• No results found

Happy Running?: Using an Accelerometer to Predict the Affective State of a Runner

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Share "Happy Running?: Using an Accelerometer to Predict the Affective State of a Runner"

Copied!
5
0
0

Bezig met laden.... (Bekijk nu de volledige tekst)

Hele tekst

(1)

Amsterdam University of Applied Sciences

Happy Running?

Using an Accelerometer to Predict the Affective State of a Runner van der Bie, Joey; Kröse, Ben

DOI

10.1007/978-3-319-26005-1_26 Publication date

2015

Document Version

Author accepted manuscript (AAM) Published in

Ambient Intelligence

Link to publication

Citation for published version (APA):

van der Bie, J., & Kröse, B. (2015). Happy Running? Using an Accelerometer to Predict the Affective State of a Runner. In B. De Ruyter, A. Kameas, P. Chatzimisios, & I. Mavrommati (Eds.), Ambient Intelligence: 12th European Conference, AmI 2015 Athens, Greece,

November 11–13, 2015 Proceedings (pp. 357-360). (Lecture Notes in Computer Science; Vol.

9425). Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-26005-1_26

General rights

It is not permitted to download or to forward/distribute the text or part of it without the consent of the author(s) and/or copyright holder(s), other than for strictly personal, individual use, unless the work is under an open content license (like Creative Commons).

Disclaimer/Complaints regulations

If you believe that digital publication of certain material infringes any of your rights or (privacy) interests, please let the Library know, stating your reasons. In case of a legitimate complaint, the Library will make the material inaccessible and/or remove it from the website. Please contact the library:

https://www.amsterdamuas.com/library/contact/questions, or send a letter to: University Library (Library of the University of Amsterdam and Amsterdam University of Applied Sciences), Secretariat, Singel 425, 1012 WP Amsterdam, The Netherlands. You will be contacted as soon as possible.

Download date:27 Nov 2021

(2)

Happy Running?

Using an accelerometer to predict the affective state of a runner

Joey van der Bie, Ben Kröse Amsterdam University of Applied Sciences

Wibautstraat 2-4, 1091GM, Amsterdam j.h.f.van.der.bie@hva.nl, b.j.a.krose@hva.nl

Abstract. This paper explores a method for deducing the affective state of run- ners using his/her movements. The movements are measured on the arm using a smartphone’s built-in accelerometer. Multiple features are derived from the measured data. We studied which features are most predictive for the affective state by looking at the correlations between the features and the reported affect.

We found that changes in runners’ movement can be used to predict change in affective state.

Keywords: affect • emotion • accelerometer • smartphone • physical activity

1 Introduction

Knowing how an athlete feels can help a sports coach in communication and set- ting up a more tailored training schedule. Also a continuous measurement of the af- fective state would be advantageous for a automatic digital coaching system (coach- ing-app). Affective experiences in activities are usually measured using video record- ings [1]. This is unpractical for everyday use. It would be more practical to use a smartphone worn by the athlete, making it possible for a coaching-app to use its affec- tive state. We are developing a system that infers the affective state of a runner from the movement characteristics measured by the accelerometer sensor of a smartphone mounted on the arm. In this paper we present our work on the analysis of the accel- erometer data: which movement features from the data correlate best with the per- ceived affective state?

2 Related work

Recent studies have shown that spatio-temporal body features are indicative for the affective state of a person [1].

In the field of running intensity has an influence on the affective state. Continuous running above the ventilatory threshold (the point during exercise at which respiration starts to increase at a faster rate) changes the affective experience negatively. Running below the ventilatory threshold has little influence on the affective state. [2]

(3)

Accelerometers can be used at various places on the body to determine type of ac- tivity. The raw signal can be transformed into features and used as an indicator for an activity. [3]

3 Experimental Set-up and Data

We created a dataset consisting of accelerometer data from a smartphone worn by the runners and affective state reported by the runners. Test subjects ran on a treadmill on their ventilatory threshold until they were tired. The threshold was determined for each runner separately in a previous session based on the method of Bood et al. [4].

Running speed, heart rate, movement, ability to talk, perceived exertion and perceived affective state are measured. The data collection was part of a larger experiment. This study uses the accelerometer data and perceived affective state.

Movement was measured using a smartphone’s (Google Nexus 5) three degrees of freedom accelerometer (with a sample rate of 100Hz), worn by the runner in a band on the left arm. The phone’s X-axis measured forward movement, the Y-axis meas- ured vertical movement and the Z-axis measured sideways movement. Perceived affective state was measured with the 11-point Feeling scale [5] by verbal expression of the subject once every two minutes while running. The questionnaire was displayed on a screen in front of the user.

Eighteen runners participated, four male, fourteen female, with an average age of 23 years (STD 3 years). They ran an average of 16,6 minutes (STD 5 minutes) of which an average of 8 minutes at a constant speed. In total 96 affective responses were given while running at constant speed.

4 Feature extraction

We were interested in features related to the intensity of the movement and to the regularity of the movement. Corresponding to each affective response a data sample of 12000 data points (two minutes of data) was created from the movement signal.

The data was transformed with a Hanning-window. For each sample the mean of the signal, the variance of the signal and the entropy of the Fourier transform were deter- mined. We used the individual X-, Y- and Z-axes of the accelerometer as signals. In total we had nine features (three axes times three features) for every time sample.

5 Results

Figure 1a shows the reported affect for the participants as a function of time during the eight minutes of constant speed. All participants show the expected tendency to- wards negative affect as described by [2]. However, we also observe inter-person differences: some participants report a structural higher affect than others.

(4)

a b

Fig. 1. a. Affective responses as a function of time.

b. Normalized affective responses as a function of time.

In our first approach we determine correlations between the reported affect and the nine accelerometer features. We determined the p values using an independent sample size of 18 (the total number of participants). Overall the correlation values are low.

The highest correlation value can be found in the mean of the X-axis (-0,20) with a p value of 0,42. The correlation values are displayed in table 1. One reason for the low correlation can be the inter-person differences, both in affect as in feature values.

Feature r p Feature r p Feature r p

Mean X -0,2 0,42 Mean Y 0,05 0,85 Mean Z -0,07 0,79

Variance X -0,17 0,51 Variance Y -0,005 0,98 Variance Z 0,01 0,97

Entropy X 0,09 0,72 Entropy Y -0,01 0,98 Entropy Z -0,19 0,46

Table 1. Correlation of features with affect. r = correlation value. p = significance

Feature r p Feature r p Feature r p

Mean X -0,07 0,78 Mean Y 0,27 0,28 Mean Z 0,25 0,32

Variance X -0,52 0,03 Variance Y -0,38 0,12 Variance Z -0,74 4,03×10-4

Entropy X -0,11 0,66 Entropy Y -0,03 0,89 Entropy Z -0,03 0,91

Table 2. Correlation of normalized features with normalized affect. r = correlation value. p = significance

In order to filter out individual characteristics, the feature values and affect values are standardized using Z-score normalization for each individual participant. Normal- ized affective responses are depicted in figure 1b. We determine the most likely in- dicative features by calculating the correlation between each feature and the affective

-5 -4 -3 -2 -1 0 1 2 3 4 5

0 0,25 0,5 0,75 1

Affect

Time (normalized p.p.) Affect over time while running at constant speed

Trendline

-3 -2 -1 0 1 2 3

0 0,25 0,5 0,75 1

Affect (normalized p.p.)

Time (normalized p.p.) Normalized affect over time while running at

constant speed

Trendline

(5)

response. The highest correlation value can be found in the variance of the Z-axis (which indicates the sideways movement of the phone) with a p value of 4,03×10-4 and the variance of the X-axis (which indicates the forward movement of the phone) with a p value of 0,03. The correlation values are displayed in table 2.

6 Conclusion

We observe that the inter-person characteristics on affective response are large and that the absolute affect is hard to predict from the accelerometer data. When using normalized response data, a strong inverse correlation between affective state and the runners’ forward and sideways movement characteristics is observed. The low corre- lation values of the Y-axis features show that vertical movement characteristics do not change sufficiently in relation to affective state.

We measured movement on the arm and not on other parts of the body, therefore we cannot determine if the movement is specific for the arm or for the whole body.

Concluding, our model can be used to detect changes in affective state based on changes in movement. Using the accelerometer sensor of a smartphone, our model can be applied in a digital coaching-app.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS.

We thank the participants, the Radboud University, the University of Twente and the VU University of Amsterdam for making the creation of the dataset possible.

This publication was supported by the Dutch national program COMMIT and the Amsterdam Creative Industries Network.

7 REFERENCES

1. Kleinsmith, A., & Bianchi-Berthouze, N. Affective body expression perception and recognition: A survey. Affective Computing, IEEE Transactions on, 4(1), 15-33 (2013)

2. Ekkekakis, P., Parfitt, G., & Petruzzello, S. J. The pleasure and displeasure people feel when they exercise at different intensities. Sports Medicine, 41(8), 641-671 (2011)

3. Bicocchi, N., Mamei, M., & Zambonelli, F. Detecting activities from body-worn accelerometers via instance-based algorithms. Pervasive and Mobile Compu- ting, 6(4), 482-495 (2010)

4. Bood, R. J., Nijssen, M., van der Kamp, J., & Roerdink, M. The power of auditory- motor synchronization in sports: enhancing running performance by coupling ca- dence with the right beats. PloS one, 8(8), e70758 (2013)

5. Hardy, C. J., & Rejeski, W. J. Not what, but how one feels: The measurement of affect during exercise. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 11(3), 304-317.

(1989)

Referenties

GERELATEERDE DOCUMENTEN

Based on the aboveinformatiOn that highiights the seriousness oftheimpact ofthe phenomenon Ofchiid−headed househoIds,andIn the absenceofresearch studies that focus speCificaiiy

Uit het feit dat mexiletine (Namuscla®) net zo effectief is als bestaande mexiletine preparaten maar wel veel duurder, volgt dat mexiletine (Namuscla®) niet kosteneffectief

Per saldo zijn voor het gemiddelde glasgroentebedrijf de opbrengsten in 2005 wel gestegen: de opbrengsten liggen 3% hoger dan vorig jaar.. Hogere opbrengstprijzen voor snijbloemen

In Figure 1 the input data (external air temperature, relative humidity and solar irradiation) and the output data (indoor air temperature and relative humidity) are shown

De eerste analyses geven een beeld van de dagelijkse mobiliteit in Nederland, uitgedrukt in het aantal verplaatsingen, de afgelegde afstand en reisduur per

Voor het onderzoeken van de convergente validiteit van de RVL zijn er Pearson correlaties berekend tussen de relatiedimensies van de RVL (Na- bijheid, Conflict en

Now we will introduce the Erd˝ os-R´ enyi graphs and a proposition regarding the neighbourhood sizes of these graphs, which will be useful in the rest of this thesis.. 2.2 Erd˝

In general, the analysis of the self-reported emotion scores in study 2 confirm the findings of study 1: the affective mirror succeeds in inducing positive emotions in