• No results found

The effect of time pressure on referential overspecification

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Share "The effect of time pressure on referential overspecification"

Copied!
2
0
0

Bezig met laden.... (Bekijk nu de volledige tekst)

Hele tekst

(1)

Tilburg University

The effect of time pressure on referential overspecification

Koolen, R.; Gatt, A.; van Gompel, R.P.G.; Krahmer, E.; van Deemter, K.

Publication date:

2016

Document Version

Peer reviewed version

Link to publication in Tilburg University Research Portal

Citation for published version (APA):

Koolen, R., Gatt, A., van Gompel, R. P. G., Krahmer, E., & van Deemter, K. (2016). The effect of time pressure on referential overspecification. Poster session presented at RefNet Round Table Event 2016, Aberdeen, United Kingdom.

General rights

Copyright and moral rights for the publications made accessible in the public portal are retained by the authors and/or other copyright owners and it is a condition of accessing publications that users recognise and abide by the legal requirements associated with these rights. • Users may download and print one copy of any publication from the public portal for the purpose of private study or research. • You may not further distribute the material or use it for any profit-making activity or commercial gain

• You may freely distribute the URL identifying the publication in the public portal

Take down policy

If you believe that this document breaches copyright please contact us providing details, and we will remove access to the work immediately and investigate your claim.

(2)

The effect of time pressure on referential overspecification

Speakers often produce definite referring expressions that are overspecified. For example, they may refer to a chair as “the green chair” in a situation where only one chair is present. Many factors have been found to induce overspecification, for example that speakers rely on quick heuristics during the attribute selection process, and that they thus prefer to include perceptually salient attributes such as color. After all, salient attributes are easily and quickly perceived by the visual system.

We argue that the use of heuristics suggests the existence of a two-stage model of attribute selection. Firstly, there is the early stage, where perceptually salient attributes are selected irrespective of whether they rule out any of the distractor objects in the scene. Secondly, there is the later stage, which is basically an object-by-object scan of the scene to see if there are any distractors left. The early is stage is speaker-oriented, while the later stage is more listener-oriented (Arnold, 2008).

To find evidence for such a two-stage model, we performed a reference production

experiment with a manipulation of Time Pressure. Half of the speakers took part in the self-paced condition, and took as much time as needed to inspect the scene and to describe the target. The other half of the speakers took part in the system-paced condition and performed their task under time pressure: although they could as well take as much time as needed to describe a target, the visual scenes disappeared automatically after 1000 milliseconds for each trial. Within participants, we manipulated the visual scenes in terms of the attributes that were required to identify the target (color; size; color or size).

We expected to find that speakers are more likely to overspecify their referring expressions in the system-paced condition than in the self-paced condition. This expectation was indeed borne out by the data: irrespective of the attributes that were required in a particular scene, redundant modifiers were more frequent in the system-paced condition (40.7%) than in the self-paced condition (31.3%), β = 0.72; SE = 0.57; p < .05. Thus, under pressure, speakers did not seem to have enough time to take the addressee perspective into account (Horton & Keysar, 1996), and mainly included attributes that simply pop out of the scene. We regard our results as evidence for a two-stage model of attribute selection, akin to recent computational models of attribute selection (e.g., Mitchell et al., 2013).

Referenties

GERELATEERDE DOCUMENTEN

Past research has examined the moderating effect of context and individual differences on the relationship between time pressure and decision-making, but the

To test the hypothesis that consumers under time pressure are more likely to make an impulse purchase, particularly when having a fast LHS (H3), a 2 (time stress; high vs.

- This alternative uses a ‘cell layout’ for the test facilities resulting in a decrease of the lead time of TCS and the possibility of using a pull system in order to decrease

Appendix 14: Lead time of test facilities with new sequence of operations Appendix 15: Test facility occupation with new sequence of operations Appendix 16: Lead time of test

This mediated relationship was curvilinear; with time pressure having little to no e ffect on transformational leadership via state core self-evaluations when time pressure is below

Traditional ways of doing business are being disrupted by collaborative economy organisations – and we might also see traditional models of business disrupted as people turn to

The loop lies in a vertical plane and rotates about a vertical diameter with constant angular velocity ω.. Prove that the product v ` of the product of the speed of the ball and

-  We measured the proportion of descriptions that was overspecified , and expected to find a higher proportion of overspecified descriptions for speakers with limited rather