• No results found

University of Groningen Cracking the code Borleffs, Lotte Elisabeth

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Share "University of Groningen Cracking the code Borleffs, Lotte Elisabeth"

Copied!
21
0
0

Bezig met laden.... (Bekijk nu de volledige tekst)

Hele tekst

(1)

Cracking the code

Borleffs, Lotte Elisabeth

IMPORTANT NOTE: You are advised to consult the publisher's version (publisher's PDF) if you wish to cite from

it. Please check the document version below.

Document Version

Publisher's PDF, also known as Version of record

Publication date:

2018

Link to publication in University of Groningen/UMCG research database

Citation for published version (APA):

Borleffs, L. E. (2018). Cracking the code: Towards understanding, diagnosing and remediating dyslexia in

Standard Indonesian. Rijksuniversiteit Groningen.

Copyright

Other than for strictly personal use, it is not permitted to download or to forward/distribute the text or part of it without the consent of the author(s) and/or copyright holder(s), unless the work is under an open content license (like Creative Commons).

Take-down policy

If you believe that this document breaches copyright please contact us providing details, and we will remove access to the work immediately and investigate your claim.

Downloaded from the University of Groningen/UMCG research database (Pure): http://www.rug.nl/research/portal. For technical reasons the number of authors shown on this cover page is limited to 10 maximum.

(2)

REFERENCES

Acha, J., Laka, I., & Perea, M. (2010). Reading development in agglutinative languages: Evidence from beginning, intermediate, and adult Basque readers. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 105, 359-375. DOI:10.1016/j.jecp.2009.10.008

Adsett, C. R., & Marchand, Y. (2010). Syllabic Complexity: A Computational Evaluation of Nine

European Languages. Journal of Quantitative Linguistics, 17, 269-290.

DOI:10.1080/09296174. 2010.512161

Ans, B., Carbonnel, S., & Valdois, S. (1998). A connectionist multiple-trace memory model for polysyllabic word reading. Psychological Review, 105, 678-723. DOI:10.1037/0033-295X.105.4.678-723

Aravena, S., Snellings, P., Tijms, J., & van der Molen, M. W. (2013). A lab-controlled simulation of a letter–speech sound binding deficit in dyslexia. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 115, 691-707. DOI:10.1016/j.jecp.2013.03.009

Aravena, S., Tijms, J., Snellings, P., & van der Molen, M. W. (2017). Predicting Individual Differences in Reading and Spelling Skill With Artificial Script–Based Letter–Speech Sound Training. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 1-13. DOI:10.1177/0022219417715407

Aro, K., Aro, T., Ahonen, T., Räsänen, T., Hietala, A., & Lyytinen, H. (1999). The development of phonological abilities and their relation to reading acquisition: Case studies of six Finnish children. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 32, 457-463. DOI:10.1177/002221949903200511 Aro, M. (2004). Learning to read: The effect of orthography (Jyväskylä Studies in Education,

Psychology and Social Research, publication No. 237). Jyväskylä, Finland: University of Jyväskylä.

Aro, M., & Wimmer, H. (2003). Learning to read: English in comparison to six more regular orthographies. Applied Psycholinguistics, 24, 621-635. DOI:10.1017.S0142716403000316 Arvaniti, A. (2012). The usefulness of metrics in the quantification of speech rhythm. Journal of

Phonetics, 40, 351-373. doi:10.1016/j.wocn.2012.02.003

Arvaniti, A., & Rodriquez, T. (2013). The role of rhythm class, speaking rate and F0 in language discrimination. Laboratory Phonology, 4, 7-38. doi:10.1515/lp-2013-0002

Aud, S., Wilkinson-Flicker, S., Kristapovich, P., Rathbun, A., Wang, X., and Zhang, J. (2013). The Condition of Education 2013 (NCES 2013-037). U.S. Department of Education, National Center for Education Statistics. Washington, DC. Retrieved 3 March 2016 from https://nces.ed.gov/pubs2013/2013037.pdf

Balota, D. A., Yap, M. Y., & Cortese, M. J. (2006). Visual word recognition: The journey from features to meaning. In M. Traxler & M. A. Gernsbacher (Eds.), The Handbook of Psycholinguistics (2nd ed., pp. 285-375). New York, NY: Academic Press, 2006.

Bane, M. (2008). Quantifying and measuring morphological complexity. In C. B. Chang & H. J. Haynie (Eds.), Proceedings of the 26th West Coast Conference on Formal Linguistics (pp.

69-76). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Proceedings Project.

Barca, L., Burani, C., Di Filippo, G., & Zoccolotti, P. (2006). Italian developmental dyslexia and proficient readers: Where are the differences? Brain and Language, 98, 347-351. DOI:10.1016/j.bandl.2006.05.001

Beddington, J., Cooper, C.L., Field, J., Goswami, U., Huppert, F.A., Jenkins, R., et al. (2008). The mental wealth of nations. Nature, 455, 1057-1060. DOI:10.1038/4551057a

(3)

Bekebrede, J., van der Leij, A., & Share, D. L. (2009). Dutch dyslexic adolescents: Phonological-core variable-orthographic differences. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 22, 133-165. DOI:10.1007/s11145-007-9105-7

Ben-Dror, I., Bentin, S., & Frost, R. (1995). Semantic, phonological, and morphological skills in reading-disabled and normal children: Evidence from perception and production of spoken Hebrew. Reading Research Quarterly, 30, 876-893. DOI:10.2307/748202

Berninger, V. W., Abbott, R. D., Nagy, W., & Carlisle, J. (2010). Growth in Phonological, Orthographic, and Morphological Awareness in Grades 1 to 6. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 39, 141-163. DOI:10.1007/s10936-009-9130-6

Berthiaume, R., & Daigle, D. (2014). Are Dyslexic Children Sensitive to the Morphological Structure of Words When They Read? The Case of Dyslexic Readers in French. Dyslexia, 20, 241-260. DOI:10.1002/dys.1476

Bertram, R., Laine, M., Virkkala, M. M. (2000). The role of derivational morphology in vocabulary acquisition: Get by with a litter help from my morpheme friends. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 41, 287-296. DOI:10.1111/1467-9450.00201

Bishop, D. V. M., & Snowling, M. J. (2004). Developmental dyslexia and specific language impairment: Same or different? Psychological Bulletin, 130, 858-886. DOI: 10.1037/0033-2909.130.6.858

Blomert, L. & Willems, G. (2010). Is there a causal link from a phonological awareness deficit to reading failure in children with familiar risk for dyslexia? Dyslexia, 16, 300-317. DOI:10.1002/dys.405

Boets, B., De Smedt, B., Cleuren, L., Vandewalle, E., Wouters, J., & Ghesquière, P. (2010). Towards a further characterization of phonological and literacy problems in Dutch-speaking children with dyslexia. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 28, 5-31. DOI:10.1348/026151010X485223

Boets, B., Wouters, J., van Wieringen, A., De Smedt, B., & Ghesquiere, P. (2008). Modelling relations between sensory processing, speech perception, orthographic and phonological

ability, and literacy achievement. Brain and Language, 106, 29-40.

DOI:10.1016/j.bandl.2007.12.004

Borgwaldt, S. R., Hellwig, F. M., & De Groot, A. M. B. (2004). Word-initial entropy in five languages: Letter to sound and sound to letter. Written Language & Literacy, 7, 165-184. DOI:10.1075/wll.7.2.03bor

Borgwaldt, S. R., Hellwig, F. M., & De Groot, A. M. B. (2005). Onset entropy matters - Letter to phoneme mappings in seven languages. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 18, 211-229. DOI:10.1007/S11145-005-3001-9

Borleffs, E., Maassen, B. A. M., Lyytinen, H., & Zwarts, F. (2017). Measuring orthographic transparency and morphological-syllabic complexity in alphabetic orthographies: A narrative review. Reading and Writing: An interdisciplinary Journal, 30(8), 1617–1638. DOI:10.1007/s11145-017-9741-5

Bortolini, U. (1976). Tipologia sillabica dell`italiano: Studio statistico [Syllable typology in Italian: A statistical study]. In R. Simone, U. Vignuzzi, & G. Ruggiero (Eds.), Studi di fonetica e fonologia (pp. 5-22). Roma, Italy: Bulzoni.

(4)

Bradley, D. C., Sánchez-Casas, R. M., & García-Albea, J. E. (1993). The status of the syllable in the perception of Spanish and English. Language and Cognitive Processes, 8, 197-233. DOI:10.1080/01690969308406954

Brem, S., Bach, S., Kucian, K., Guttorm, T. K., Martin, E., Lyytinen, H., Brandeis, D., & Richardson, U. (2010). Brain sensitivity to print emerges when children learn letter-speech sound correspondences. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 107, 7939-7944. DOI:10.1073/pnas.0904402107

Bruck, M., & Treiman, R. (1990). Phonological awareness and spelling in normal children and dyslexics: The case of initial consonant clusters. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 50, 156-178. DOI:10.1016/0022-0965(90)90037-9

Brus, B. T. H., & Voeten, M. J. M. (1979). Een-Minuut-Test. Berkhout: Nijmegen.

Burani, C., Marcolini, S., & Stella, G. (2002). How early does morpholexical reading develop in

readers of a shallow orthography? Brain and Language, 81, 568-586.

DOI:10.1006/brln.2001.2548

Burani, C., Marcolini, S., De Luca, M., & Zoccolotti, P. (2008). Morpheme-based reading aloud: Evidence from dyslexic and skilled Italian readers. Cognition, 108, 243-262. DOI:10.1016/j.cognition.2007.12.010

Caravolas, M., Lervåg, A., Defior, S., Seidlová-Málková, G., Hulme, C. (2013). Different Patterns, but Equivalent Predictors, of Growth in Reading in Consistent and Inconsistent Orthographies. Psychological Science, 24, 1398 -1407. DOI:10.1177/0956797612473122

Caravolas, M., Lervåg, A., Mousikou, P., Efrim, C., Litavký, M., Onochie-Quintanilla, E., ... & Hulme, C. (2012). Common patterns of prediction of literacy development in different

alphabetic orthographies. Psychological Science, 23, 678-686. DOI:

10.1177/0956797611434536

Caravolas, M., Volín, J., & Hulme, C. (2005). Phoneme awareness is a key component of alphabetic literacy skills in consistent and inconsistent orthographies: Evidence from Czech and English children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 92, 107-139. DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2005.04.003

Carlisle, J. F. (2000). Awareness of the structure and meaning of morphologically complex words: impact of reading. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 12, 169-190. DOI:10.1023/A:1008131926604

Carlisle, J. F., & Stone, C. A. (2005). Exploring the role of morphemes in word reading. Reading Research Quarterly, 40, 428-449. DOI:10.1598/RRQ.40.4.3

Carrillo, S. M., Alegría, J., & Marín J. (2013). On the acquisition of some basic word spelling mechanisms in a deep (French) and a shallow (Spanish) system. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 26, 799-819. DOI:10.1007/S11145-012-9391-6

Casalis, S., & Louis-Alexandre, M. (2000). Morphological analysis, phonological analysis and learning to read French: A longitudinal study. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 12, 303-335. DOI:10.1023/A:1008177205648

Casalis, S., Colé, P., & Sopo, D. (2004). Morphological awareness in Developmental Dyslexia. Annals of Dyslexia, 54, 114-138. DOI:10.1007/s11881-004-0006-z

Castles, A., & Coltheart, M. (1993). Varieties of developmental dyslexia. Cognition, 47, 149-180. DOI:10.1016/0010-0277(93)90003-E

(5)

Clements, G. N. (1990). The Role of the Sonority Cycle in Core Syllabification. In J. Kingston & M. E. Beckman (Eds.), Papers in laboratory phonology 1: Between the grammar and physics of speech (pp. 283-333). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, MA.

Cohen, J., & Cohen, P. (1975). Applied Multiple Regression/Correlation Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences. New Jersey: Erlbaum.

Cohen, M. J., Morgan, A. M., Vaughn, M., Riccio, C. A., & Hall, J. (1999). Verbal fluency in children: Developmental issues and differential validity in distinguishing children with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder and two subtypes of dyslexia. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 14, 433-443.

Colé, P., Bouton, S., Leuwers, C., Casalis, S., & Sprenger-Charolles, L. (2012). Stem and derivational-suffix processing during reading by French second and third graders. Applied Psycholinguistics, 33, 97-120. DOI:10.1017/S0142716411000282

Coltheart, M., Curtis, B., Atkins, P., & Haller, M. (1993). Models of Reading Aloud: Dual-Route and Parallel-Distributed-Processing Approaches. Psychological Review, 100, 589-608. DOI:10.1037/0033-295X.100.4.589

Coltheart, M., Rastle, K., Perry, C., Langdon, R., & Ziegler, J. (2001). DRC: A Dual Route Cascaded Model of Visual Word Recognition and Reading Aloud. Psychological Review, 108, 204-256. DOI:10.1037//0033.295X.108.1.204

Constantinidou, M., & Stainthorp, R. (2009). Phonological awareness and reading speed deficits in reading disabled Greek-speaking children. Educational Psychology, 29, 171-186. DOI: 10.1080/01443410802613483

Cortiella, C. & Horowitz, S. H. (2014). The State of Learning Disabilities: Facts, Trends and Emerging Issues. New York: National Center for Learning Disabilities.

Cossu, G., Gugliotta, M., & Marshall, J.C. (1995). Acquisition of reading and written spelling in a transparent orthography: Two non-parallel processes? Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 7, 9-22. DOI:10.1007/BF01026945

Covington, M., & McFall, J. D. (2010). Cutting the gordian knot: The Moving-Average Type-Token

Ratio (MATTR). Journal of Quantitative Linguistics, 17, 94-100.

DOI:10.1080/09296171003643098

Cutler, A., Mehler, J., Norris, D., & Segui, J. (1986). The syllable’s differing role in the segmentation of French and English. Journal of Memory and Language, 25, 385-400. DOI:10.1016/0749-596X(86)90033-1

Dandache, S., Wouters, J., & Ghesquière, P. (2014). Development of reading and phonological skills of children at family risk for dyslexia: A longitudinal analysis from kindergarten to sixth Grade. Dyslexia, 20, 305-329. DOI:10.1002/dys.1482

Das, T., Padakannaya, P., Pugh, K. R., & Singh, N. C. (2011). Neuroimaging reveals dual routes to reading in simultaneous proficient readers of two orthographies. Neuroimage, 54, 1476-1487. DOI:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.09.022

Dauer, R. M. (1983). Stress-timing and syllable-timing reanalyzed. Journal of Phonetics, 11, 51-62.

De Jong, P. F., & Van der Leij, A. (1999). Specific contributions of phonological abilities to early reading acquisition: Results from a Dutch latent variable longitudinal study. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 450-476. DOI:10.1037/0022-0663.91.3.450

(6)

De Jong, P. F., & Van der Leij, A. (2002). Effects of Phonological Abilities and Linguistic Comprehension on the Development of Reading. Scientific Studies of Reading, 6, 51-77. DOI:10.1207/S1532799XSSR0601_03

De Jong, P. F., & Van der Leij, A. (2003). Developmental Changes in the Manifestation of a Phonological Deficit in Dyslexic Children Learning to Read a Regular Orthography. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, 22-40. DOI:10.1037/0022-0663.95.1.22

De Luca, M., Borrelli, M., Judica, A., Spinelli, D., & Zoccolotti, P. (2002). Reading words and pseudowords: An eye movement study of developmental dyslexia. Brain and Language, 80, 617-626. DOI:10.1006/brln.2001.2637

De Vos, T. (1992). Tempo Test Rekenen (TTR) [Arithmetic Number Fact Test]. Nijmegen: Berkhout.

Deacon, S. H., Parrila, R., & Kirby, J. R. (2006). Processing of derived forms in high-functioning dyslexics. Annals of Dyslexia, 56, 103-128. DOI:10.1007/s11881-006-0005-3

Dedina, M. J., & Nusbaum, H. C. (1991). PRONOUNCE: A program for pronunciation by analogy. Computer Speech and Language, 5, 55-64. DOI:10.1016/0885-2308(91)90017-K

Defior, S., Martos, F., & Aguilar, E. (2003). Influence of reading level on word spelling according to the phoneme-grapheme relationship. In R. M. Joshi, C. K. Leong, & B. L. K. Kaczmarek (Eds.), Literacy acquisition: The role of phonology, morphology and orthography (pp.131-151). Amsterdam: IOS Press.

Delattre, P. (1965). Comparing phonetic features of English, French, German and Spanish. Heidelberg: Jurnius Gross Verlag.

Dewi, F. (2003). Membaca menulis (13th ed., Vol. 1). Semarang, Indonesia: Mandira.

Duff, F.J., & Hulme, C. (2012). The role of children's phonological and semantic knowledge in

learning to read words. Scientific Studies of Reading, 16, 504–525.

DOI:10.1080/10888438.2011.598199

Duncan, L. G., Colé, P., Seymour, P. H. K., & Magnan, A. (2006). Differing sequences of metaphonological development in French and English. Journal of Child Language, 33, 369-399. DOI:10.1017/S030500090600732X

Duncan, L. G., Seymour, P. H. K., & Hill, S. (2000). A small-to-large unit progression in metaphonological awareness and reading? The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology A: Human Experimental Psychology, 53(A), 1081-1104. DOI:10.1080/713755936 Durgunôglu, A. Y., & Öney, B. (1999). A cross-linguistic comparison of phonological awareness and word recognition. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 11, 281-299. DOI:10.1023/A:1008093232622

Ehri, L. C. (2002). Reading processes, acquisition, and instructional implications. In G. Reid, & J. Wearmouth (Eds.), Dyslexia and literacy: Theory and practice (pp. 167-186). West Sussex, UK: Wiley and Sons.

Ehri, L. C. (2005). Learning to read words: Theory, findings and issues. Scientific Studies of Reading, 9, 167-188. DOI:10.1207/s1532799xssr0902_4

Eklund, K., Torppa, M., Aro, M., Leppänen, P. H. T., & Lyytinen, H. (2015). Literacy skill development of children with familial risk for dyslexia through Grades 2, 3, and 8. Journal of Educational Psychology, 107, 126-140. DOI:10.1037/a0037121

Elbro, C., & Arnbak, E. (1996). The role of morpheme recognition and morphological awareness in dyslexia. Annals of Dyslexia, 46, 209-240. DOI:10.1007/BF02648177

(7)

Elbro, C., & Jensen, M. N. (2005). Quality of phonological representations, verbal learning, and phoneme awareness in dyslexic and normal readers. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 46, 375-384. DOI:10.1111/j.1467-9450.2005.00468.x

Ellis, A. W. (1985). The cognitive neuropsychology of developmental and acquired dyslexia: A critical survey. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 2, 169-205. DOI:10.1080/02643298508252865 Escribano, C. (2007). Evaluation of the double-deficit hypothesis subtype classification of readers

in Spanish. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 40, 319-330.

DOI:10.1177/00222194070400040301

Fenk-Oczlon, G., & Fenk, A. (2008). Complexity trade-offs between the subsystems of language. In M. Miestamo, K. Sinnemäki, & F. Karlsson (Eds.), Language Complexity: Typology, Contact, Change (pp. 43-66). Philadelphia: John Benjamins Publishing Company.

Fowler, A., & Liberman, I. Y. (1995). Morphological awareness as related to early reading and spelling ability. In L. Feldman (Ed.), Morphological aspects of language processing (pp. 157-188). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Frederick, W. H., & Worden, R. L. (2011). Indonesia: a country study. Federal Research Division Library of Congress. Washington, D.C.: U.S. Government Printing.

Frost, R. (2005). Orthographic systems and skilled word recognition processes in reading. In M. J. Snowling & C. Hulme (Eds.), The science of reading: A handbook (pp. 272-295). MA, USA: Blackwell.

Frost, R. (2012). Towards a universal model of reading. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 35, 263-329. DOI:10.1017/S0140525X11001841

Frota, S., & Vigário, M. (2001). On the correlates of rhythmic distinctions: The European/Brazilian Portuguese case. Probus, 13, 247-275. DOI:10.1515/prbs.2001.005

Furnes, B., & Samuelsson, S. (2010). Predicting reading and spelling difficulties in transparent and opaque orthographies: A comparison between Scandinavian and U.S./Australian children. Dyslexia, 16, 119-142. DOI:10.1002/dys.401

Furnes, B., & Samuelsson, S. (2011). Phonological awareness and rapid automatized naming predicting early development in reading and spelling: Results from a cross-linguistic

longitudinal study. Learning and Individual Differences, 21, 85-95.

DOI:10.1016/j.lindif.2010.10.005

Génard, N., Mousty, P., Content, A., Alegria, J., Leybaert, J., & Morais, J. (1998). Methods to establish subtypes of developmental dyslexia. In P. Reitsma & L. Verhoeven (Eds.), Problems and interventions in literacy development (pp. 163-176). Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer. Georgiou, G. K., Parrila, R., & Papadopoulos, T. C. (2008). Predictors of word decoding and reading fluency across languages varying in orthographic consistency. Journal of Educational Psychology, 100, 566-580. DOI:10.1037/0022-0663.100.3.566

Georgiou, G. K., Torppa, M., Manolitsis, G., Lyytinen, H., & Parrila, R. (2012). Longitudinal predictors of reading and spelling across languages varying in orthographic consistency. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 25, 321–346. DOI:10.1007/s11145-010-9271-x

Gilger, J. W., Pennington, B. F., & De Fries, J. C. (1991). Risk for Reading Disability as a Function of Parental History in Three Family Studies. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 3, 205-217. DOI:10.1007/BF00354958

(8)

Glatz, T., Tops, W., Borleffs, E., Richardson, U., Maurits, N., Desoete., A., & Maassen, B. A. M. (submitted). Dynamic assessment of the effectiveness of digital game-based literacy training in beginning readers.

Goldsmith, J. (2001). Unsupervised learning of the morphology of a natural language. Computational linguistics, 27, 153-198. DOI:10.1162/089120101750300490

Gomez, C., & Reason, R. (2002). Cross‐linguistic transfer of phonological skills: a Malaysian perspective. Dyslexia, 8, 22-33. DOI:10.1002/dys.195

Goswami, U. (1999). Causal connections in beginning reading: The importance of rhyme. Journal of Research in Reading, 22, 217-240. DOI:10.1111/1467-9817.00087

Goswami, U., & Bryant, P. (1990). Phonological skills and learning to read. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Goswami, U., & East, M. (2000). Rhyme and analogy in beginning reading: Conceptual and

methodological issues. Applied Psycholinguistics, 21, 63-93. DOI:

10.1017/S0142716400001041

Goswami, U., Gombert, J. E., & Barrera, L. F. (1998). Children’s orthographic representations and linguistic transparency: Nonsense word reading in English, French, and Spanish. Applied Psycholinguistics, 19, 19-52. DOI:10.1017/S0142716400010560

Gough, P., & Tunmer, W. (1986). Decoding, reading and reading disability. Remedial and Special Education, 7, 6-10. DOI:10.1177/074193258600700104

Grainger, J., & Ziegler, J. C. (2011). A dual-route approach to orthographic processing. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 1-13. DOI:10.3398/fpsyg.2011.00054

Habib, M. (2000). The neurological basis of developmental dyslexia. An overview and working hypothesis. Brain, 123, 2373-2399. DOI:10.1093/brain/123.12.2373

Habib, M. & Giraud, K. (2013). Dyslexia. In O. Dulac, M. Lassonde, & H. Sarnat (Eds.), Handbook of Clinical Neurology (pp. 229-235). Elsevier. DOI:10.1016/B978-0-444-52891-9.00023-3 Harm, M. W., & Seidenberg, M. S. (1999). Phonology, reading acquisition, and dyslexia: Insights

from connectionist models. Psychological Review, 106, 491-528. DOI:10.1037//0033-295X.106.3.491

Hatcher, J., & Snowling, M. J. (2002). The phonological representations hypothesis of dyslexia: From theory to practice. In G. Reid & J. Wearmouth (Eds.), Dyslexia and literacy: Theory and practice (pp. 69-83). New York: Wiley.

Hatcher, P. J., Hulme, C., & Snowling, M. J. (2004). Explicit phoneme training combined with phonic reading instruction helps young children at risk of reading failure. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 338-358. DOI:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00225.x

Hatcher, P. J., Hulme, C., Miles, J. N. V., Carroll, J. M., & Hatcher, J., Gibbs, S., … Snowling, M. J. (2006). Efficacy of small group reading interventions for beginning readers with reading-delay: A randomized controlled trial. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 47, 820-827. DOI:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2005.01559.x

Heikkilä, R., Torppa, M., Aro, M., Närhi, V., & Ahonen, T. (2016). Double-deficit hypothesis in a clinical sample: extension beyond reading. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 49, 546-560. DOI: 10.1177/0022219415572895

Hofmann, M. J., Stenneken, O., Conrad, M., & Jacobs, A. M. (2007). Sublexical frequency measures for orthographic and phonological units in German. Behavior Research Methods, 39, 620-629. DOI:10.3758/BF03193034

(9)

Holopainen, L., Ahonen, T., & Lyytinen, H. (2001). Predicting delay in reading achievement in a highly transparent language. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 34, 401-413. DOI: 10.1177/002221940103400502

Holopainen, L., Ahonen, T., & Lyytinen, H. (2002). The role of reading by analogy in first grade

Finnish readers. Scandinavian Journal of Educational Research, 46, 83-98.

DOI:10.1080/00313830120115624

Hoover, W. A., & Gough, P. B. (1990). The Simple View of Reading. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 2, 127-160. DOI:10.1007/BF00401799

Horton, R., & Arvaniti, A. (2013). Cluster and classes in the rhythm metrics. San Diego Linguistic Papers, 4, 28-52.

Hulme, C., Hatcher, P. J., Nation, K., Brown, A., Adams, J., & Stuart, G. (2002). Phoneme Awareness Is a Better Predictor Of Early Reading Skills Than Onset-Rime Awareness. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 82, 2-28. DOI:10.1006/jecp.2002.2670

Hutzler F., & Wimmer, H. (2004). Eye movements of dyslexic children when reading in a regular orthography. Brain and Language, 89, 235-242. DOI:10.1016/S0093-934X(03)00401-2 Itô, J. (1989). A prosodic theory of epenthesis. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory, 7(2),

217-259. DOI:10.1007/BF00138077

Jap, B. A. J., Borleffs, E., & Maassen, B. A. M. (2017). Towards identifying dyslexia in Standard Indonesian: the development of a reading assessment battery. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 30, 1729-1751. DOI:10.1007/s11145-017-9748-y

Jiménez González, J. E., & Jiménez Rodríguez, R. (1999). Errores en la escritura de sílabas con grupos consonánticos: Un estudio transversal [Spelling errors in syllables with consonant clusters: A cross-sectional study]. Psicothema, 11, 125-135.

Jiménez, J. E., Rodríguez, C., & Ramírez, G. (2009). Spanish developmental dyslexia: Prevalence, cognitive profile and home literacy experiences. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 103, 167-185. DOI:10.1016/j.jecp.2009.02.004

Joanisse, M. F., Manis, F. R., Keating, P., & Seidenberg, M. S. (2000). Language deficits in children in dyslexic children: Speech perception, phonology, and morphology. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 77, 30-60. DOI:10.1006/jecp.1999.2553

Johnston, R. S., & Watson, J. E. (2004). Accelerating the development of reading, spelling and phonemic awareness skills in initial readers. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 17, 327-357. DOI:10.1023/B:READ.0000032666.66359.62

Juola, P. (1998). Measuring linguistic complexity: The morphological tier. Journal of Quantitative Linguistics, 5, 206-213. DOI:10.1080/09296179808590128

Juola, P. (2008). Assessing linguistic complexity. In M. Miestamo, K. Sinnemäki, & F. Karlsson (Eds.), Language complexity: Typology, contact, change (pp. 89-108). Amsterdam, The Netherlands: John Benjamins Publishing Company.

Juul, H., Poulsen, M., & Elbro, C. (2014). Separating speed from accuracy in beginning reading development. Journal of Educational Psychology, 106, 1096 -1106. DOI:10.1037/a0037100 Kairaluoma, L., Torppa, M., Westerholm, J., Ahonen, T., & Aro, M. (2013). The nature of and

factors related to reading difficulties among adolescents in a transparent orthography. Scientific Studies of Reading, 17, 315-332. DOI:10.1080/10888438.2012.701257

(10)

Katz, L., & Frost, R. (1992). The reading process is different for different orthographies: The orthographic depth hypothesis. In L. Katz & R. Frost (Eds.), Orthography, phonology, morphology, and meaning (pp. 67-84). Amsterdam: North-Holland.

Kettunen, K. (2014). Can type-token ratio be used to show morphological complexity of

languages? Journal of Quarterly Linguistics, 21, 223-24.

DOI:10.1080/09296174.2014.911506

Kettunen, K., Sadeniemi, M., Lindh-Knuutila, T., & Honkela, T. (2006). Analysis of EU languages through text compression. In T. Salakoski, F. Ginter, S. Pyysao, & T. Pahikkala (Eds.), FinTAL 2006, LNAI 4139 (pp. 99-109). Berlin Heidelberg, Germany: Springer-Verlag.

Kibby, M. Y., Marks, W., Morgan, S., & Long, C. J. (2004). Specific impairment in developmental reading disabilities: A working memory approach. Journal of learning disabilities, 37, 349-363. DOI:10.1177/00222194040370040601

Kolmogorov, A. N. (1965). Three approaches to the quantitative definition of information. Problems in Information Transmission, 1, 1-7. DOI:10.1080/00207166808803030

Kortteinen, H., Närhi, V., & Ahonen, T. (2009). Does IQ matter in adolescents' reading disability? Learning and Individual Differences, 19, 257-261. DOI:10.1016/j.lindif.2009.01.003

Kuo, L. J., & Anderson, R. C. (2006). Morphological Awareness and Learning to Read: A

Cross-Language Perspective. Educational Psychologist, 41, 161-180.

DOI:10.1207/s15326985ep4103_3

Kyle, F., Kujala, J., Richardson, U., Lyytinen, H., & Goswami, U. (2013). Assessing the Effectiveness of Two Theoretical Motivated Computer-Assisted Reading Interventions in the United Kingdom: GG Rime and GG Phoneme. Reading Research Quarterly, 48, 61-76. DOI:10.1002/rrq.038

Laks, B. (1995). A connectionist account of French syllabification. Lingua, 95, 51-76. DOI:10.1016/0024-3841(95)90101-9

Landerl, K. (2000). Influences of orthographic consistency and reading instruction on the development of nonword reading skills. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 15, 239-257. DOI:10.1007/BF03173177

Landerl, K., & Reitsma, P. (2005). Phonological and morphological consistency in the acquisition of vowel duration spelling in Dutch and German. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 92, 322-344. DOI:10.1016/J.Jecp.2005.04.005

Landerl, K., & Wimmer, H. (2000). Deficits in phoneme segmentation are not the core problem of dyslexia: Evidence from German and English children. Applied Psycholinguistics, 21, 243-262. DOI:10.1017/S0142716400002058

Landerl, K., & Wimmer, H. (2008). Development of word reading fluency and spelling in a consistent orthography: An 8-year follow-up. Journal of Educational Psychology, 100, 150-161. DOI:10.1037/0022-0663.100.1.150

Landerl, K., Ramus F., Moll K., Lyytinen, H., Leppanen, P. H. T., Lohvansuu, K., … Schulte-Körne, G. (2013). Predictors of developmental dyslexia in European orthographies with varying complexity. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 54, 686-694. DOI:10.1111/jcpp.12029

Landerl, K., Wimmer, H., & Frith, U. (1997). The impact of orthographic consistency on dyslexia: a German-English comparison. Cognition, 63, 315-34. DOI:10.1016/S0010-0277(97)00005-X

(11)

Lee, L. W. (2008). Development and validation of a reading-related assessment battery in Malay for the purpose of dyslexia assessment. Annals of Dyslexia, 58, 37-57. DOI: 10.1007/s11881-007-0011-0

Lee, W. L., & Wheldall, K. (2011). Acquisition of Malay Word Recognition Skills: Lessons From Low-progress Early Readers. Dyslexia, 17, 19-37. DOI:10.1002/dys.421

Leikin, M., & Hagit, E. Z. (2006). Morphological processing in Adult Dyslexia. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 35, 471-490. DOI:10.1007/S10936-006-9025-8

Leinonen, S., Müller, K., Leppänen, P. H. T., Aro, M., Ahonen, T., & Lyytinen, H. (2001). Heterogeneity in adult dyslexic readers: Relating processing skills to the speed and accuracy of oral text reading. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 14, 265-296. DOI:10.1023/A:1011117620895

Lepola, J., Poskiparta, E., Laakkonen, E., & Niemi, P. (2005). Development of and relationship between phonological and motivational processes and naming speed in predicting word

recognition in Grade 1. Scientific Studies of Reading, 9, 367-399.

DOI:10.1207/s1532799xssr0904_3

Leppänen, U., Niemi, P., Aunola, K., & Nurmi, J. -E. (2006). Development of Reading and Spelling Finnish From Preschool to Grade 1 and 2. Scientific Studies of Reading, 10, 3-30. DOI:10.1207/s1532799xssr1001_2

Lété, B., Peereman, R., & Fayol, M. (2008). Consistency and word-frequency effects on word spelling among first- to fifth-grade French children: a regression based study. Journal of Memory and Language, 58, 962-977. DOI:10.1016/j.jml.2008.01.001

Levelt, C., & Van de Vijver, R. (1998). Syllable types in cross-linguistic and developmental grammars. Paper given at the Third Biannual Utrecht Phonology Workshop, Utrecht. In Frota, S., & Vigário, M. (2001). On the correlates of rhythmic distinctions: The European/Brazilian Portuguese case. Probus, 13, 247-275. DOI:10.1515/prbs.2001.005

Lewis, M. P., Simons, G. F., & Fennig, C. D. (Eds.) (2013). Ethnologue: Languages of the World,

Seventeenth edition. Dallas, Texas: SIL International. Online version:

http://www.ethnologue.com.

Loukina, A., Kochanski, G., Rosner, B., Keane, E., & Shih, C. (2011). Rhythm measures and dimensions of durational variation in speech. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 129. 3258-3270. DOI:10.1121/1.3559709

Lovett, M. W., Steinbach, K. A., & Frijters, J. C. (2000). Remediating the Core Deficits of Developmental Reading Disability A Double-Deficit Perspective. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 33, 334-358. DOI:10.1177/002221940003300406

Lovio, R., Halttunen, A., Lyytinen, H., Näätänen, R., & Kujala, T. (2012). Reading skill and neural processing accuracy improvement after a 3-hour intervention in preschoolers with difficulties in reading-related skills. Brain Research, 1448, 42-55. DOI:10.1016/j.brainres.2012.01.071 Lyon, G. R., Shaywitz, S. E., & Shaywitz, B. A. (2003). A definition of dyslexia. Annals of Dyslexia,

53, 1-14. DOI:10.1007/s11881-003-0001-9

Lyytinen, H., Aro, M., Holopainen, L., Leiwo, M., Lyytinen, P., & Tolvanen, A. (2006). Children’s language development and reading acquisition in a highly transparent orthography. In R. M. Joshi & P. G. Aaron (Eds.), Handbook of orthography and literacy (pp. 47-62). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

(12)

Lyytinen, H., Erskine, J., Ahonen, T., Aro, M., Eklund, K., Guttorm, T., ... & Viholainen, H. (2008). Early identification and prevention of dyslexia: Results from a prospective follow-up study of children at familial risk for dyslexia. In G. Reid, A. Fawcett, F. Manis, & L. Siegel (Eds.), The SAGE Handbook of Dyslexia (pp. 121-146). London, UK: Sage Publishers.

Lyytinen, H., Erskine, J., Hämäläinen, J., Torppa, M., & Ronimus, M. (2015). Dyslexia—Early identification and prevention: Highlights from the Jyväskylä longitudinal study of dyslexia. Current Developmental Disorders Reports, 2, 330-338. DOI:10.1007/s40474-015-0067-1 Lyytinen, H., Erskine, J., Kujala, J., Ojanen, E., & Richardson, U. (2009). In search of a

science-based application: A learning tool for reading acquisition. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 50, 668-675. DOI:10.1111/j.1467-9450.2009.00791.x

Lyytinen, H., Leinonen, S., Nikula, M., Aro, M,, Leiwo, M. (1995). In search of the core features of dyslexia: observations concerning dyslexia in the highly orthographically regular Finnish language. In V. W. Berninger (Ed.), The varieties of orthographic knowledge II: relationships to phonology, reading, and writing (pp. 177-204). Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer Academic Publishers.

Lyytinen, P., & Lyytinen, H. (2004). Growth and predictive relations of vocabulary and inflectional morphology in children with and without familial risk for dyslexia. Applied Psycholinguistics, 25, 397-411. DOI:10.1017/S0142716404001183

Lyytinen, H., Ronimus, M., Alanko, A., Poikkeus, A.-M., & Taanila, M. (2007). Early identification of dyslexia and the use of computer game-based practice to support reading acquisition. Nordic Psychology, 59, 109-126. DOI:10.1027/1901-2276.59.2.109

Mahony, D., Singson, M., & Mann, V. (2000). Reading ability and sensitivity to morphological

relations. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 12, 191-218.

DOI:10.1023/A:1008136012492

Manis, F. R., Seidenberg, M. S., Doi, L. M., McBride-Chang, C., & Peterson, A. (1996). On the basis of two subtypes of developmental dyslexia. Cognition, 58, 157-195. DOI:10.1016/0010-0277(95)00679-6

Mann, V. (2000). Introduction to special issue on morphology and the acquisition of alphabetic writing systems. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 12, 143-147. DOI:10.1023/A:1008190908857

Mann, V., & Wimmer, H. (2002). Phoneme awareness and pathways into literacy: A comparison of German and American children. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 15, 653-682. DOI:10.1023/A:102098470

Marcel, T. (1980). Phonological awareness and phonological representation: Investigation of a specific spelling problem. In U. Frith (Ed.), Cognitive processes in spelling (pp.373-403). San Diego, CA: Academia Press.

Marchand, Y., & Damper, R. I. (2000). A multistrategy approach to improving pronunciation by analogy. Computational Linguistics, 26, 195-219. DOI:10.1162/089120100561674

Marchand, Y., & Damper, R. I. (2007). Can syllabification improve pronunciation by analogy of English? Natural Language Engineering, 13, 1-24. DOI:10.1017/S1351324905004043 Marinus, E., & De Jong, P. F. (2010). Variability in the word-reading performance of dyslexic

readers: effects of letter length, phoneme length and digraph presence. Cortex, 46, 1259-1271. DOI:10.1016/j.cortex.2010.06.005

(13)

Martens, V. E. G., & De Jong, P. F. (2006). The effect of word length on lexical decision in dyslexic

and normal reading children. Brain and Language, 98, 140-149.

DOI:10.1016/j.bandl.2006.04.003

Melby-Lervåg, M. Halaas Lyster, S.-A., Hulme, C (2012). Phonological skills and their role in learning to read: A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 138, 322-352. DOI:10.1037/a0026744

Moeliono, A. M., & Dardjowidjojo, S. (Eds.). (1988). Tata bahasa baku bahasa Indonesia. Departemen Pendidikan dan Kebudayaan, Republik Indonesia.

Moll, K., Ramus, F., Bartling, J., Bruder, J., Kunze, S., Neuhoff, N., ... Landerl, K. (2014). Cognitive mechanisms underlying reading and spelling development in five European orthographies. Learning and Instruction, 29, 65-77. DOI:10.1016/j.learninstruc.2013.09.003

Morfidi, E., Van der Leij, A., De Jong, P. F., Scheltinga, F., & Bekebrede, J. (2007). Reading in two orthographies: A cross-linguistic study of Dutch average and poor readers who learn English as a second language. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 20, 753-784. DOI:10.1007/s11145-006-9035-9

Muter, V., Hulme, C., Snowling, M. J., & Stevenson, J. (2004). Phonemes, rimes, vocabulary, and grammatical skills as foundations of early reading development: Evidence from a longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 40, 665-681. DOI:10.1037/0012-1649.40.5.665

Nagy, W. E., Carlisle, J. F., & Goodwin, A. P. (2013). Morphological Knowledge and Literacy Acquisition. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 47, 3-12. DOI:10.1177/0022219413509967 Nationaal Referentiecentrum Dyslexie (2013). Protocol Dyslexie Diagnostiek en Behandeling 2.0.

[Protocol for Diagnotics and Treatment of Dyslexia 2.0] (original edition Blomert, L., 2006). http://www.nrd.nu/images/downloads/protocol_dyslexie_%20diagnostiek_en_behandeling_2 .0.pdf. Accessed 17 March 2016.

Nichols, J. & Bickel, B. (2013). Locus of Marking: Whole-language Typology. In M. S. Dryer & M. Haspelmath (Eds.), The World Atlas of Language Structures. Munich: Max Planck Digital Library.

Niemi, J., Laine, M., & Tuominen, J. (1994) Cognitive morphology in Finnish: Foundations of a

new model. Language and Cognitive Processes, 9, 423-446.

DOI:423-446.10.1080/01690969408402126

Niessen, M., Frith, U., Reitsma, P., & Öhngren, B. (2000). Learning disorders as a barrier to human development 1995-1999. Evaluation report. Technical Committee COST Social Sciences. In Seymour, P. H. K., Aro, M., & Erskine, J. M. (2003). Foundation literacy acquisition in European orthographies. British Journal of Psychology, 94, 143-174.

Norton, E. S., & Wolf, M. (2012). Rapid automatized naming (RAN) and reading fluency: Implications for understanding and treatment of reading disabilities. Annual Review of Psychology, 63, 427-452. DOI:10.1146/annurev-psych-120710-100431

Ojanen, E., Ronimus, M., Ahonen, T., Chansa-Kabali, T., February, P., Jere-Folotiya, J., … & Lyytinen, H. (2015). GraphoGame - a catalyst for multi-level promotion of literacy in diverse contexts. Frontiers in Psychology, 6: 671. DOI :10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00671

Ŏney, B., & Durgunoğlu, A. Y. (1997). Beginning to read in Turkish: A phonologically transparent orthography. Applied Psycholinguistics, 18, 1-15. DOI:10.1017/S014271640000984X

(14)

Öney, B., & Goldman, S. R. (1984). Decoding and comprehension skills in Turkish and English: Effects of the regularity of grapheme-phoneme correspondences. Journal of Educational Psychology, 76, 557-568. DOI: 10.1037/0022-0663.76.4.557

Papadopoulos, T. C., Georgiou, G. K., & Kendeou, P. (2009). Investigating the double-deficit hypothesis in Greek: Findings from a longitudinal study. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 42, 528-547. DOI:10.1177/0022219409338745

Patel, T. K., Snowling, M. J., & De Jong, P. F. (2004). A cross-linguistic comparison of children learning to read in English and Dutch. Journal of Educational Psychology, 96, 785-797. DOI:10.1037/0022-0663.96.4.785

Pennington, B. F. (2006). From single to multiple deficit models of developmental disorders. Cognition, 101, 385-413. DOI:10.1016/j.cognition.2006.04.008

Pennington, B. F., & Lefly, D. L. (2001). Early reading development in children at family risk for dyslexia. Child Development, 72, 816-833. DOI:10.1111/1467-8624.00317

Pennington, B. F., Cardoso-Martins, C., Green, P. A., & Lefly, D. L. (2001). Comparing the Phonological and double deficit hypotheses for developmental dyslexia. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 14, 707-755. DOI:10.1023/A:101223901

Pennington, B. F., Santerre-Lemmon, L., Rosenberg, J., MacDonald, B., Boada, R., Friend, A, ... & Olson, R. K. (2012). Individual prediction of dyslexia by single versus multiple deficit models. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 121, 212-224. DOI:10.1037/a0025823

Perfetti, C. A. (1985). Reading ability. New York: Oxford University Press.

Perfetti, C. (2007). Reading Ability: Lexical Quality to Comprehension. Scientific Studies of Reading, 11, 357-383. DOI:10.1080/10888430701530730

Perfetti, C. A., & Harris, L. N. (2013). Universal reading processes are modulated by language

and writing system. Language Learning and Development, 9, 296-316.

DOI:10.1080/15475441.2013.

Perfetti, C. A., & Hart, L. (2001). The lexical quality hypothesis. In L. Verhoeven, C. Elbro, & P. Reitsma (Eds.), Precursors of functional literacy (pp. 189-214). Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.

Perfetti, C., Cao, F., & Booth, J. (2013). Specialization und Universals in the Development of Reading Skill: How Chinese Research Informs a Universal Science of Reading. Scientific Studies of Reading, 17, 5-21. DOI:10.1080/10888438.2012.689786

Perfetti, C. A., Zhang, S., & Berent, I. (1992). Reading in English and Chinese: Evidence for a “universal” phonological principle. In R. Frost & L. Katz (Eds.), Orthography, phonology, morphology, and meaning (pp. 227-248). Amsterdam, The Netherlands: North-Holland. Perry, C., Ziegler, J. C., & Zorzi, M. (2007). Nested Incremental Modeling in the Development of

Computational Theories: The CDP+ Model of Reading Aloud. Psychological Review, 114, 273-315. DOI:10.1037/0033-295X.114.2.273

Perry, C., Ziegler, J. C., & Zorzi, M. (2010). Beyond single syllables: Large-scale modeling of reading aloud with the Connectionist Dual Process (CDP++) model. Cognitive Psychology, 61, 106-151. DOI:10.1016/j.cogpsych.2010.04.001

Peterson, R. L., & Pennington, B. F. (2012). Developmental dyslexia. The Lancet, April 17, 1-12. DOI:10.1016/S0140-6736(12)60198-6

Peterson, R. L., & Pennington, B. F. (2015). Developmental dyslexia. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 11, 283-307. DOI:10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032814-112842

(15)

Pols, L. C. W., & Schouten, M. E. H. (1978). Identification of deleted consonants. The Journal the Acoustical Society of America, 64, 1333-1337. DOI:10.1121/1.382100

Porpodas, C. D., Pantelis, S. N., & Hantziou, E. (1990). Phonological and lexical encoding processes in beginning readers: Effects of age and word characteristics. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 2, 197-208. DOI:10.1007/BF00257971

Prentice, D. J. (1987). Malay (Indonesian and Malaysian). In B. Comrie (Ed.), The world’s major languages. London: Croom Helm.

Prinzmetal, W., Treiman, R., & Rho, S. H. (1986). How to see a reading unit. Journal of Memory and Language, 25, 461-475. DOI:10.1016/0749-596X(86)90038-0

Protopapas, A., & Vlahou, E. L. (2009). A comparative quantitative analysis of Greek orthographic transparency. Behavior Research Methods, 41, 991-1008. DOI:10.3758/BRM.41.4.991 Puolakanaho, A., Ahonen, T., Aro, M., Eklund, K., Leppänen, P. H. T., Poikkeus, A.-M., . . .

Lyytinen, H. (2008). Developmental links of very early phonological and language skills to the second grade reading outcomes: Strong to accuracy but only minor to fluency. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 41, 353-370. DOI:10.1177/0022219407311747

Quémart, P., & Casalis, S. (2015). Visual processing of derivational morphology in children with developmental dyslexia: Insights from masked priming. Applied Psycholinguistics, 36, 345-376. DOI:10.1017/S014271641300026X

Quémart, P., Casalis, S., & Colé, P. (2011). The role of form and meaning in the processing of written morphology: A priming study in French developing readers. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 109, 478-496. DOI:10.1016/j.jecp.2011.02.008

Rack, J. P., Snowling, M., & Olson, R. K. (1992). The nonword reading deficit in developmental dyslexia: A review. Reading Research Quarterly, 27, 29-53. DOI: 10.2307/747832

Ramus, F., Nespor, M., & Mehler, J. (1999). Correlates of linguistic rhythm in the speech signal. Cognition, 73, 265-292. DOI:10.1016/S0010-0277(99)00058-X

Ramus, F., Rosen, S., Dakin, S. C., Day, B. L., Castellote, J. M., White, S., & Frith, U. (2003). Theories of developmental dyslexia: insights from a multiple case study of dyslexic adults. Brain, 126, 841-865. DOI:10.1093/brain/awg076

Raveh, M., & Schiff, R. (2008). Visual and auditory morphological priming in adults with

developmental dyslexia. Scientific Study of Reading, 12, 221-252.

DOI:10.1080/10888430801917068

Raven, J. C., & Court, J. H. (1962). Coloured progressive matrices. London: HK Lewis.

Read, C. (1975). Children’s categorization of speech sounds in English (NCTE Research Rep.

No. 17). Urbana, IL: National Council of Teachers of English.

Regtvoort, A., Zijlstra, H., & Van der Leij, A. (2013). The effectiveness of a 2‐year supplementary tutor‐assisted computerized intervention on the reading development of beginning readers at risk for reading difficulties: A randomized controlled trial. Dyslexia, 19, 256-280. DOI:10.1002/dys.1465

Reiter, A., Tucha, O., & Lange, K. W. (2004). Executive Functions in children with dyslexia. Dyslexia, 11, 116-131. DOI:10.1002/dys.289

Richardson, U., & Lyytinen, H. (2014). The GraphoGame method: the theoretical and methodological background of the technology-enhanced environment for learning to read. Human Technology, 10, 39-60. DOI:10.17011/ht/urn.201405281859

(16)

Rissanen, J. (1984). Universal coding, information, prediction, and estimation. IEEE Transactions on Information Theory IT, 30, 629-636. DOI:10.1109/TIT.1984.1056936

Rouibah, A., & Taft, M. (2001). The role of syllabic structure in French visual word recognition. Memory and Cognition, 29, 373-381. DOI:10.3758/BF03194932

Sadeniemi, M., Kettunen, K., Lindh-Knuutila, T., & Honkela, T. (2008). Complexity of European Union Languages: A comparative approach. Journal of Quantitative Linguistics, 15, 185-211. DOI:10.1080/09296170801961843

Saine, N. L., Lerkkanen, M.-K., Ahonen, T., Tolvanen, A., & Lyytinen, H. (2010). Predicting word-level reading fluency outcomes in three contrastive groups: Remedial and computer-assisted remedial reading intervention, and mainstream instruction. Learning and Individual Differences, 20, 402-414. DOI:10.1016/j.lindif.2010.06.004

Saine, N. L., Lerkkanen, M.-K., Ahonen, T., Tolvanen, A., & Lyytinen, H. (2011). Computer‐ assisted remedial reading intervention for school beginners at risk for reading disability. Child Development, 82, 1013-1028. DOI:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01580.x

Sardjunani, N., & Suryadi, A. (2005). Public-Private Financing Schemes for Early Child Education Services: Lessons Learned from Indonesia. PowerPoint presentation at symposium on Early Child Development: A Priority for Sustained Economic Growth and Equity, Washington, D.C., September 28. World Bank.

Sardjunani, N., Suryadi, A., & Dunkelberg, E. (2007). Indonesia: Public Financing of Block Grants for Privately Delivered Services. In M. E. Young, & L. M. Richardson (Eds.), Early Child Development, From Measurement to Action: A priority for Growth and Equity (pp. 233-252). Washington, D.C.: World Bank.

Schiff, R, & Raveh, M. (2007). Deficient Morphological Processing in Adults with Developmental Dyslexia: Another Barrier to Efficient Word Recognition? Dyslexia, 13, 110-129. DOI:10.1002/dys.322

Schiff, R. (2012). Shallow and Deep Orthographies in Hebrew: The Role of Vowelization in Reading Development for Unvowelized Scripts. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 41, 409-424. DOI:10.1007/S10936-011-9198-7

Schiff, R., Katzir T., & Shoshan N. (2013), Reading accuracy and speed of vowelized and unvowelized scripts among dyslexic readers of Hebrew: the road not taken. Annals of Dyslexia, 63, 171-185. DOI:10.1007/S11881-012-0078-0

Schmalz, X., Marinus, E., Coltheart, M., & Castles, A. (2015). Getting to the bottom of orthographic depth. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 22, 1614-1629. DOI:10.3758/s13423-015-0835-2

Schreuder, R., & Van Bon, W. H. J. (1989). Phonemic analysis: Effects of word properties. Journal of Research in Reading, 12, 59-78. DOI:10.1111/j.1467-9817.1989.tb00303.x

Serrano, F., & Defior, D. (2012). Spanish dyslexic spelling abilities: the case of consonant clusters. Journal of Research in Reading, 35, 169-182. DOI:10.1111/j.1467-987.2010.01454.x Seymour, P. H. K. (1990). Developmental dyslexia. In M. W. Eysenck (Ed.), Cognitive

Psychology: An international review (pp. 135-196). Chichester: Wiley.

Seymour, P. H. K. (1997). Foundations of orthographic development. In C. Perfetti, L. Rieben, & M. Fayol (Eds.), Learning to spell (pp. 319-337). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Seymour, P. H. K. (1999). Cognitive architecture of early reading. In I. Lundberg, F. E Tønnessen, & I. Austad (Eds.), Dyslexia: Advances in theory and practice (pp. 59-73). Dordrecht: Kluwer.

(17)

Seymour, P. H. K., Aro, M., & Erskine, J. M. (2003). Foundation literacy acquisition in European

orthographies. British Journal of Psychology, 94, 143-174.

DOI:10.1348/000712603321661859

Shankweiler, D., Crain, S., Katz, L., Fowler, A. E., Liberman, A. E., Brady, S. A., … Shaywitz, B. A. (1995). Cognitive profiles of reading-disabled children: Comparisons of language skills in phonology, morphology, and syntax. Psychological Science, 6, 149-156. DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.1995.tb00324.x

Shannon, C. E. (1948). A mathematical theory of communication. Bell System Technical Journal, 27, 379-423, 623-656. DOI:10.1002/j.1538-7305.1948.tb01338.x

Share, D. L. (1995). Phonological recoding and self-teaching: Sine qua non of reading acquisition. Cognition, 55, 151-218. DOI: 10.1016/0010-0277(94)00645-2

Share, D. L. (2008). On the Anglocentricities of current reading research and practice: The perils of overreliance on a “outlier” orthography. Psychological Bulletin, 134, 584-615. DOI:10.1037/0033-2909.134.4.584

Shaywitz, S. E. (1998). Dyslexia. New England Journal of Medicine, 338, 307-312. DOI:10.1056/NEJM199801293380507

Shaywitz, S. E. (1998). Dyslexia. New England Journal of Medicine, 338, 307-312.

Shaywitz, S. E., Morris, R., & Shaywitz, B. A. (2008). The Education of Dyslexic Children from Childhood to Young Adulthood. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 451-475. DOI:10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093633

Shosted, R. (2006). Correlating complexity: A typological approach. Linguistic Typology, 10, 1-40. DOI:10.1515/LINGTY.2006.001

Shu, H., McBride-Chang, C., Wu, S., & Liu, H. Y. (2006). Understanding Chinese developmental dyslexia: Morphological awareness as a core cognitive construct. Journal of Educational Psychology, 89, 122-133. DOI:10.1037/0022-0663.98.1.122

Siegel, L. S. (2008). Morphological awareness Skills of English Language Learners and Children

With Dyslexia. Topics in Language Disorders, 28, 15-27.

DOI:10.1097/01.adt.0000311413.75804.60

Sneddon, J. (2003). The Indonesian language: Its history and role in modern society. Sydney, Australia: University of New South Wales.

Snellings, P., Van der Leij, A., Blok, H., & De Jong, P. F. (2010). Reading fluency and speech perception of beginning readers with persistent reading problems: the perception of initial stop consonants and consonant clusters. Annals of Dyslexia, 60, 151-174. DOI:10.1007/S11881-010-0039-4

Snowling, M. J. (1995). Phonological processing and developmental dyslexia. Journal of Research in Reading, 18, 132-138. DOI:10.1111/j.1467-9817.1995.tb00079.x

Soderberg, C. D., & Olson, K. S. (2008). Illustrations of the IPA: Indonesian. Journal of the International Phonetic Association, 38, 209-213. DOI:10.1017/S0025100308003320

Soodla, P., Lerkkanen, M. -K., Kikas, E., Niemi, P., & Nurmi, J.-E. (2015). Does early reading instruction promote the rate of acquisition? A comparison of two orthographies. Learning and Instruction, 38, 14-23. DOI:10.1037/t00118-000

(18)

Spencer, L. H., & Hanley, J. R. (2003). Effects of orthographic transparency on reading and phoneme awareness in children learning to read in Wales. British Journal of Psychology, 94, 1-28. DOI:10.1348/000712603762842075

Spinelli, D., De Luca, M., Di Filippo, G., Mancini, M., Martelli, M., & Zoccolotti, P. (2005). Length effect in word naming in reading: Role of reading experience and reading deficit in Italian readers. Developmental Neuropsychology, 27, 217-235. DOI:10.1207/s15326942dn2702_2 Sprenger-Charolles, L., & Colé, P. (2003). Lecture et Dyslexie. Paris: Dunod.

Sprenger-Charolles, L., & Siegel, L. S. (1997). A longitudinal study of the effects of syllabic structure on the development of reading and spelling in French. Applied Psycholinguistics, 18, 485-505. DOI:10.1017/S014271640001095X

Sprenger-Charolles, L., Colé, P., Lacert, P., & Serniclaes, W. (2000). On subtypes of developmental dyslexia: Evidence from processing time and accuracy scores. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 54, 88-104. DOI:10.1037/h0087332

Sprenger-Charolles, L., Siegel, L. S. (1997). A longitudinal study of the effects of syllabic structure on the development of reading and spelling in French. Applied Psycholinguistics, 18, 485-505. DOI:10.1017/S014271640001095X

Sprenger-Charolles, L., Siegel, L. S., Jiménez, J. E., & Ziegler, C.J. (2011). Prevalence and Reliability of Phonological Surface, and Mixed Profiles in Dyslexia: A Review of Studies Conducted in Languages Varying in Orthographic Depth. Scientific Studies of Reading, 15, 498-521. DOI:10.1080/10888438.2010.524463

Stanovich, K. E., Siegel, L. S., & Gottardo, A. (1997). Converging evidence for phonological and surface subtypes of reading disability. Journal of Educational Psychology, 89, 114-127. DOI:10.1037/0022-0663.89.1.114

Steffler, D. J., Varnhagen, C. K., Friesen, C. K., & Treiman, R. (1998). There’s more to children’s spelling than the errors they make: Strategic and automatic processes for one-syllable words. Journal of Education & Psychology, 90, 492-505. DOI:10.1037/0022-0663.90.3.492

Struiksma, A. J. C. (2003). Lezen gaat voor [Reading First]. Amsterdam: VU Uitgeverij.

Stump, G. T. (2001). Inflection. In A. Spencer & A. Zwicky (Eds.), The handbook of morphology (pp. 13-43). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley and Sons.

Sunseth, K., & Bowers, P. G. (2002). Rapid naming and phonemic awareness: Contributions to reading, spelling, and orthographic knowledge. Scientific Studies of Reading, 6, 401-429. Taft, M. (1979). Lexical access via an orthographic code: The basic orthographic syllabic structure

(BOSS). Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior, 18, 21-39. DOI:10.1016/S0022-5371(79)90544-9

Tellegen, P. J. & Laros, J. A. (2011). SON-R 6-40. Snijders-Oomen niet-verbale intelligentietest. I. Verantwoording. Amsterdam: Hogrefe uitgevers

Tilanus, E. A. T., Segers, E., & Verhoeven, L. (2013). Diagnostic profiles of children with developmental dyslexia in a transparent orthography. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 34, 4194-4202. DOI:10.1016/j.ridd.2013.08.039

Tilanus, E. A., Segers, E., & Verhoeven, L. (2016). Responsiveness to intervention in children with dyslexia. Dyslexia, 22, 214-232. DOI:10.1016/j.ridd.2013.08.039

Tilsen, S., & Arvaniti, A. (2013). Speech rhythm analysis with decomposition of the amplitude envelope: Characterizing rhythmic patterns within and across languages. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 134, 628-639. DOI:10.1121/1.4807565

(19)

Torgesen, J. K., Wagner, R. K., Rashotte, C. A., Burgess, S., & Hecht, S. (1997). Contributions of phonological awareness and rapid automatic naming ability to the growth of word-reading skills in second-to fifth-grade children. Scientific studies of reading, 1, 161-185.

Torgesen, J. K., Wagner, R. K., Rashotte, C. A., Rose, E., Lindamood, P., Conway, T., & Garvan, C. (1999). Preventing reading failure in young children with phonological processing disabilities: Group and individual responses to instruction. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 579-593. DOI:10.1037/0022-0663.91.4.579

Torppa, M., Lyytinen, P., Erskine, J., Eklund, K., & Lyytinen, H. (2010). Language Development, Literacy Skills, and Predictive Connections to Reading in Finnish Children With and Without

Familial Risk for Dyslexia. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 43, 308-321.

DOI:10.1177/0022219410369096

Torppa, M., Parrila, R., Niemi, P., Lerkkanen, M.-K., Poikkeus, A.-M., & Nurmi, J.-E. (2013). The double deficit hypothesis in the transparent Finnish orthography: a longitudinal study from kindergarten to Grade 2. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 26, 1353-1380. DOI:10.1007/s11145-012-9423-2

Traficante, D., Marcolini, S., Luci, A., Zoccolotti, P., & Burani, C. (2011). How do roots and suffixes influence reading of pseudowords: A study of Italian children with and without dyslexia. Language and Cognitive Processes, 26, 777-793. DOI:10.1080/01690965.2010.496553 Treiman, R. (1991). Children’s Spelling Errors on Syllable-Initial Consonant Clusters. Journal of

Educational Psychology, 83, 346-360. DOI:10.1037/0022-0663.83.3.346 Treiman, R. (1993). Beginning to spell. New York: Oxford Press University.

Treiman, R., & Weatherston, S. (1992). Effects of linguistic structure on children’s ability to isolate initial consonants. Journal of Educational Psychology, 84, 174-181. DOI:10.1037/0022-0663.84.2.174

Treiman, R., Mullennix, J., Bijeljac-Babic, R., & Richmond-Welty, E. D. (1995). The special role of rimes in the description, use, and acquisition of English orthography. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 124, 107-136. DOI:10.1037/0096-3445.124.2.107 Trenta, M., Benassi, M., Di Filippo, G., Pontillo, M., & Zoccolotti, P. (2013). Developmental

dyslexia in a regular orthography: Can the reading profile be reduced to strategic control? Cognitive Neuropsychology, 30, 147-171. DOI:10.1080/02643294.2013.814569

Tressoldi, P. E., Stella, G., & Faggella, M. (2001). The development of reading speed in Italians with dyslexia: A longitudinal study. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 34, 414-417. DOI:10.1177/002221940103400503

Tyler, A., & Nagy, W. (1989). The acquisition of English derivational morphology. Journal of Memory and Language, 28, 649-667. DOI:10.1016/0749-596X(89)90002-8

UNESCO (2016). Indonesia. http://uis.unesco.org/en/country/ID. Accessed 20 December 2017. U.S. Department of Education (2011). National Assessment of Educational Progress (NAEP)

Reading Assessments. Washington, DC: Institute of Education Sciences, National Center for Education Statistics.

Vaessen, A., Bertrand, D., Tóth, D., Csépe, V., Faísca, L, & Reis, A. (2010). Cognitive Development of Fluent Word Reading Does Not Qualitatively Differ Between Transparent and

Opaque Orthographies. Journal of Educational Psychology, 102, 827-842.

(20)

Vaessen, A. A., & Blomert, L. (2010). Long-term cognitive dynamics of fluent reading

development. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 105, 213-231.

DOI:10.1016/j.jecp.2009.11.005

Vaessen, A., Gerretsen, P., & Blomert, L. (2009). Naming speed problems do not reflect a second independent core deficit in dyslexia: Double-deficits explored. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 103, 202-221. DOI:10.1016/j.jecp.2008.12.004

Van Bergen, E., de Jong, P. F., Plakas, A., Maassen, B., & van der Leij, A. (2012). Child and parental literacy levels within families with a history of dyslexia. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 53, 28-36. DOI:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2011.02418.x

Van den Bos, K. P. (2003). Snel Serieel Benoemen; Experimentele versie. Groningen: Rijksuniversiteit Groningen.

Van den Bosch, A. P. J., Content, A., Daelemans, W. M. P., & De Gelder, B. L. M. F. (1994). Measuring the complexity of writing systems. Journal of Quantitative Linguistics, 1, 178-188. DOI:10.1080/09296179408590015

Van der Leij, A., Van Bergen, E., Van Zuijen, T., De Jong, P., Maurits, N., & Maassen, B. (2013). Precursors of Dyslexia: An Overview of the Longitudinal Dutch Dyslexia Program Study. Dyslexia, 19, 191-213. DOI:10.1002/dys.1463

Van Gorp, K., Segers, E., & Verhoeven, L. (2016). Enhancing Decoding Efficiency in Poor Readers via a Word Identification Game. Reading Research Quarterly, 52, 105-123. DOI:10.1002/rrq.156

Vartiainen, J., Aggujaro, S., Lehtonen, M., Hultén, A., Laine, M., & Salmelin, R. (2009). Neural dynamics of reading morphologically complex words. NeuroImage, 47, 2064-2072. DOI:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.06.002

Vellutino, F. R., Fletcher, J. M., Snowling, M. J., & Scanlon, D. M. (2004). Specific reading disability (dyslexia): what have we learned in the past four decades? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 2-40. DOI:10.1046/j.0021-9630.2003.00305.x

Verhoeven, L. (1993). Grafementoets. Toets voor Auditieve Synthese. Handleiding. Arnhem: Cito. Wagner, R. K., & Torgesen, J. K. (1987). The Nature of Phonological Processing and Its Causal

Role in the Acquisition of Reading Skills. Psychological Bulletin, 101, 192-212.

Wechsler, D. (1974). Wechsler intelligence scale for children, revised: WISC-R; manual. New York: Psychological Corporation.

Wimmer, H. (1993). Characteristics of developmental dyslexia in a regular writing system. Applied psycholinguistics, 14, 1-33. DOI:10.1017/S0142716400010122

Wimmer, H., & Goswami, U. (1994). The influence of orthographic consistency on reading development: Word recognition in English and German children. Cognition, 51, 91-103. DOI:10.1016/0010-0277(94)90010-8

Wimmer, H., & Hummer, P. (1990). How German speaking first graders read and spell: Doubts on the importance of the logographic stage. Applied Psycholinguistics, 11, 349-368. DOI:10.1017/S0142716400009620

Wimmer, H., & Schurz, M. (2010). Dyslexia in regular orthographies: Manifestation and causation. Dyslexia, 16, 283-299. DOI:10.1002/dys.411

Wimmer, H., Mayringer, H., & Landerl, L. (2000). The double-deficit hypothesis and difficulties in learning to read a regular orthography. Journal of Educational Psychology, 92, 668-680.

(21)

Winskel, H. (2013). Reading and writing in Southeast Asian languages. Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences, 97, 437-442. DOI:10.1016/j.sbspro.2013.10.256

Winskel H. & Lee, L. W. (2013). Learning to read and write in Malaysian/Indonesian: A transparent alphabetic orthography. In H. Winskel, & P. Padakannaya (Eds.), South and Southeast Asian Psycholinguistics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Winskel, H. & Widjaja, V. (2007). Phonological awareness, letter knowledge and literacy development in Indonesian beginner readers and spellers. Applied Psycholinguistics, 28, 21-43. DOI:10.1017/S0142716407070026

Wolf, M., & Bowers, P. G. (1999). The double-deficit hypothesis for the developmental dyslexias. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 415-438. DOI:10.1037/0022-0663.91.3.415

World Bank (2006). Indonesia - Early Childhood Education and Development Project. Washington, DC: World Bank.

http://documents.worldbank.org/curated/en/128861468283753678/Indonesia-Early-Childhood-Education-and-Development-Project. Accessed 20 December 2017.

Zevin, J. D., & Seidenberg, M. S. (2006). Simulating consistency effects and individual differences in nonword naming: A comparison of current models. Journal of Memory & Languages, 54, 145-160. DOI:10.1016/j.jml.2005.08.002

Ziegler, J. C., Bertrand, D., Tóth, D., Csépe, V., Reis, A., Faísca, L., ... & Blomert, L. (2010). Orthographic Depth and Its Impact on Universal Predictors of Reading: A Cross-Language Investigation. Psychological Science, 21, 551-559. DOI:10.1177/0956797610363406 Ziegler, J. C., Castel, C., Pech-Georgel, C., George, F., Alario, F. X., & Perry, C. (2008).

Developmental dyslexia and the Dual Route Model of Reading: Simulating Individual differences and subtypes. Cognition, 107, 151-178. DOI:10.1016/j.cognition.2007.09.004 Ziegler, J. C., & Goswami, U. (2005). Reading Acquisition, Developmental Dyslexia, and Skilled

Reading Across Languages: A Psycholinguistic Grain Size Theory. Psychological Bulletin, 131, 3-29. DOI:10.1037/0033-2909.131.1.3

Ziegler, J. C., Jacobs, A. M., & Stone, G. O. (1996). Statistical analysis of the bidirectional inconsistency of spelling and sound in French. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 28, 504-515. DOI:10.3758/BF03200539

Ziegler, J. C., Perry, C., & Coltheart, M. (2000) The DRC model of visual word recognition and reading aloud: An extension to German, European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 12, 413-430. DOI:10.1080/09541440050114570

Ziegler, J. C., Perry, C., Ma-Wyatt, A., Ladner, D., & Schulte-Körne, G. (2003). Developmental dyslexia in different languages: Language-specific or universal? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 86, 169-193. DOI:10.1016/S0022-0965(03)00139-5

Ziegler, J. C., Stone, G. O., & Jacobs, A. M. (1997). What’s the pronunciation for -OUGH and the spelling for /u/? A database for computing feedforward and feedback consistency in English. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 29, 600-618.

Žarić, G., González, G. F., Tijms, J., van der Molen, M. W., Blomert, L., & Bonte, M. (2014). Reduced neural integration of letters and speech sounds in dyslexic children scales with individual differences in reading fluency. PloS One, 9, DOI:e110337

Referenties

GERELATEERDE DOCUMENTEN

To analyse individual cognitive profiles and to test the fit of single- and multiple- deficit models, the researchers first counted the cognitive deficit(s) in individual

A linear regression analysis was conducted to determine whether the relationship between pre-test phonological skills and post-test reading and decoding fluency was

Although large-scale randomized controlled studies are needed to confirm the effectiveness of GraphoGame SI, our correlation results show that progress in the game (as

Yet, whereas the body of research focusing on East Asian languages is growing (i.e. Chinese, Japanese and Korean), still very little research has been conducted on

Other than for strictly personal use, it is not permitted to download or to forward/distribute the text or part of it without the consent of the author(s) and/or copyright

Yet, still very little research has been conducted into reading and spelling development in the languages of Southeast Asia, among which is the highly transparent Standard

De t-test-resultaten van de taken gericht op lezen en decoderen, foneemdeletie, dictee, passieve spelling en snel serieel benoemen van letters of cijfers lieten bij zowel

In 2007, she started working as a research assistant at the Developmental Psychology department of the University of Amsterdam, combining this with another bachelor’s