• No results found

Cover Page The handle http://hdl.handle.net/1887/51345

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Share "Cover Page The handle http://hdl.handle.net/1887/51345"

Copied!
7
0
0

Bezig met laden.... (Bekijk nu de volledige tekst)

Hele tekst

(1)

Cover Page

The handle http://hdl.handle.net/1887/51345 holds various files of this Leiden University dissertation

Author: Tsegaye, M.T.

Title: Plural Gender: Behavioral evidence for plural as a value of Cushitic gender with reference to Konso

Issue Date: 2017-07-05

(2)

7. References

Alario, F.-X., & Caramazza, A. (2002). The production of determiners: Evidence from French. Cognition, 82, 179-223

Black, P., & Shako O., (1973). Konso dictionary. Ms

Bordag, D., & Pechmann, T. (2008). Grammatical gender in speech production: Evidence from Czech. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 37, 69-85.

Caramazza, A. (1997). How many levels of processing are there in lexical access?. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 14, 177-208.

Caramazza, A., Miozzo, M., Costa, A., Schiller, N. O., & Alario, F.- X. (2001). A cross-linguistic investigation of determiner production. In E. Dupoux (Ed.), Language, Brain and Cognitive Development: Essays in Honor of Jacques Mehler (pp. 209–226).

Cambridge, MA: MIT Press

Central Statistics Agency of Ethiopia (1996). The 1994 National population and housing census.

Central Statistics Agency of Ethiopia (2009). The 2007 National population and housing census

Corbett, G. G. (1991). Gender. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.

Corbett, G. G. (2005). 30 Number of Genders. In World Atlas of Language Structures, ed. by Martin Haspelmath, Matthew S.

Dryer, David Gil and Bernard Comrie, pp. 126-129. Oxford:

Oxford University Press.

(3)

PLURALGENDER 224

Corbett, G. G. (2006). Agreement. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Corbettt, G. G. (2012). Features. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Corbett, G. G., & Hayward, R. J. (1987). Gender and number in Bayso. Lingua, 73, 1-28.

Costa, A., Kovacic, D., Fedorenko, E., & Caramazza, A. (2003). The gender congruency effect and the selection of freestanding and bound morphemes: evidence from Croatian. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 29, 1270-1282.

Costa, A., Sebastian-Galles, N., Miozzo, M., & Caramazza, A.

(1999). The gender congruity effect: Evidence from Spanish and Catalan. Language and Cognitive Processes, 14, 381– 391.

Cubelli, R., Lotto, L., Paolieri, D., Girelli, M. & Job, R. (2005).

Grammatical gender is selected in bare noun production: Evidence from the picture-word interference paradigm. Journal of Memory and Language 53: 42-59.

Dell, G. S. (1986). A spreading-activation theory of retrieval in sentence production. Psychological Review, 93, 283.

Garrett, M. F. (1982). Production of speech: Observations from normal and pathological language use. In A. W. Ellis (Ed.), Normality and pathology in cognitive functions (pp. 19–76).

London: Academic Press.

(4)

Hayward, R. J. (1979). Bayso revisited: some preliminary linguistic observations-II. Bulletin of the School of Oriental and African Studies, University of London, 42, 101-32.

Hayward, R.J. (1981). Nominal suffixes in Dirayta (Gidole). Bulletin of the School of Oriental and African Studies, University of London, 44, 126-144.

Hayward, R.J. (1984). The Arbore language: A first investigation including a vocabulary .kuschitische Sprachstudien 2. Hamburg:

Helmut Buske.

Jaeger, T. F., & Norcliffe, E. J. (2009). The cross-linguistic study of sentence production. Language and Linguistics Compass, 3, 866- 887.

Janssen, N., Schiller, N. O., & Alario, F. X. (2014). The selection of closed-class elements during language production: A reassessment of the evidence and a new look on new data. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 29, 695-708.

Jescheniak, J. D., Schriefers, H., & Lemhöfer, K. (2014). Selection of freestanding and bound gender-marking morphemes in speech production: A review. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 29, 684-694.

Miozzo, M., & Caramazza, A. (1999). The selection of determiners in noun phrase production. Journal of Experimental Psychology:

Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 25, 907–922.

Mous, M. (1993). A grammar of Iraqw (Cushitic Language Studies, 9) Hamburg: Helmut Buske.

(5)

PLURALGENDER 226

Mous, M. (2008). Number as an exponent of gender in Cushitic. In Frajzyngier, Z., & Shay, E. (Eds.), Interaction of morphology and syntax: Case studies in Afroasiatic (Typological Studies in Language 75, pp. 137-160). John Benjamins.

La Heij, W., Mak, P., Sander, J., & Willeboordse, E. (1998). The gender-congruency effect in picture-word tasks. Psychological Research, 61, 209-219.

Lemhöfer, K., Schriefers, H., & Jescheniak, J. D. (2006). The processing of free and bound gender-marked morphemes in speech production: Evidence from Dutch. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 32, 437-442.

Levelt, W. J. M. (1989). Speaking: From intention to articulation.

Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Levelt, W. J. M, Roelofs, A., & Meyer, A. S. (1999). A theory of lexical access in speech production. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 1–75.

Orkaydo, O. O. (2013). A grammar of Konso. Utrecht: LOT.

Pillinger, S., & Galboran, L. (1999). A Rendille dictionary: including a grammatical outline and an English-Rendille index. Cologne:

Köppe.

Savà, G. (2005). A Grammar of Ts' amakko . Cologne: Köppe.

Schiller, N. O. (2013). Psycholinguistic approaches to the investigation of grammatical gender in speech production: An overview and new data. In G. G. Corbett, (ed.), The Expression of Gender (pp. 161-190). Berlin, New York: De Gruyter.

(6)

Schiller, N. O., & Caramazza, A. (2002). The selection of grammatical features in word production: The case of plural nouns in German. Brain and Language, 81, 342–357.

Schiller, N. O., & Caramazza, A. (2003). Grammatical feature selection in noun phrase production: Evidence from German and Dutch. Journal of Memory and Language, 48, 169-194.

Schiller, N. O., & Costa, A. (2006). Different selection principles of freestanding and bound morphemes in language production. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 32, 1201-1207.

Schriefers, H. (1993). Syntactic processes in the production of noun phrases. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 19, 841-850.

Schriefers, H., & Teruel, E. (2000). Grammatical gender in noun phrase production: The gender interference effect in German. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26, 1368-1377.

Schriefers, H., Jescheniak, J. D., & Hantsch, A. (2002). Determiner selection in noun phrase production. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 28, 941–950.

Schriefers, H., Jescheniak, J. D., & Hantsch, A. (2005). Selection of gender-marked morphemes in speech production. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 31, 159-168.

(7)

PLURALGENDER 228

Tsegaye, M., Mous, M., & Schiller, N. (2013). Plural as a value of Cushitic gender: Evidence from gender congruency effect experiments in Konso (Cushitic). In G.G. Corbett (ed.), The Expression of Gender (pp. 191-214). Berlin, New York: De Gruyter.

Tsegaye, M., Mous, M., & Schiller, N. O. (submitted).

Psycholinguistic evidence for plural as a value of gender in Konso.

Van Berkum, J. J. (1997). Syntactic processes in speech production:

The retrieval of grammatical gender. Cognition, 64, 115-152.

Referenties

GERELATEERDE DOCUMENTEN

This dissertation presents a psycholinguistic study on the processing of gender and number in Konso, a Cushitic language of Ethiopia, in which there appears to be a third value

In the multiple- reference non-plural gender definite noun naming, increased competition was produced during bound morpheme selection due to the activation of

Table 28 Mean naming latencies (RTs) in ms and error rates in percentage (%e) by number and gender for null subject sentence

In the studies reported in the present dissertation, both picture-word interference and simple picture naming tasks were used to determine the status of the so-called

To account for the failure of replicating Schriefers’ (1993) result in the production of adjective + noun that involve the selection of a gender-marked bound morpheme,

If comparable gender congruency effects appear to be observed in naming plural and non-plural (masculine.. and feminine) gender definite nouns, plural is indeed

The finding of congruency effect only in the single-object picture naming condition, where there were different forms for different genders, has shown that the

In the multiple- reference non-plural gender definite noun naming, increased competition was produced during bound morpheme selection due to the activation of