• No results found

Exposure to infant images enhances attention control in mothers

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Share "Exposure to infant images enhances attention control in mothers"

Copied!
10
0
0

Bezig met laden.... (Bekijk nu de volledige tekst)

Hele tekst

(1)

Tilburg University

Exposure to infant images enhances attention control in mothers

Karreman, A.; Riem, M.M.E.

Published in:

Cognition and Emotion DOI:

10.1080/02699931.2019.1689101

Publication date: 2020

Document Version

Publisher's PDF, also known as Version of record

Link to publication in Tilburg University Research Portal

Citation for published version (APA):

Karreman, A., & Riem, M. M. E. (2020). Exposure to infant images enhances attention control in mothers. Cognition and Emotion, 34(5), 986-993. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2019.1689101

General rights

Copyright and moral rights for the publications made accessible in the public portal are retained by the authors and/or other copyright owners and it is a condition of accessing publications that users recognise and abide by the legal requirements associated with these rights. • Users may download and print one copy of any publication from the public portal for the purpose of private study or research. • You may not further distribute the material or use it for any profit-making activity or commercial gain

• You may freely distribute the URL identifying the publication in the public portal Take down policy

If you believe that this document breaches copyright please contact us providing details, and we will remove access to the work immediately and investigate your claim.

(2)

Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at

https://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=pcem20

Cognition and Emotion

ISSN: 0269-9931 (Print) 1464-0600 (Online) Journal homepage: https://www.tandfonline.com/loi/pcem20

Exposure to infant images enhances attention

control in mothers

Annemiek Karreman & Madelon M. E. Riem

To cite this article: Annemiek Karreman & Madelon M. E. Riem (2019): Exposure to infant images enhances attention control in mothers, Cognition and Emotion, DOI: 10.1080/02699931.2019.1689101

To link to this article: https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2019.1689101

© 2019 The Author(s). Published by Informa UK Limited, trading as Taylor & Francis Group

View supplementary material

Published online: 14 Nov 2019.

Submit your article to this journal

Article views: 670

View related articles

(3)

BRIEF ARTICLE

Exposure to infant images enhances attention control in mothers

Annemiek Karreman and Madelon M. E. Riem

Department of Medical and Clinical Psychology, CoRPS - Center of Research on Psychological and Somatic disorders, Tilburg University, Tilburg, the Netherlands

ABSTRACT

Viewing cute images has been reported to promote performance on tasks requiring carefulness, possibly related to an enhanced positive emotional state. However, it is unclear whether viewing infant images also enhances attention control in mothers. Therefore, this experimental study examined whether exposure to images of infants affected mothers’ performance on a visual search task, studying associations with happy facial expressivity. Mothers (N = 101, Mage= 30.88) were randomly assigned

to one of two conditions in which they either viewed images of infants or images of adults. Before and after viewing images, mothers performed a visual search task. Mothers’ happy facial expressions at baseline and when viewing images were analysed. Viewing images of infants, in contrast to viewing images of adults, improved task performance indexed by accurateness, but not the number of correct responses. Images of infants elicited happy facial expressivity, which was associated with the number of correct responses on the visual search task. This study showed that viewing images of infants evokes happy facial expressions in mothers and can improve mothers’ performance on a perceptual-cognitive task requiring attention control. Mothers’ responses to infant images may be explained as an attentional preparedness for caregiving.

ARTICLE HISTORY Received 19 February 2019 Revised 3 September 2019 Accepted 30 October 2019 KEYWORDS

Cuteness; attention control; visual search task; emotions; facial expressivity

Cute infant faces capture attention, elicit positive emotional expressions, and trigger caregiving responses (Brosch, Sander, & Scherer, 2007; Glocker et al.,2009; Hildebrandt & Fitzgerald,1978). Cuteness has been associated with baby schema, described by Lorenz (1943) with features such as a large head, high rounded forehead, large eyes, and rounded cheeks. Responses to baby schema have been pro-posed to activate the parental care motivational system, consisting of affective and cognitive mechan-isms that evolved to regulate behavioural responses, preparing the individual to protect and provide care (Hofer, Buckels, White, Beall, & Schaller,2017; Schaller,

2018).

Experimental studies have shown that this parental care motivational system becomes activated when individuals view cute images of infants or baby

animals. For example, it has been shown that viewing images of infants or baby animals improves careful behaviour, measured by performance onfine motor dexterity tasks (Nittono, Fukushima, Yano, & Moriya, 2012; Sherman, Haidt, & Coan, 2009; Sherman, Haidt, Iyer, & Coan, 2013). Nittono et al. (2012) have furthermore shown that viewing cute images of baby animals improved performance on visual search tasks. This may be interpreted as improved top-down attention control, because pre-vious research has shown that visual search tasks involve executive functions, requiring individuals to identify and localise objects and to control the disen-gagement of attention (Han & Kim,2004;2009; Peter-son, Beck, & Wong,2008). Cute images may trigger a positive emotional state that is associated with approach motivation and a tendency toward

© 2019 The Author(s). Published by Informa UK Limited, trading as Taylor & Francis Group

This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives License (http://creativecommons.org/ licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/), which permits non-commercial re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited, and is not altered, transformed, or built upon in any way.

CONTACT Annemiek Karreman a.karreman@tilburguniversity.edu

Supplemental data for this article can be accessed https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2019.1689101 COGNITION AND EMOTION

(4)

systematic processing, resulting in enhanced atten-tion control that may be part of the individual’s cogni-tive processes needed to provide care (Nittono et al.,

2012). Indeed, recent studies have shown that mothers’ executive functioning is a determinant of parenting behaviours (Crandall, Deater-Deckard, & Riley, 2015), is related to a correct interpretation of the infants’ mental states (Yatziv, Kessler, & Atzaba-Poria,2018) and is associated with mothers’ curiosity towards their infants’ mental states (Rutherford et al.,

2018).

Happy facial expressions may be another behav-ioural manifestation of an activated parental care motivational system, as positive emotional facial expressions are important in mother-infant interaction to facilitate the attachment process (Beebe et al.,2016; Bowlby,1969). Individuals who view images of infants respond with increased facial zygomaticus muscle activity, a facial electromyographic response associ-ated with happy facial expressions (Hildebrandt & Fitz-gerald, 1978). Another study showed that this response was more strongly induced by infant and baby animal images than by neutral images and attractive images unrelated to baby schema (Nittono & Ihara, 2017). However, it is still unknown whether happy facial expressivity in response to cute images is associated with attention control. In addition, exper-imental studies to date on cuteness effects (Nittono et al.,2012; Sherman et al.,2009;2013) have not yet focused on mothers of young children. Thus, it is unknown how viewing cute images of infants is related to attention control in young mothers, who have a role in nurturing infants.

This experimental study is thefirst to examine the effects of viewing images of infants, compared to viewing images of adults, on mothers’ performance on a visual search task, additionally examining relations with happy facial expressivity. Mothers were randomly assigned to one of two conditions (adult images, infant images). Before and after viewing images, mothers performed a visual search task. First, effects of viewing images on visual search task performance were tested. Two measures of visual search task performance were examined: the number of correct responses and accurateness of per-formance (i.e. error rate). Mothers who viewed infant faces were predicted to perform better on the visual search task (i.e. more correct responses and more accurate performance) than mothers who viewed adult faces. Second, effects of viewing images on happy facial expressions were tested. Happy facial

expressions before viewing images (baseline) and when viewing images were analysed. Mothers who viewed infant faces were predicted to respond to infant images with stronger increased happy facial expressivity compared to mothers who viewed adult faces. Third, we examined whether increased happy facial expressivity in mothers when viewing infant images was associated with better performance on the visual search task. Increased happy facial expres-sivity when viewing infant images was expected to be associated with better performance on the visual search task.

Methods

We report how we determined our sample size, all data exclusions (if any), all manipulations, and all measures in the study.

Participants

Mothers were recruited by contacting day-care centres and by asking among acquaintances. Exclu-sion criteria were hearing problems and insufficient mastery of the Dutch language. The total sample con-sisted of 101 mothers (Mage= 30.88 years, SDage=

4.23) with an infant up to three years of age (Mage=

13.68 months, SDage= 8.99). The sociodemographic

characteristics of the participants are shown in Table S1. A priori power analysis using G*Power 3.1 for repeated measures (within-between interaction) showed that a sample size of 90 is sufficient to detect small to medium effects (f = .15, α = .05, power .80, 2 groups, 2 measurements).

Procedure

Mothers were visited at home by trained undergradu-ate students as part of a larger study on the perception of infant signals. They were asked to complete an online questionnaire (not used in this study, with the exception of demographic details), one or two weeks before the visit took place. The home visit included tasks for three studies, lasting about 60 min in total. The visit started withfitting electrodes of an ambulat-ory monitoring system for the measurement of skin conductance for the purpose of other studies. The tasks of the current study were administeredfirst, fol-lowed by tasks that were part of other studies.

Mothers participated in the adult images (n = 44) or infant images (n = 57) condition. Data collection

(5)

started after randomly assigning participant numbers to one of the two conditions, by selecting a random sample of approximately 50% of all cases in SPSS. The design was a 2 (pre-test, post-test) x 2 (adult images, infant images) within-between subjects design. First, mothers performed a visual search task (based on Nittono et al., 2012), which was the pre-test of task performance. Next, mothers viewed and ranked the adult or infant images. Finally, mothers performed the visual search task again, which was the post-test of task performance (see Figure S1 for a flow chart of the procedure). Facial expressions were recorded using a webcam during the presen-tation of the images. Permission for this study was obtained from the local ethics committee (protocol number: EC-2016.38). All mothers signed for consent.

Stimuli

Eight images of adult faces or infant faces were pre-sented on a computer screen, four seconds per image (Sherman et al., 2013). Next, mothers were given eight plasticised sheets (paper size A4) with coloured photo images of the same faces. Mothers were asked to rank the images according to kindness (from“least kind” to “most kind”) in the adult images condition or cuteness (from “least cute” to “most cute”) in the infant images condition (Nittono et al.,

2012). By ranking all images, mothers were to a larger extent exposed to the images than when viewing the images only.

The adult and infant images were selected from a larger set of images tested in a pilot study, with N = 44 women (age M = 28.01 years, SD = 6.01, 79.5% without children). In the pilot study, 20 infant images and 20 adult images were rated on cuteness and kindness on a scale ranging from 1 to 4. Eight infant and eight adult (four women) images with com-parable scores on cuteness (M = 3.12, SD = .43) and kindness (M = 3.11, SD = 0.27) were selected for the current study.

Measures

Visual search task performance

An adapted version of the visual search task used by Nittono et al. (2012) was administered. Eighteen different matrices, consisting of 40 digits, were printed on paper (digits 0–9, 4 rows by 10 columns). All digits were distributed randomly. A target digit was shown on the left side of each matrix. The

target digit was different for each matrix. Mothers were instructed to search the matrix for this particular digit and to cross out these digits in the matrix. If they had completed a matrix they could proceed to the next one. They were instructed to complete as many matrices as quickly and as accurately as possible in three minutes. They could stop when the test leader said ‘stop’. This instruction was followed by an example matrix, in which the target digits had already been crossed out, and a practice matrix, which was completed together with the test leader. Correct trials ranged from two to six. Mothers per-formed the visual search task before viewing adult or infant images (pre-test) and directly after viewing adult or infant images (post-test). For both measures, two scores were calculated: the number of correct digits, representing the number of correct responses, and the error rate, representing the accuracy (pro-portion of incorrect digits plus missed target digits). Missed target digits of the final matrix searched by the participant were not included in the error rate because these digits could have been missed because time was up.

Happy facial expressions

Mothers’ happy facial expressions were recorded and analysed afterwards using Noldus’s FaceReader 7 of the Behavioral Physiology lab (GO-LAB, Tilburg Univer-sity). This software was trained using the Facial Action Coding System (Ekman, Friesen, & Hager,2002). FaceR-eaderfirst detects the face, then models the face in 3D by describing over 500 key points in the face and the facial texture of the face entangled by these points, and finally classifies the face, using an artificial neural network to recognise patterns (https://www. noldus.com/facereader/facial-expression-analysis). High accuracy and convergent validity have been reported (Lewinski, den Uyl, & Butler, 2014). Happy facial expressions were analysed twice: at baseline during an 8.41-second episode of neutral written instructions and during a 32-second episode in which the mother viewed images of adults or infants that were presented on the computer screen. Intervals of 0.033 s were analysed, resulting in 253 scores at baseline and 960 scores in each condition. Scores rep-resent the strength of an emotional expression, ranging from 0 (absence of emotion) to 1 (full intensity of emotion). The missing data rate was 5.2%. Intact data were averaged to create the happy facial expression scores at baseline and when viewing images.

(6)

Statistical analyses

Data were first inspected for missings, outliers, and distributions. Two repeated measures ANOVAs were performed to examine effects of viewing infant images on mothers’ task performance. The first analy-sis included the number of correct digits as dependent variable, the second analysis included error rate as dependent variable. In both analyses, time (pre, post) was entered as a within-subject factor and con-dition (adult images, infant images) as a between-subject factor. Next, a repeated measure ANOVA was performed to test effects of viewing infant images on mothers’ happy facial expressions. Again, time (baseline, viewing images) was entered as a within-subject factor and condition (adult images, infant images) as a between-subject factor. Simple effects tests were conducted for the interpretation of effects, based on pairwise comparisons among the estimated marginal means. Finally, to test whether happy facial expressivity was associated with mothers’ task performance, we calculated correlations between the happy facial expressivity difference score (i.e. viewing images minus baseline score) and task performance difference scores (i.e. post-test minus pre-test scores) in both conditions.

Results

Preliminary and descriptive analyses

Visual search task scores of six participants were missing due to procedural errors, and facial expression scores of five participants were missing due to technical errors (n = 3), a procedural error (n = 1), and because no consent was given to be video-taped (n = 1). Outliers were replaced by the most extreme value of the variable in the dataset after excluding the outliers (number of correct digits before viewing images: n = 1, error rate after viewing images n = 5, baseline happiness n = 5, number of correct digits difference score n = 2). Ana-lyses were repeated including outliers, which yielded the same results (not reported). Skewness and kurto-sis values of the variables were below 1.53 and 1.50 respectively, with the exception of post-test error rate (skewness: 1.61, kurtosis: 2.37). Skewness and kurtosis of all variables were in an acceptable range (Curran, West, & Finch,1996).

Independent samples t-tests showed that mothers in the adult images condition did not differ from

mothers in the infant images condition on age, t(99) = .67, p = .50, Cohen’s d = 0.13, number of children, t (94) = .36, p = .72, Cohen’s d = 0.08, age of the young-est child, t(94) = .67, p = .51, Cohen’s d = 0.14, pre-test number of correct digits, t(93) =−.25, p = .80, Cohen’s d = 0.05, pre-test error rate, t(93) = −1.26, p = .21, Cohen’s d = 0.26, and baseline happy facial expressions, t(92.62) =−1.20, p = .24, Cohen’s d = 0.24. Table S4 presents the descriptive statistics for each condition.

In both conditions, the pre-test number of correct digits was not significantly correlated with the pre-test error rate (adult images condition r = 0.04, p = .79, infant images condition r =−0.11, p = .41). Simi-larly, the post-test number of correct digits was not significantly correlated with the post-test error rate (adult images condition r =−0.26, p = .10, infant images condition r = 0.06, p = .68).

Visual search task performance

The analysis examining the number of correct digits showed no time X condition effect, F(1, 93) = .05, p = .82, partialɳ2= .001, but an effect of time, F(1, 93) = 37.62, p < .001, partial ɳ2= .29. The number of correct responses improved after viewing the images in both conditions, which may be due to practice (pre-test M = 62.29, 95% CI [58.47, 66.11], post-test M = 69.72, 95% CI [66.02, 73.42]). In contrast, the analy-sis with the error rate as dependent variable showed no main effect of time, F(1, 93) = .91, p = .34, partial ɳ2= .01, but a significant time X condition effect, F(1,

93) = 4.29, p = .04, partialɳ2= .04. Simple effects tests revealed that mothers showed an increased error rate after viewing adult images, F(1, 93) = 4.02, p = .048, partial ɳ2= .04 (pre-test M = .02, 95% CI [.01, .03], post-test M = .04, 95% CI [.03, .05]), whereas the error rate of mothers who viewed infant images stayed the same, F(1, 93) = .73, p = .40, partial ɳ2= .008 (pre-test M = .03, 95% CI [.02, .04], post-test

M = .03, 95% CI [.02, .04]). These results are visualised in Figure 1. See Figure S2 for scatterplots, showing the changes across individuals (according to Weissger-ber, Milic, Winham, & Garovic, 2015). Thus, viewing images of infants improved mothers’ accurate per-formance on the visual search task.

Happy facial expressions

The analysis examining happy facial expressions showed an effect of time, F(1, 94) = 46.33, p < .001

(7)

partialɳ2= .33, as well as a significant time X condition effect, F(1, 94) = 16.65, p < .001 partial ɳ2= .15. Simple effects tests showed that, compared to baseline, happy facial expressivity increased when viewing images of infants, F(1, 94) = 69.38, p < .001 partial ɳ2= .43 (pre-test M = .06, 95% CI [.05, .08], post-test

M = .27, 95% CI [.21, .32]), but not when viewing images of adults, F(1, 94) = 3.24, p = .08 partial ɳ2= .03 (pre-test M = .05, 95% CI [.03, .07], post-test

M = .10, 95% CI [.04, .16]). Figure 2 visualises these results. Scatterplots are shown in Figure S3 (Weissger-ber et al.,2015).

Correlations between the facial expression di ffer-ence score and task performance difference scores showed no association in the adult image condition (r =˗ 0.11, p = .50 for number of correct digits differ-ence score, r = 0.14, p = .40 for error rate difference score). Neither was there a significant association

Figure 1.Mean (SE) error rate scores for the adult images and infant images conditions, before (pre) and after (post) viewing images.

Notes: Error rate scores represent the proportion of incorrect digits plus missed target digits in the visual search task. A significant result of the simple effects test, indicating a difference between pre- and post-test scores, is marked with a star. *p < .05.

Figure 2Mean (SE) happy facial expressivity scores for the adult images and infant images conditions, at baseline and when viewing images.

Note: A significant result of the simple effects test, indicating a difference between baseline score and score when viewing images, is marked with a star. ***p < .001.

(8)

between the facial expression difference score and the error rate difference score in the infant image condition, (r =˗0.20, p = .15). However, the facial expression difference score was positively associated with the number of correct digits difference score (r = 0.27, p = .049; see Figure S4 for a scatter plot). These findings indicate that increased happy facial expressivity in mothers when viewing images of infants was positively associated with an increased number of correct responses.

Discussion

In support of our hypothesis, this study showed that exposure to images of infants improved mothers’ accurate performance on a visual search task. Mothers’ number of correct responses on the visual search task also improved, but unexpectedly as well in mothers who viewed images of adults. In addition, support was found for a role of happy facial expressions: Infant images elicited happy facial expressivity in mothers, which was positively associ-ated with the number of correct responses on the visual search task. However, no associations were found between induced happy facial expressivity and task accuracy.

Thesefindings extend prior research on effects of cute images in two ways. First, effects were demon-strated in mothers of young children, who performed a visual search task that required attention control. Enhanced attention control may be part of the cogni-tive processes in mothers needed to provide care (Nittono et al.,2012), which corresponds with previous studies have shown that mothers’ executive function-ing is related to parentfunction-ing behaviours and aspects important for parental care, such as an increased attention for and correct interpretation of infants’ mental states (Crandall et al.,2015; Rutherford et al.,

2018; Yatziv et al.,2018). Prior research showed that cute images of baby animals improved university stu-dents’ performance on a visual search task (Nittono et al.,2012). In contrast to ourfindings, effects were reported on the number of correct matrices, but not the error rate. One explanation for these contrasting results is that task accurateness may rely to a larger extent on attention control than the number of correct responses, which may not only demand execu-tive functions related to the allocation and control of attention (Han & Kim, 2004) but also other abilities. For example, it may also rely on motor speed, which may be less relevant in mothers to provide care.

Moreover, task accurateness likely relates to some extent to tenderness, which may be especially improved by cuteness in mothers. Indeed, Sherman et al. (2009) found that cute images induced tender-ness in students, especially reported by women, which was positively associated with performance on afine-motor dexterity task. Tenderness, suggested to be aroused when the parental motivational system is activated (Hofer et al.,2017; Schaller,2018), may par-ticularly be triggered by cuteness in mothers to help them with their caregiving practices.

Second, increased happy facial expressions when viewing infant images were found associated with better performance on the perceptual-cognitive task, which has not been demonstrated before. Facial expressions are likely a behavioural manifestation of the parental care motivational system (Hofer et al.,

2017; Schaller, 2018). Mothers’ happy facial expressions play an important role in the affective mother–child interchange, eliciting positive emotion expressions in their infants in return (Mendes, Seidl-de-Moura, & de Oliveira Siqueira,2009). The effect of viewing cute pictures is suggested to be related to, at least to some extent, a positive emotional state (Nittono et al.,2012). Sherman et al. (2009) did not find positive emotion experience to be responsible for the link between cuteness and carefulness in their study. Future research should elucidate the mechanism among happy facial expressions, a posi-tive emotional state, and activation of the parental care motivational system.

Several limitations of this study should be noted. First, we did not relate individual cuteness ratings of the infant images to mothers’ task performance and happy facial expressions. However, previous study findings suggest that responses to images of infant faces are independent of perceived cuteness ratings (Endendijk, Spencer, van Baar, & Bos, 2018; Hildebrandt & Fitzgerald, 1978). Second, the visual search task we used was an adapted version of the task used in prior research (Nittono et al.,2012). In the original task participants were asked to mentally count the number of target digits, using working memory without using hands. The task difference may relate to differences in study findings. Third, we did not examine possible moderating effects of mothers’ psychological problems, such as depression and anxiety, which may bias the processing of infant images (Webb & Ayers, 2015). Lastly, it should be noted that performance on the perceptual-cognitive task may host not only attentional control but also

(9)

other executive functions, such as response inhi-bition. However, our assumption that enhanced per-formance on the visual search task reflects an attentional effect is in line with neuroimaging research suggesting that cute infant stimuli elicit selective attentional biasing by triggering specific and rapid neural responses in affective brain regions, such as the orbitofrontal cortex (Kringelbach, Stark, Alexander, Bornstein, & Stein,2017).

Conclusion

This study showed that viewing images of infants can improve mothers’ accuracy on a perceptual-cog-nitive task requiring attention control. Further, infant images elicited increased happy facial expressions, which in turn were associated with mothers’ per-formance on the task. Mothers’ responses to infant images may be explained as a biologically based attentional preparedness for caregiving. Research should unravel the mechanisms through which cute-ness elicits attention control and how it supports parental care.

Disclosure statement

No potential conflict of interest was reported by the authors.

References

Beebe, B., Messinger, D., Bahrick, L. E., Margolis, A., Buck, K. A., & Chen, H. (2016). A systems view of mother-infant face-to-face communication. Developmental Psychology, 52, 556–571.

doi:10.1037/a0040085.

Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss: Vol. 1. Attachment. New York: Basic Books.

Brosch, T. B., Sander, D., & Scherer, K. R. (2007). That baby caught my eye… attention capture by infant faces. Emotion, 7, 685– 689.doi:10.1037/1528-3542.7.3.685

Crandall, A., Deater-Deckard, K., & Riley, A. W. (2015). Maternal emotion and cognitive control capacities and parenting: A conceptual framework. Developmental Review, 36, 105–126.

doi:10.1016/j.dr.2015.01.004

Curran, P. J., West, S. G., & Finch, J. F. (1996). The robustness of test statistics to nonnormality and specification error in confi-rmatory factor analysis. Psychological Methods, 1, 16–29.

doi:10.1037/1082-989X.1.1.16

Ekman, P., Friesen, W. V., & Hager, J. C. (2002). Facial action coding system: An investigator’s guide. Salt Lake City: Research Nexus. Endendijk, J. J., Spencer, H., van Baar, A. L., & Bos, P. A. (2018). Mothers’ neural responses to infant faces are associated with activation of the maternal care system and observed intrusiveness with their own child. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 18, 609–621. doi:10.3758/s13415-018-0592-6

Glocker, M. L., Langleben, D. D., Ruparel, K., Loughead, J. W., Gur, R. C., & Sachser, N. (2009). Baby schema in infant faces induces cuteness perception and motivation for caretaking in adults. Ethology, 115, 257–263. doi:10.1111/j.1439-0310. 2008.01603.x

Han, S.-H., & Kim, M.-S. (2004). Visual search does not remain efficient when executive working memory is working. Psychological Science, 15, 623–628.doi:10.1111/j.0956-7976. 2004.00730.x

Han, S.-H., & Kim, M.-S. (2009). Do the contents of working memory capture attention? Yes, but cognitive control matters. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 35, 1292–1302. doi:10.1037/ a0016452

Hildebrandt, K. A., & Fitzgerald, H. E. (1978). Adults responses to infants varying in perceived cuteness. Behavioural Processes, 3, 159–172.doi:10.1016/0376-6357 (78)90042-6

Hofer, M. K., Buckels, E. E., White, C. J., Beall, A. T., & Schaller, M. (2017). Individual differences in activation of the parental care motivational system: An empirical distinction between protection and nurturance. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 9, 907–916. doi:10.1177/194855061 7728994

Kringelbach, M. L., Stark, E. A., Alexander, C., Bornstein, M. H., & Stein, A. (2017). On cuteness: Unlocking the parental brain and beyond. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 20, 545-558.doi:10. 1016/j.tics.2016.05.003

Lewinski, P., den Uyl, T. M., & Butler, C. (2014). Automated facial coding: Validation of basic emotions and FACS AUs in FaceReader. Journal of Neuroscience, Psychology, and Economics, 7, 227–236.doi:10.1037/npe0000028

Lorenz, K. (1943). Die angeborenen Formen möglicher Erfahrung [Innate forms of potential experience]. Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie, 5, 235–409. doi:10.1111/j.1439-0310.1943. tb00655.x

Mendes, D. M. L. F., Seidl-de-Moura, M. L., & de Oliveira Siqueira, J. (2009). The ontogenesis of smiling and its association with mothers’ affective behaviors: A longitudinal study. Infant Behavior and Development, 32, 445–453.doi:10.1016/j.infbeh. 2009.07.004

Nittono, H., Fukushima, M., Yano, A., & Moriya, H. (2012). The power of kawaii: Viewing cute images promotes a careful behavior and narrows attentional focus. PLoS One, 7, e46362.doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0046362

Nittono, H., & Ihara, N. (2017). Psychophysiological responses to Kawaii pictures with or without baby schema. SAGE Open, 7, 1–11.

Peterson, M. S., Beck, M. R., & Wong, J. H. (2008). Were you paying attention to where you looked? The role of executive working memory in visual search. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 15, 372-377.doi:10.3758/PBR.15.2.372

Rutherford, H. J. V., Byrne, S. P., Crowley, M. J., Bornstein, J., Bridgett, D. J., & Mayes, L. C. (2018). Executive functioning predicts reflective functioning in mothers. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 27, 944–952. doi:10.1007/s10826-017-0928-9

Schaller, M. (2018). The parental care motivational system and why it matters (for everyone). Current Directions in Psychological Science, 27, 295–301. doi:10.1177/0963721 418767873

(10)

Sherman, G. D., Haidt, J., & Coan, J. A. (2009). Viewing cute images increases behavioral carefulness. Emotion, 9, 282–286.doi:10. 1037/a0014904

Sherman, G. D., Haidt, J., Iyer, R., & Coan, J. A. (2013). Individual differences in the physical embodiment of care: Prosocially oriented women respond to cuteness by becoming more physically careful. Emotion, 13, 151–158. doi:10.1037/ a0029259

Webb, R., & Ayers, S. (2015). Cognitive biases in processing infant emotion by women with depression, anxiety and post-trau-matic stress disorder in pregnancy or after birth: A systepost-trau-matic

review. Cognition and Emotion, 29, 1278-1294.doi:10.1080/ 02699931.2014.977849

Weissgerber, T. L., Milic, N. M., Winham, S. J., Garovic, V. D. (2015). Beyond bar and line graphs: Time for a new data presentation paradigm. PLoS Biology, 13, e1002128. doi:10.1371/journal. pbio.1002128

Yatziv, T., Kessler, Y., & Atzaba-Poria, N. (2018). What’s going on in my baby’s mind? Mothers’ executive functions contribute to individual differences in maternal mentalization during mother-infant interactions. PLoS ONE, 13, e0207869.doi:10. 1371/journal.pone.0207869

Referenties

GERELATEERDE DOCUMENTEN

The primary objective and research question of the study concerns the design of a mobile application for the trading of used books at a university following a design science

Hierdie benaderingswyse kan in die keuse van die volgende temas gevind word: 'n geografiese en geologiese oorsig wat die natuurlike omgewing beskryf, 'n oorsig

vraatschade die door zowel larven als kevers in het veld wordt toegebracht aan al deze getoetste plan- ten, duidt de afwezigheid van aantrekking door middel van geur- stoffen

Changes in the extent of recorded crime can therefore also be the result of changes in the population's willingness to report crime, in the policy of the police towards

Hypothesis 2c predicted that the positive relationship from job satisfaction of teachers to student outcomes the next year was stronger than the positive relationship form student

H1: e-bike users are more satisfied with their commuting, shopping and leisure trips than conventional bicycle users Supported H2: instrumental attributes of (e-)bikes are

Trip satisfaction of both e-bike users and conventional bicyclists depend largely on how they perceive the instrumental attributes, which in turn influences how the hedonic

Second, we examined the effects of enhancement and suppression on mothers’ self-reported perception of the laugh sound, self- reported intended sensitive and insensitive