time or treatment effect.

Table 7: test results from a paired samnie t-test.

Paired Samples T-test -

ADFdroppings (%) NG vs. SG Correlation F Value

7

ADF grass (%) NGvs. SG

p_

Correlation

0.649

n

-0.425

Digestibility (%) NG vs. SG Correlation

0.400 -0.608

n 4

C-) U)

0)

U)

U) Cu

5.2.4 Intake Rate

When all data of the three treatments are combined and plotted against height of the grass (see figure 8) there is a significant quadratic relationship (Non linear regression;

R (sq.) = 0.112, df= 74, F 4.65, p = 0.013).

.08— 0

0 __

.06 0 0

ci 0 0

o 0

o 0

ci

B 0

ci:

ci 0

00

8oo

0

o 0 0

0

D o

.02 0 0 98

0 0 0

0 0

0.00, 0

00

0 2 4 6 8 10 12 14

Heigth of the grass (cm) Figure8: Intake rate of alltreatments plotted against height.

Intake rate increases till the grass reaches a height of Ca. 4-5 cm. from here intake rate decreases again. There seems to be an optimum in the graph, which is at ca. 4 cm. At this height of the grass the intake rate is maximal.

(19)

Figure 9a-c shows where the geese forage during the observation period for all three combinations (GO vs. NG, SG vs. NG and SG vs. GO).

Weeknumber (1999)

Figure 9a: Foraging time (fraction) during the observation period of goose grazing and no grazing.

Week number (1999)

(fraction) during the observation period of no grazing and sheep 5.3. Preference of the goose

5.3.1. Foraging preference from observation

1.0

n= 1

.8

n= 1

.6 C0

C.)w

.4—

E 0)C 0) U-0

.4

.2

0.0

treatment

goose grazing

[]no grazing

1.0

C0

C-)

.4-

0)C 0)

Cu

0

LL

0.0

Figure 9b: Foraging time grazing.

[Jt',b grazing

:sep grazing

(20)

Treatment

Goose grazing

[]sieep grazing

Figure 9c: Foraging time (fraction) during the observation period of goose grazing and sheep grazing.

Preference of the geese is tested with a T-test. If the geese have preference for a treatment, it is expected that they will spend more then 50 % of the time on that treatment. The data (of no grazing for 9a, of sheep grazing for 9b and 9c) are therefor tested against 0.5. The results of the 1-test are shown in table 8. The goose has a significant preference of sheep grazed areas over the no grazing and goose grazing areas; and a preference of areas with no grazing over areas with goose grazing.

Table 8:

1.0

.8

.6

0

U

0)C 0)

Co

0

LL .4

.2

0.0

Week number (1 999)

5.3.3. Combination of dropping counts and observation

To find out whether preference measured by dropping counts correlates with the preference measured by observation, these preferences are plotted against each other (figure 10). Dropping counts were done after the observation was performed (ca. 3 hours). Only one, random chosen, data point per cage, which means that only one of the two treatments, is taken in the figure and analysis.

(21)

a

a

0

j .20

a

!

0.01

o.o

.

1.0

Foraging preference (fraction) by observation

Figure 10: Foraging preference measured by observation and 3 h dropping counts plotted (n =

15).

There is a significant correlation between foraging preference by observation and by 3h dropping counts (see table 9). The results from the dropping counts support the preference results from the observation.

Table 9: Test results from the Spearman's rho test for foraging preference by observation, 3h drop ing counts and 24h dropping counts.

Foraging preference 3h dropping count

Observation Correlation Coefficient p (2-tailed)

n

0.768 0.001 15

24h dropping count Correlation Coefficient p (2-tailed)

N

0.850 0.004 9

While the geese stay in the cage for 24 hours, the grass of the preferred foraging area can be depleted or trampled by the geese. This effect can change the preference of the geese over time. To test whether there was an effect of depletion or trampling the 3h and 24h dropping counts are plotted (figure 11).

There is a significant relation between these dropping counts (see table 9); therefor there is no such effect.

(22)

U)

0

C.)

0)C

00

0

•0 -c

c'J

>%

.0

C0

C-) Cu

q)0

C0) L.

0)

a.

0)C 0)

Cu

U-0

1.0

A B

aA

0A A

.8 A A A

A

.6 A

A

0

.4

00

0

.2 000 0

0c:D° 00

0 0

0.0 -

0 1 2 j 4 7

1.0

.8

.6

.4

0

ci ci

0

.2

0.0 0.0

0

.2 .4 .6 .8

Foraging preference (fraction) by 3h dropping counts Figure11: Foragingpreference by 3 and 24 h dropping counts (n= 9).

1.0

5.4. Foraging preference and vegetation

The preference of the geese for a treatment might be explained by one of the

vegetation parameters. Differences in for example height or biomass of the grass of the different treatments can influence the choice the geese made.

Foraging preference, from observation as well as 3h dropping count, of the geese is plotted against vegetation height and biomass (see figure 12a-d).

1.0

.8

.6 (0

0

C0(

0(

8

C

00

0.0

Height of the grass (cm)

Treatment

A Shupgrazing

0 Nograzing

0 Goos. grazing

Biomass (gr dryeight/ 100 cm2)

(23)

Figure 12a-d: Foraging preference (by observation and 3h dropping counts) plotted against vegetation parameters (heightand biomass).

The preference of the geese for biomass (figure 12b & 12d) is not so clear; they constantly select for SG in the SG vs. UG combination, whether the biomass is higher or not. This suggests that there is another parameter, then biomass, for which the geese show preference.

Height might be this

parameter (figure 12a & 12c); here the geese do select more on a specific height of the grass, ± 4,0 cm. The mean height of the data with foraging preference> 50 % is 3,9877± 0,097 (mean ± std. error).

U,Q Ca

2a

cv)

I

Height of the grass (cm)

Biomass (gr dry\ightJ1oo cm2)

Treatment

A hpgr.,g

'bgr.zfrg

0 Goose grazing

(24)

6. Discussion

In this study the hypothesis is tested that Brent geese prefer foraging on grazed vegetation.

6.1. Preference of the geese for the combination SGvs. NG 6.1.1. Height and intake rate

The geese prefer clearly the sheep grazed areas to the no grazing areas (figure 9b).

This preference is best related with the preference of the geese for a certain grass height (figure 12a). This figure shows that the geese prefer a grass height of ca. 4 cm.

A study of Summers & Atkins (1991)on Brent geese and Aster tripolium found similar results. Summers & Atkins showed that Brent geese when offered leaves with a length in a range of 4-180 mm preferred those leaves with a leaf length in the range of 40-5 9 mm. They state that the geese are selecting leaves of the mid-range to maximise the rate of intake. Almost the same result is found in this study. As shown infigure 8 grass height is related with intake rate. The figure shows an increase in intake rate when grass becomes taller but intake rate decreases again when the grass is too high. The graph has an optimum at a grass height of ca. 4-5 cm. This suggests that the geese can maximise their intake rate when they forage on grass with a height of ca. 4-5 cm.

This result fits in the theory of van der Wal et a!. (1998). They state that, in contrast with the conventional theory which states food intake saturates at high standing crop (Holling 1965), herbivore intake decreases with high standing crop. This theory is the alternative hypothesis in figure 13 (van der Wal et a!. 1998).

Plant standing crop

Figure 13: Theoretical relationships between herbivore consumption and plant standing crop, as predicted by conventional theory and the alternative hypothesis (Van der Wal eta!. 1998).

They found a trend that suggests that daily food intake is lower at high standing crop.

This implies that herbivores do not select for areas with the highest standing crop but for a standing crop on intermediate level.

The same result was found in this study. Figure 8 shows great resemblance to the alternative hypothesis in figure 13. In figure 8 height can be seen as a parameter for standing crop and intake rate for herbivore consumption.

Herbivore Consumption

(25)

As shown in figure 12a-d height and biomass both relate to the foraging preference of the geese. However height seems to relate better with preference than biomass. The intake rate of a goose seems to be more affected by height of the vegetation instead of biomass.

When one looks through the eyes of a goose it might become clear. A goose, while looking from almost straight above, isonly able to forage on the top layer of the grass.

Most of the biomass, all short and new sprouts, is covered by the longest leaves of the grass sward and therefore not attainable for the goose. Therefore height and not biomass may be the direct parameter relevant for the goose.

6.1.2. Nitrogen content and digestibility

No significant difference in nitrogen content of the grass between the two treatments is found. However, there seems to be a slight difference in nitrogen content between SG and NG; from the 17th week nitrogen content is in the SG areas higher than in the NG areas.

Ydenberg and Prins (1981) did find differences in nitrogen content in early spring.

They found that in February / March nitrogen content of grazed areas is significantly higher than in ungrazed areas. The difference in nitrogen content between these treatments continued until the second half

of April. At the end of April / in

the

beginning of May the nitrogen content of the grazed areas decreased immenselytill the nitrogen level of the ungra.zedareas.

The reason that I did not find any differences could be a seasonal effect. The results from Ydenberg and Prins suggest that the nitrogen difference between the treatments last till half of April. Grass samples for this study have been collected in the period from 12 of April till the end of May. Thus, at that time period the differencein nitrogen content is not existing anymore.

Boudewijn (1981) found that digestibility of polder grass is increasing during early spring but is decreasing again in May. He suggested that this could be a reason why the geese leave the polder area and shift to the salt marshes.

However, there was no difference in digestibility found between the treatments (table 7). A difference over time was also not found.

6.1.3 Optimisation of the nutrient intake

The geese are expected to prefer sheep grazed vegetation because of the short, dense and young vegetation with high nitrogen content (Summers & Critchley 1990, Vickery

& Sutherland 1992). A study of Therkildsen & Madsen (1999) shows that geese select for high-quality food, but that there is a trade-off between quality and quantity when it comes to intake rates. Therefore the geese are expected to select those foraging areas

where they can optimise their nutrient intake rate (Prop & Derenburg

1991,

Riddington & Hassall 1997, Bos et a!. in prep).

Figure 14 shows the nutrient intake, the product of intake rate with nitrogen content, for the treatments SG and NG. A goose will prefer grazing on those areas with the optimal mix of nutritional value and intake rate so they can optimise his nutrient intake.

(26)

.3

02

(0 a, (U

C C

z .1

0.0 0.0

Nutrient intake (NO)

Figure14: Nutrient intake for no grazing and sheep grazing plotted against each other (n = 17).

The y = x line is a visual division for nutrient intake for the two treatments. If a dot is above the line, it means that a goose can get the highest nutrient intake at the SG pasture and vice versa. All data points in figure 14 are lying above the y = x line. This implies that a goose can get the highest nutrient intake in the sheep grazed areas. This explains why the geese show preference for these areas. In the sheep grazed areas a goose can optimise his nutrient intake.

6.2. Preference of the geese for the combination SGvs. GG 6.2.1 Nutrient intake

The preference of the geese in the combination SG vs. UG can be explained with nutrient intake. It probably also explains the preference in thecombination SG vs. GG, however no measurements of the nitrogen content of the grass of the goose grazed areas are performed, so nutrient intake can not be calculated for this combination.

Studies have shown that sheep grazing increases nitrogen content of the grass (Gordon

& Lindsay 1990, Vickery et al. 1994). A study of Beaulieu et a!. 1996 shows the effect of goose grazing on nitrogen content. Nitrogen concentration was highest in heavily grazed plants, intermediate in lightly grazed plants and lowest in ungrazed plants.

Goose grazing also leads to a large reduction in the grass standing crop (Percival &

Houston 1992).

Differences between these two treatments in nitrogen content or digestibility are not described in any study. I expect, also because of the seasonal effect mentioned in 6.1.2, no difference in nitrogen content between the treatments. Both geese and sheep graze the vegetation short with an increase in tillers (figure 4 & 5), which enhances nitrogen content of the grass (Gordon & Lindsay 1990, Vickery el a!. 1994).

.1 .2 .3

(27)

6.2.2. Height and intake rate

The geese show a clear preference for the sheep grazed areas (figure 9c). The grass height is slightly lower in the goose grazed areas than in the sheep grazed areas, but the difference is not significant. Figure 8 shows that intake rate at 4 cm (intake rate(4cm) 0.06 g/sec) is higher than the intake rate at 3 cm (intake rate(3cm) 0.045 g/sec). The geese prefer grazing on the sheep grazed areas to the goose grazed areas, cause there they can optimise their intake rate.

There might be some other factors that could influence preference of the geese. The geese who were held for the maintenance of the goose grazed areas were grazing on the plots until three days before testing. These maintenance geese were held 4 days on and 4 days off the plot. In the natural situation a flock of geese walk through a grass pasture, deplete the area and walk on to the next area. Foraging geese can describe a cyclic pattern in the exploitation ofthe feeding grounds, revisiting an area to forage on the regrowth of the plants (Prins el a!. 1980). In this experiment the geese were forced to graze for a relatively long period

on this particular plot. Because of this the

vegetation of the plot can have been in an unattractive (depletion, trampling) stage for the testing geese, even although the grass had three days recovery time.

6.3. A comparison with Ameland

On the island of Ameland the geese do not leave the polder in early spring; number of geese in the polder remains the same until migration starts (van der Graaf 1998, Arends 1998). On Schiermonnikoog the geese make a habitat shift from the polder to the salt marsh in the beginning of April (Prins & Ydenberg 1985, McKay ci a!. 1994, Vickery el a!. 1995). The major difference between the two islands that might cause the difference in goose behaviour is the management of the polder. The polder of Ameland is sheep grazed, while the polder of Schiermonnikoog is cattle grazed, which means for the time the geese use the polder that the grass is ungrazed.

This study has tried to bring a piece of Ameland to Schiermonnikoog to see what happens by sheep grazing ofa polder meadow on Schiermonnikoog.

On both the islands thegeese prefer foraging on areas with a short grass sward with an average height of ca. 3.5 cm on Ameland and 4 cm on Schiermonnikoog. It seems that there is on Ameland as well as on Schiermonnikoog a decrease in grazing pressure (foraging pressure; figure 12 a-d) when the grass is too short, but not enough data of the polder of Ameland are available. Both on Ameland and on Schiermonnikoog the trend of a foraging optimum related to grass height is found; both data sets endorse this.

(28)

6.4. Geesegrazing without facilitation of sheep

This study shows that geese prefer foraging on sheep grazed areas when offered a choice between sheep grazed and ungrazed. The sheep facilitate the geese by keeping the vegetation in a preferable stage. Can goose grazing facilitate other geese later in the season?

A few studies show that goose grazing can indeed facilitate goose grazing later in the season. A study of Drent & van der Wa! (1999) showsthat Brent geese grazing on Plantago visit Plantago patches once every 4-7 days. A recent study of Rowcliffe et a!. (1995) shows evidence for cyclic grazing in Brent geese of Puccineiiamaritima

swards on the salt marshes. The geese seem to be able to maintain the grazing lawn in a highly productive and attractive stadium. On the polder of Ameland the geese have some favourite grazing areas, which they keep short and return to these areas regularly (van der Graaf 1998).

So, geese can maintaina area if they start grazing early in the season when the grass is still short. Can the geese also create a 'favourite' site and maintain it late in the season when the grass has alreadygrown high?

When the grass is high intake rate is low (figure 8). Because of the low intake rate a large time will be spent in decreasing the grass height. The low intake rate can also have an effect on the health of the geese. On ungrazed parcels the geese have to forage on food with a low quality and a high digestibility.

During the testing period the geese were weighted (figure 3). The weight of the male goose decreases rapidly in week 19-20 (pers. observation). In week 19 the geese were, because of the digestibility measurements, feeding on NG areas 24 hours a day for 4 days in a row. The weight loss of the male suggests that the geese are not well adapted to forage on high grass, which was at that time of a height of ca. 15-cm. However, there is no such trend visible for the female.

This sudden decrease in weight of the male goose can be an indication that grass height can be a limiting factor in creating a new 'favourite' grazing area.

6.5. Comparing polder feeding with salt marsh feeding

The geese wintering on Schiermonnikoog have to accumulate body reserves for the migration to their breeding grounds. Most of the body reserve is gained in the last seven weeks before migration (Ydenberg & Prins 1981). In these last seven weeks most of the geese on Schiermonnikoog forage on the salt marsh. A study of Prop &

Deerenberg (1991) is used tocompare data.

Are the geese able to gain enough body weight when feeding on the polder? Not all

data are available therefore only speculations can be made.

Prop & Deerenberg (1991) found that the digestivecapacity of the Brent goose is 1.67 fresh / mm. The mean intake rate of the sheep grazed areas as shown in figure 8, is ca

(29)

0.06 g fresh weight / sec or 3.6 g fresh / mm. Intake rate of sheep grazed grass is too high and digestive capacity will be a limiting factor. A goose has to compensate for this. The testing birds, while foraging on the polder grasses, reduced their foraging time per bout. They were only foraging for about 30 % ofthe all observation time.

Free living geese foraging on the polder decrease their foraging time on the polder during the season. In March they forageCa. 80 % of the time and in May only 45 % of the time (Veeneklaas, R.M. in prep.). The foraging percentage of free living geese foraging on the saltmarsh remains equal over time, Ca. 80 % of the time. By reducing the foraging time and increasing the resting time, a high intake rate on polder grasses can be sustained. Therefore it is expected that geese feeding on the polder will spend more time on resting then geese feeding on the saltmarsh. A study of Veeneklaas (in prep.) states this expectation.

(30)

7. Conclusions

Height and not treatment seems to be the major determinant a goose selects on.

Nevertheless, there is a correlation between sward height and treatment because treatment is a way of achieving a certain grass height. Goose grazing results in the lowest, sheep grazing in an intermediate and no grazing in the highest vegetation

height.

On the polder of Schiermonnikoog geese prefer grazing on sheep grazed areas to the ungrazed areas. The sheep grazed areas have a higher tiller density, a lower sward height and biomass and a higher intake rate. Because of the higher intake rate, the geese can obtain a higher in nutrient intake.

The geese prefer grazing on the sheep grazed areas to the goose grazed areas. Goose grazed areas have a lower height, tiller density and intake rate, the grass is too short to forage on. Other factors influencing the preference of the geese could be the trampling and/or the depletion caused by the maintenance geese.

(31)

8. Appendices

Appendix 1: Schiermonnikoog

Appendix 1: The island of Schiermonnikoog with poWer and saltmarsh. Inlay: The polder of Schiermonnikoog with the 4 favourite spots of the Brent geese for the last few years (3 dots and the star) and the place of the experiment (star).

(32)

9. Acknowledgements

This study would not have been possible without the help of many people. Iwould like to thank my supervisor Maarten Loonen who has been very patient and helped me writing this paper. Daan Bos and Julia Stahl who gave me advise about the practical work. Daan Bos, Roos Veeneklaas and the students of the Ethologie Research and Dierecologie course for helping with the fieldwork.

I am grateful to Mr. Visser who allowed me to do experiments on his land and Mr.

Talsma who showed great confidence by letting me take care of his sheep. I am grateful to Otto Overdijk and all other people of Natuurmonumenten, especially the man who lent me his lawn mower.

I am grateful to Willem van Hall and Bert Venema, who performed nitrogen analysis and helped with the ADF-analysis.

(33)

10. References

Abaye. A.O., Allen, V.G. & Fontenot, J.P. (1997) Grazing sheep and cattle together or separately: Effect on soil and plants. AgronomyJournal 89 (3), 380-386.

Arends, N.H. (1998) Habitat use of Brent geese on Ameland: habitat switching from the polder to the saltmarsh. Doctoraalverslag, R.U.G

Baker, R.R. (1978) The evolutionary ecology of animal migration. Hodder and Stoughton, London.

Beaulieu, J., Gauthier, G. & Rochefort, L. (1996) The growth response of graminoid plants to goose grazing in a High Arctic environment. Journal of Ecology 84 (6), 905- 914.

Belovsky, G.E. & Jordan, P.A. (1981) The Time-Energy Budget of a Moose. Theor.

Pop. Biology 14, 76-104.

Bos, D., Loonen, M.J.J.E. & Stahl, J. (in prep) On the relative importance of food quantity and quality in patch choice by Brent geese.

Boudewijn, T. (1984) The role of digestibility in the selection of spring feeding sites by Brent geese. Wildfowl 35, 97-105.

Coppock, D.L., Ellis,

J.E., Detling, J.K. & Dyer, MI.

(1983) Plant-Herbivore Interactions in a North American Mixed-Grass Prairie. II. Responses of Bison to Modification of Vegetation by PrairieDogs. Oecologia 56, 10-15.

Crawley, M.J. (1983) Herbivory: the dynamics of animal-plant interactions. Blackwell Scientific Publications, Oxford.

Drent, R. & Wal, R. van der (1999) Cyclic grazing in vertebrates and the manipulation of the food resource. In: 01ff, H., Brown, V.K. & Drent, RH. (1999) Herbivores:

interactions between plants and predators. pp, 271-296. Blackwell Science, Oxford.

Ebbinge, B. 5. (1992) Regulation of numbers of dark-bellied Brent Geese Bran/a Bernicla bernicla on spring staging sites. Ardea 80, 203-228.

Graaf,

S. van der (1998) Do sheep facilitate

goose grazing? Impact of grazing management on the distribution

of Brent geese Branta

bernicla bernicla.

Doctoraalverslag, R.U. G.

Graaf, S. van der (in prep.) Impact of grazing management on the distribution of Brent geese Bran/a bernicla bern/cia.

Grant, S.A., Bartham, G.T. & Torvell, L. (1981) Components of regrowth in grazed and cut Lolium perenne swards. Grass and Forage Science 36, 155-168.

(34)

Gordon, I.J. (1988) Facilitation of red deer grazing by cattle and its impact on red deer performance. J. of App!. Ecology 25, 1-10.

Gordon, I.J. & Lindsay, W.K. (1990) Could mammalian herbivores "manage" their resources? Oikos 59, 270-280.

Haar, R. van der (1999) Opnamesnelheid van de rotgans (Branta berniclabernicla) op verschillende vegetatietypen

en de voorkeur Voor

een bepaald vegetatietype.

Stageversiag, Van Hall Instituut, Leeuwarden.

Hassall, M. & Riddington, R. (in prep.) Foraging behaviour and site selection by Brent geese Branta b bernicla.

Hobbs, M.T., Baker, D.L., Bear, G.D. & Bowden, D.C. (1996) Ungulate grazing in sagebrush grassland: mechanisms of resource competition. Ecological Applications 6 (1), 200-2 17.

Holmes, W. (1980) Grass,

its production and utilisation. Blackwell Scientific Publications, Oxford.

Holling, C.S. (1965) The functional response of predators to prey density and its role in mimicry and population regulation. Mem. Entomol. Soc. Can. 45, 1-60.

Jones, M.B, Collet, B. & Brown, 5.

(1982) SWard growth under cutting and continuous stocking management: sward structure, tiller density and leaf turnover.

Grass and Forage Science 37, 67-73.

Kralovec, J., Frycek, A., Rais, I. & Frycek, N. (1992) Retrospective analysis and

present view on nitrate content in herbage of

grasslands. Scientia Agriculturae Bohemoslovaca 23 (4), 283-291.

McKay, H.V., Bishop, J.D. & Ennis,

D.C. (1994) The possible importance of nutritional requirements for Dark-bellied Brent Geese in the seasonal shift from winter cereals to pasture. Ardea 82 (1), 123-132.

McNaughton, S.J. (1976) Serengeti Migratory Wildebeest: Facilitation of Energy Flow by Grazing. Science 191, 92-94.

Middleton, B.A. (1992) Habitat and food preferences of greylag and barheaded geese wintering in the Keoladeo National Park, India. Journal of Tropical Ecology 8 (2),

18 1-193.

Murphy, W.M., Barreto, A.D., Silman, J.P. & Dindal, D.L. (1995) Cattle and sheep grazing effects on soil organisms, fertility and Compaction in a smooth-stalked meadowgrass-dominant white clover sward. Grass and Forage Science 50 (3), 191-

194.

Owen, M., Nugent, M. & Davies, N. (1977) Discrimination between grass species and nitrogen-fertilised vegetation by young barnacle geese. Wildfowl 28, 21-26.

(35)

Parsons, A.J. & Penning, P.D. (1988) The effect of the duration of regrowth on photosynthesis, leaf death and the average rate of growth in a rotationally grazed sward. Grass and Forage Science43, 15-27.

Percival, SM. & Houston, D.C. (1992) The effect of winter grazing by barnaclegeese on grasslands yields on Islay. J. of Appi. Ecology 29 (1), 35-40.

Prins, H.H.Th. & Ydenberg, R.C. (1985) Vegetation growth and seasonal habitat shift of the Barnacle Goose. Oecologia 66, 122-125.

Prop, J. & Deerenberg, C. (1991) Spring staging in Brent Geese Branta bernicla bernicla: Feeding constraints and the impact of diet on the accumulation of body reserves. Oecologia 87(1), 19-28.

Riddington, R., Hassall, M. & Lane, S.J. (1997) The selection of grass swards by Brent Geese Branta b. bernicla : Interactions between food quality and quantity.

Biological Conservation 81, 153-160.

Rowcliffe, J.M., Watkinson, AR., Sutherland, W.J. & Vickery, J.A. (1995) Cyclic winter grazing

patterns in Brent Geese and the

regrowth of salt-marsh grass.

Functional Ecology 9(6), 931-941.

Smith, R.S. & Rushton, S.P. (1994) The effects of grazing management on the vegetation of mesotrophic (meadow) grassland in

Northern England. J. of

AppI.

Ecology 31(1), 13-24.

Stahl, J. (1997) Digestibility studies in geese and other herbivores A laboratory manual. R.U.G.

Summers, R.W. & Critchley, C.N.R. (1990) Use of grassland and field selection by Brent geese Branta b. bernicla. J. of AppI. Ecology 27, 834-846.

Summers, R.W. & Atkins, C. (1991) selection by Brent Geese Branta bernicla for different leaf lengths of Aster tripolium on saltmarsh. Wildfowl42, 33-36.

Therkildsen, OR. & Madsen, J. (1999) Goose grazing selectivity along a depletion gradient. Ecography 22, 516-520.

Veeneklaas, R.M. (in prep.) Heartrate as an indication for daily energy expenditure of Brent and Barnacle Geese. Doctoraal onderzoek, RUG.

Vickery, J.A., Sutherland, W.J. & Lane, S.J. (1994) The management of grass pastures for Brent geese. J. of Appi. Ecology31, 282-290.

Vickery, J.A., Watkinson, A.R. & Sutherland, W.J. (1994) The solutions to the Brent goose problem: an economic analysis. J. of AppI. Ecology 31, 371-382.

(36)

Vickery, J.A., Sutherland, W.J., Watkinson, A.R., Lane, S.J. & Rowcliffe, J.M. (1995) Habitat switching by dark-bellied Brent geese Branta b. bernicla (Z.) in relation to food depletion. Oecologia 103, 499-508.

Wal, R. van der (1998) Defending the marsh: Herbivores in a Dynamic Coastal Ecosystem. Proefschrifi, R.U. 0., Groningen.

Wal, R. van der, Koppel, J. van de & Sagel, M. (1998) On the relation between herbivore foraging efficiency and plant standing crop: an experiment with barnacle geese. Oikos 82, 123-130.

Ydenberg R.C. & Prins H.H.Th. (1981) Spring grazing and manipulation of food quality by Barnaclegeese. J. of Appi. Ecology 18, 443-453.

Referenties

GERELATEERDE DOCUMENTEN

For the analysis of the dynamic effect of the growing tree line along the railway in Agri-N and Agri-S, we used the combined wader densities/100 ha per 200 m strips for each quarter

Het is een grijze en kille donderdagmiddag in februari wanneer Jan Polak, Bertie Barendregt, Niekus Mons, Rene Kouters en Bert Morssink een fietstocht maken door de

Met een voornamelijk agrarisch gebruik heeft de polder een open karakter, hoewel een aantal kassen de openheid beperken.. op het open gebied is in het verleden al

Deze heeft de financiële bijdrage van de slavenhandel aan de industriële revolutie over- schat en de betekenis van de Westindische slavernij voor de Engelse economie sterk

Door het risico op resistentie tegen curatieve middelen en het saneren van middelen is het vinden van nieuwe toepassingsstrategieën en nieuwe middelen noodzakelijk.. Ascosporen

We can conclude that if all the pumps in the Pluit polder are working on their full capacity, the pump- and storage capacity meet the design criteria to handle a rainfall event with

In het LOP is een aantal plekken aangewezen waar ontwikkelingen mogelijk zouden kunnen zijn. Aan de nieuwe ontwikkelingen zijn strenge voorwaarden verbonden. Zo zijn er enkele

Copyright and moral rights for the publications made accessible in the public portal are retained by the authors and/or other copyright owners and it is a condition of