• No results found

Postural control during reaching while sitting and general motor behaviour when learning to walk

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Share "Postural control during reaching while sitting and general motor behaviour when learning to walk"

Copied!
9
0
0

Bezig met laden.... (Bekijk nu de volledige tekst)

Hele tekst

(1)

University of Groningen

Postural control during reaching while sitting and general motor behaviour when learning to

walk

Boxum, Anke G.; La Bastide-Van Gemert, Sacha; Dijkstra, Linze-Jaap; Furda, Anna;

Reinders-Messelink, Heleen A.; Hadders-Algra, Mijna

Published in:

Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology DOI:

10.1111/dmcn.13931

IMPORTANT NOTE: You are advised to consult the publisher's version (publisher's PDF) if you wish to cite from it. Please check the document version below.

Document Version

Publisher's PDF, also known as Version of record

Publication date: 2019

Link to publication in University of Groningen/UMCG research database

Citation for published version (APA):

Boxum, A. G., La Bastide-Van Gemert, S., Dijkstra, L-J., Furda, A., Reinders-Messelink, H. A., & Hadders-Algra, M. (2019). Postural control during reaching while sitting and general motor behaviour when learning to walk. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 61(5), 555-562. https://doi.org/10.1111/dmcn.13931

Copyright

Other than for strictly personal use, it is not permitted to download or to forward/distribute the text or part of it without the consent of the author(s) and/or copyright holder(s), unless the work is under an open content license (like Creative Commons).

Take-down policy

If you believe that this document breaches copyright please contact us providing details, and we will remove access to the work immediately and investigate your claim.

Downloaded from the University of Groningen/UMCG research database (Pure): http://www.rug.nl/research/portal. For technical reasons the number of authors shown on this cover page is limited to 10 maximum.

(2)

DEVELOPMENTAL MEDICINE & CHILD NEUROLOGY ORIGINAL ARTICLE

Postural control during reaching while sitting and general motor

behaviour when learning to walk

ANKE G BOXUM1

|

SACHA LA BASTIDE-VAN GEMERT2

|

LINZE-JAAP DIJKSTRA1

|

ANNA FURDA1

|

HELEEN A REINDERS-MESSELINK3,4

|

MIJNA HADDERS-ALGRA1

1 University of Groningen, University Medical Center Groningen, Department of Paediatrics– Developmental Neurology, Groningen; 2 University of Groningen, University Medical Center Groningen, Department of Epidemiology, Groningen; 3 University of Groningen, University Medical Center Groningen, Department of Rehabilitation Medicine, Center for Rehabilitation, Groningen; 4 Rehabilitation Center ‘Revalidatie Friesland’, Beetsterzwaag, the Netherlands.

Correspondence to Mijna Hadders-Algra at Developmental Neurology, University Medical Center Groningen, Hanzeplein 1, 9713 GZ Groningen, the Netherlands. E-mail: m.hadders-algra@umcg.nl

PUBLICATION DATA

Accepted for publication 4th May 2018. Published online

ABBREVIATIONS

AIMS Alberta Infant Motor Scale IMP Infant Motor Profile

AIMTo study changes in muscular postural strategies and general motor behaviour during the transition to independent walking. Postural control was assessed at its two functional levels: (1) direction specificity, in which dorsal muscles are primarily activated when reaching forward; and (2) fine-tuning of direction specificity.

METHODIn an explorative longitudinal study, surface electromyograms of the arm, trunk, and neck muscles of 28 typically developing infants were recorded during reaching while sitting. Each infant was assessed in three developmental phases: during pull-to-stand (T0), first independent steps (T1), and 1 month after T1 (T2). Motor behaviour was assessed using the Infant Motor Profile (IMP). The effect on developmental outcome measures (postural parameters and IMP) of the developmental phases (T0, T1, T2) was estimated using linear mixed-effects models.

RESULTSNone of the postural parameters changed significantly over time. However, individual developmental trajectories showed infant-specific postural reorganizational changes. Total IMP score decreased between T0 and T1 (mean IMP score 95% and 91% respectively;p<0.001); between T1 and T2 IMP scores did not change (91% and 93%; p=0.073).

INTERPRETATIONTypically developing infants do not show consistent patterns of postural reorganization but show individual muscular strategies during the transition to independent walking. However, signs of reorganization of general motor behaviour are present.

Postural control is a prerequisite for many daily life activi-ties, especially for motor actions like reaching, sitting, standing, and walking. Hence, the development of postural control and motor behaviour is closely entangled. Both reaching development and the development of gross motor milestones are important for subsequent development of perception, cognition, and social interaction.1,2The impor-tant role of posture in reaching was illustrated, for exam-ple, by studies that demonstrated that infants who are not able to sit independently reach more frequently, success-fully, and unilaterally in a supported sitting position than in supine or a semi-reclined position. Infants able to sit independently are equally successful in all positions and prefer unilateral reaches.3,4

The control of posture is a complex neural task. The central nervous system deals with an interplay between con-tinuous afferent information on body position and orienta-tion from visual, vestibular, or somatosensory input and the subsequent motor commands to the muscles to maintain

posture.5 To control the many degrees of freedom in a

multi-segmented body, the central nervous system uses a functional organization of postural control into basic mus-cular synergies that are able to adapt to specific situations.6 The basic level of muscular postural control is direction specificity. It means that when the body sways forward, the dorsal muscles are primarily activated to maintain balance.7 Direction specificity during goal-directed reaching, which involves an active body sway, slowly improves during infancy. Previous research showed that in a supported sit-ting position, direction specificity is present during 40% to 50% of reaching movements at 4 months, that is, before infants are able to sit independently. It increases to 60% to

80% of the movements at 18 months,8 and reaches adult

values (100%) at the age of 2 years.9

The directionally appropriate adjustments can be

adapted to the specifics of the situation using multisensory

input.7 Modulation can be achieved by, for example,

changing the recruitment order of the postural muscles, activating muscles before the start of the movement (antici-patory activation), or adapting the degree of muscle

(3)

contraction. At an early age, infants may already use pro-prioceptive information to adapt their postural muscle activity position to some extent (supine vs supported sit-ting).10Older infants also show minor differences in postu-ral muscle activity between supported and unsupported sitting positions, especially at 18 months and, in particular, in the lumbar extensor muscle (earlier recruitment and higher electromyography [EMG] amplitude in the unsup-ported condition).11The adaptation to sensory information is also observed in studies showing that the relation between postural sway parameters and vision or haptic information becomes more apparent with increasing age and increasing motor development.12,13

Our previous research indicated that postural muscle activity during reaching while sitting with support did not change during the development of independent sitting.14 We suggested that once direction specificity is available, its degree during reaching in a supported sitting position does not influence the emergence of independent sitting. Other studies suggested that postural control might be related to the development of independent standing and walking, considering that the ultimate goal of human postural con-trol is being able to cope with the limited surface support during standing and walking.11,15,16 As no longitudinal studies have addressed muscular postural changes, the pri-mary aim of the present observational study was to assess longitudinal postural control during reaching while sitting in the course of the emergence of independent walking. Attention was also paid to head stabilization in space, as providing a stable visual and vestibular reference frame is one of the primary objectives of postural control.17Typical individuals are able to stabilize their head in space during walking, despite the abundant movements in the underly-ing body parts.18 In infants, major developmental changes in head stability occur during the first 10 to 15 weeks of independent walking,19 but information on head stability when learning to walk is lacking. A secondary aim is to assess whether the development of independent walking influences general motor behaviour. Corbetta and Bojc-zyk20 suggested that learning new patterns of muscle coor-dination while developing the skill to control balance in upright position may be accompanied by changes and regressions in already existing and established motor skills. To assess general motor behaviour, the Infant Motor Pro-file (IMP) was used.21 The IMP is a novel instrument that evaluates spontaneous play behaviour in five domains: vari-ation (size of the repertoire), adaptability (ability to select efficient strategies), symmetry, fluency, and performance (motor milestones).

We hypothesized that the achievement of independent walking might require a reorganization in postural control and in motor behaviour in general. We expect that infants who just mastered their first steps are in the middle of the process of reorganization. Consequently, their postural con-trol and motor development will differ from that in the pre-ceding phase (not able to walk independently) and the following phase (after 1mo of independent walking

experience), instead of showing steadily increasing develop-mental trajectories of the percentages of the different postu-ral parameters and IMP scores. The literature suggests the presence of such transition phases in postural control during the development of a new motor skill. For instance, Chen et al.22 indicated that changes in postural sway parameters when learning to walk are also reflected in the infant’s sit-ting posture, and van der Fits et al.10showed that there was a transient dip in postural muscle activity at 6 months – around the age that infants learn to sit independently.

We addressed the following questions: (1) do muscular postural parameters during reaching while sitting show signs of postural reorganization during the transition to

independent walking – with a dip in otherwise steadily

increasing developmental trajectories? (2) Does motor behaviour, as measured with the IMP, show signs of reor-ganization during the transition to independent walking? We expected ‘dips’ in the total IMP score and in the domains of adaptability and performance.20

METHOD Participants

Twenty-eight typically developing infants were included in the study. Infants were included if born after a gestational age of 37 weeks without prenatal, perinatal, or neonatal complications, and if they were on the verge of indepen-dent standing. The latter meant that infants were able to stand with the help of support but not able to stand inde-pendently. Exclusion criteria were: (1) admission to a pae-diatric department; (2) severe congenital abnormalities; (3) birthweight below the tenth centile; (4) neurological abnormalities; (5) parents having insufficient understanding of the Dutch or English language. The medical ethics committee of the University Medical Center Groningen approved the protocol (NL51701.042.14) and parents gave informed consent.

Procedures

The infants were assessed three times: at T0 – being able to pull to stand but not able to stand independently (‘Pulls to Stand With Support’ item of the Alberta Infant Motor Scale [AIMS]23); T1 – the infant is just able to walk inde-pendently for more than five steps (‘Early Stepping’ item

of the AIMS); and T2 – the infant had some independent

walking experience, that is, 1 month after T1 (‘Walks Alone’ item of the AIMS). Parents contacted the research team if infants were at developmental phase T0 and within a week of achieving T1, which was checked against the AIMS during the laboratory visit. During each session, postural control was assessed and neurodevelopmental con-dition was evaluated.

What this paper adds

Infants show signs of reorganization of motor behaviour when learning to walk.

Infants show individual strategies of postural reorganization when learning to walk.

(4)

Postural assessments

Postural control during reaching was assessed in two con-ditions: first, while sitting in an infant chair providing trunk support, and, second, during unsupported ‘long-leg’ sitting on a thin mattress on the floor. If the infant was not cooperating in the supported sitting position, the infant was seated on the parent’s lap (three sessions). Care was taken that the position of the infant resembled the position in the chair.8 The aim was to elicit at least 10 reaching movements per condition by presenting dif-ferent toys of about 3cm by 6cm and 40g at arm-length distance.

Bipolar surface electrodes with an interelectrode distance of approximately 14mm were placed over the following muscles on the preferred side of the body: deltoid, pec-toralis major, biceps brachii, triceps brachii, neck flexor, neck extensor, rectus abdominis, thoracal extensor, and lumbar extensor. The preferred side (right in all children) was chosen considering its ecological validity. The EMG signal was continuously recorded by means of an electro-physiological front-end amplifier (Cometa Wireless EMG, Milan, Italy) at a sampling rate of 2000Hz, and resampled to 500Hz for analysis. To assess head stability, two reflec-tive markers were placed on the right side of the head: lat-eral to the eye and on the mandibular angle. Another marker was placed on the right wrist to assess reaching kinematics. The session was video-recorded by using a dual camera system recording at a sampling rate of 50Hz. When infants started to cry or became fussy, the session was stopped. Only if data on a specific parameter of three reaching movements were present were they included in the analysis.

Clinical neurodevelopmental assessments

At each session, the Touwen Infant Neurological Examina-tion was used to assess the neurological status of the infant. The AIMS was used to classify the standing and walking ability of the infant.23 To evaluate motor behaviour, the

IMP – a video-based assessment of spontaneous and

eli-cited play behaviour– was used. The IMP evaluates

varia-tion, adaptability, symmetry, fluency and motor

performance in supine, prone, sitting, standing and walk-ing; it pays specific attention to reaching and grasping.21 The three assessments have good reliability and valid-ity.21,24,25

Data analysis

The PedEMG program (Developmental Neurology,

University Medical Center Groningen, the Netherlands)8 was used to analyse the video and EMG signals. Reaching movements with the preferred arm and in a calm beha-vioural state were selected. To identify reaching-related muscle activity, the EMG signal was synchronized with the video. Significant bursts of activation of the postural mus-cles were identified between 100ms before activation of the first activated arm muscle and the end (or first 1000ms) of the reaching movement,14 using the algorithm of Staude

and Wolf.26Kinematic data on head stability and reaching movements were retrieved using SIMI Motion System Analysis (SIMI Reality Motion Systems, Unterschleibheim, Germany).

Subsequently, direction specificity at the trunk and/or neck level was determined. If the trial was direction speci-fic at trunk level, the parameters of the second level of pos-tural control were calculated: (1) recruitment order, in which a top-down recruitment indicates that the neck mus-cle is activated before the thoracal and lumbar extensor and a bottom-up recruitment in the reverse direction; (2) anticipatory activity at the trunk or neck level, when postu-ral muscles are activated within 100ms before activation of the first activated arm muscle (prime mover); (3) mean EMG amplitude in three intervals– after subtracting base-line activity: (a) 100ms before activation of the prime mover (anticipatory activity); (b) start prime mover until 100ms; (c) 100ms to 1000ms. The total travelled path of the vector between the two head markers (angle of the head in space) and number of movement units of the mar-ker lateral to the eye were used as measures of head stabil-ity. A movement unit is an acceleration and a deceleration in the velocity profile of the marker. The use of one move-ment unit indicates that the movemove-ment is programmed in advance, multiple movement units imply multiple correc-tions in the trajectory.27

To determine whether infants were able to modulate EMG amplitude to reaching specifics, kinematic parame-ters of reaching were also retrieved: duration of the reach-ing movement, average speed, and number of movement units of the wrist.

Statistical analyses

Power calculation was based on our own data on direction specificity of infants aged 10 months to 18 months, of which we calculated means to provide data for the power calculation.8 Mean direction specificity occurring both at trunk and neck level increased from 31% (SD 17) at

10 months to 49% (SD 22) at 18 months (rho 0.20). The

calculation revealed that a group size of 26 infants allowed for a power of 0.80 (a=0.05) with Cohen’s effect size of 0.59 to detect significant changes in direction specificity.

To assess the effect of developmental phase on postural control and IMP scores, SPSS (version 23.0.0.3; IBM, Armonk, NY, USA) was used to fit mixed-effects models for repeated measurements, which take clustering of data within the infant into account. A linear mixed-effects model was used to quantify the effect of developmental phase on total IMP score and median head sway (given that head sway had a significantly skewed distribution), using restricted maximum likelihood estimation. There-after, similar models for the IMP domains were fitted. The fit of the mixed-effects models was inspected by assessing the distribution of the residuals. The effects of develop-mental phase on the dichotomous dependent variables direction specificity, recruitment order, and anticipatory activity were fitted using generalized linear mixed models

(5)

with binomial link function (logit), generalized estimating equation, robust estimator, and exchangeability working correlation matrix. For the count-dependent variable of movement unit we used a similar model with the Poisson loglinear link function. Ratios (number of trials with posi-tive outcome/total number of valid trials) were used to model outcomes to control for the different numbers of reaching movements per infant. All models were corrected for age at the moment of measure, and for sex. As the lat-ter did not allat-ter outcome significantly, only the results of the models without correction for sex are presented. If the mixed model resulted in a significant difference (p<0.05) between the three measurements, post hoc tests with Bon-ferroni correction were performed.

Spearman’s correlation was used to explore associations between EMG amplitude (the degree of muscle contrac-tion) of the direction-specific muscles (neck extensor,

tho-racal extensor, or lumbar extensor) and kinematic

parameters within one infant in one measurement. We classified the infant as being able to modulate the ampli-tude of the direction-specific muscle to reaching or head stability kinematics if the infant showed a significant corre-lation (p<0.05) between the kinematic parameter and one of the three amplitude intervals.

RESULTS

Infant characteristics and the number of EMG trials are displayed in Table I. Table SI (online supporting informa-tion) shows reaching characteristics. Of the 28 infants, two dropped out after the first session because they did not tol-erate the electrodes. At T1, the data of three infants were not taken into account in the analyses, as the infants did not meet the AIMS criterion of early stepping (scored as ‘walking alone’ [n=1]; unable to produce ‘early stepping’

during laboratory visit [n=2]). Missed sessions in the other infants or positions were a result of technical problems (n=2) or crying when the floor part of the measurements started (n=9).

Postural control

Figure 1 shows examples of direction-specific and non-direction-specific EMG recordings of an infant at T0, T1, and T2. None of the postural parameters showed a differ-ence between the three measurements, corrected for age at measurement (Table II). This indicates that the children did not show consistent changes in postural muscle activity during sitting while reaching with increasing walking development. This was true for both sitting conditions (with and without support). Only the results without cor-rection for sex are presented in Table II, as corcor-rection for sex did not alter outcome significantly. Nevertheless, the individual developmental trajectories of the various postu-ral parameters when learning to walk indicated the

pres-ence of infant-specific changes. Figure S1 (online

supporting information) shows the individual trajectories of the parameters of each infant during the development of independent walking. For each parameter some children showed a temporary peak, some a temporary dip, some a steady increase, and others a steady decrease. Table III summarizes the individual trajectories in all parameters. It illustrates that the various parameters within one infant do not show consistent patterns of ‘dips’, ‘peaks’, increases, or decreases.

The correlations between EMG amplitude and kine-matic parameters revealed that most infants did not show statistically significant correlations between EMG ampli-tude and the kinematic parameters. This indicates that the infants did not modulate EMG amplitude (adapt the degree of muscle contraction) to reaching characteristics or head stability. This was true for both testing conditions (Table SII, online supporting information provides an overview of the correlations between amplitudes and kine-matic parameters in the chair at T2.)

Infant motor profile

Table SI shows information on single items on adaptability during walking. The mixed-effects model showed that the total IMP score differed significantly between the three measurements, corrected for age (Table II). Post hoc tests revealed that there was a significant difference between T0 and T1 (mean IMP score T0 95%, T1 91%; p<0.001). A similar difference was absent between T1 and T2 (mean IMP score T1 91%, T2 93%; p=0.073). Secondary analyses indicated that the changes in the IMP scores could partly be attributed to changes in the adaptability domain (mean T0 98%, T1 87%, T2 92%; post hoc: T0–T1 p<0.001, T1–T2 p=0.006) and the fluency domain, which demon-strated a significant difference between T0 and T1 but not between T1 and T2 (mean T0 95%, T1 89%, T2 91%; post hoc T0–T1 p=0.040, T1–T2 p=0.809). The scores on the performance domain did not change between T0 and

Table I: Infant characteristics

Sex (M:F) 14:14

Median (range) gestational age (wks) 39.9 (37.0–42.0) Median (range) birthweight (g) 3654.5 (3000–4500) Neurological classification (TINE)

Normal or normal–suboptimal 28

MND 0

Clear neurological syndrome 0 Median (range) age at assessment (mo)

T0 12.5 (9.3–16.1) T1 14.4 (11.4–19.1) T2 15.6 (12.6–21.5) Median (range) age at independent

walking (mo) 14.1 (11.1–18.7) Measurements Chair T0 28 T1 23 T2 25 Long-leg sitting T0 26 T1 19 T2 21

n=28. Data are n unless otherwise indicated. F, female; M, male; MND, minor neurological dysfunction; TINE, Touwen Infant Neuro-logical Examination.

(6)

T1 but increased between T1 and T2 (mean T0 87%, T1 86%, T2 90%; post hoc: T0–T1 p=1.000, T1–T2 p<0.001).

DISCUSSION

The present study indicates that typically developing infants do not show a consistent pattern of reorganization in muscular postural parameters during seated reaching while learning to walk. However, the developmental trajec-tories of the IMP suggested that signs of a reorganization in general motor behaviour during the transition to inde-pendent walking are present.

When learning to walk, the nervous system coordinates the organization of the newly involved muscles into new functional synergies. In addition, the sensorimotor system has to incorporate, for instance, proprioceptive information from the feet into the already existing sensorimotor sys-tem.12,22 Chen et al.22 demonstrated that this reweighting of sensory information was reflected in postural sway beha-viour during sitting. The update of the sensorimotor sys-tem presumably also promotes the development of

anticipatory postural adjustments when infants learn to walk.15 However, contrary to our hypothesis, we did not observe a consistent pattern of transition in our muscular postural parameters – not even in anticipatory activity. The lack of association between the postural parameters and learning to walk, and the similarity in postural adjust-ments in the two sitting positions suggest that muscular postural control of reaching while sitting is a relatively well-mastered job for the infant, even when the infant is sitting unsupported. This differs for younger infants, where sitting with and without support is associated with different reaching skills and postural muscle activity.3,4,10 Our find-ings suggest that the expression of a possible reorganiza-tion is presumably dependent on the testing situareorganiza-tion. Possibly, our simple task of reaching while sitting allowed the infants to try and use the different postural strategies, which did not result in one predominant strategy. Our data contrast with those of Hedberg et al.,28 who used a far more challenging test situation. Their longitudinal study showed that infants who learned to stand and walk increased direction specificity with increasing age and

1 second LE NE RA NF TE PM T0 T1 T2

Figure 1: Examples of electromyography signals of the same infant sitting in the infant chair at T0, T1, and T2. Each row represents muscle activity of one muscle. The vertical dotted lines display the start and end of the reaching movement as indicated on video; the bold vertical line indicates a signif-icant increase in muscle activity using the Staude and Wolff algorithm,26i.e. the start of muscle activation. Amplitude units with intervals of 50

lV are indicated by the small horizontal lines on they-axis. Direction specificity means activation of dorsal muscles before (or in absence of) ventral muscle activity. At T0 postural activity during reaching is direction specific at trunk level with lumbar extensor (LE) and thoracal extensor (TE) being activated without (or in other cases before) activation of the rectus abdominis (RA) muscle; postural activity is not direction specific at neck level as neck exten-sor (NE) is not activated (or in other cases before neck flexor [NF]). At T1 postural activity is not direction specific and at T2 postural activity is direc-tion specific at neck and at trunk level. PM, prime mover, the first activated arm muscle (at T0 triceps brachii, at T1 deltoid, at T2 pectoralis major).

(7)

walking experience while being perturbed by a moveable platform in a standing position.28 This suggests that more challenging situations like standing and experiencing

external perturbations from a platform may force the bal-ance-control system to select the most functional strategy, that is, direction specificity.

Table II: Mixed-effects model analyses: the change in Infant Motor Profile (IMP) and postural parameters when learning to walk, corrected for age at measurement

Infant chair Long-leg position Mean estimates (95% CI) Mean estimates (95% CI)

T0 T1 T2 p T0 T1 T2 p DS trunk (%) 77 (69–84) 72 (63–80) 80 (71–87) 0.333 79 (70–86) 81 (71–88) 84 (74–91) 0.665 DS neck (%) 31 (25–39) 35 (27–44) 38 (31–46) 0.509 37 (29–44) 31 (24–38) 36 (29–44) 0.578 DS trunk and neck (%) 38 (30–45) 34 (26–43) 47 (40–55) 0.126 37 (29–46) 35 (28–43) 44 (33–55) 0.395 Top-down (%) 33 (28–39) 27 (19–37) 26 (19–34) 0.445 33 (28–39) 21 (14–31) 28 (21–35) 0.186 Bottom-up (%) 24 (18–31) 27 (21–34) 29 (22–38) 0.587 22 (17–29) 36 (27–46) 28 (21–36) 0.156 Anticipatory activity trunk (%) 22 (16–31) 24 (18–30) 25 (18–33) 0.897 27 (19–37) 26 (20–33) 27 (20–35) 0.982 Anticipatory activity neck (%) 14 (10–19) 12 (7–19) 17 (13–22) 0.392 18 (12–25) 11 (7–19) 11 (6–20) 0.283 Head sway (◦) 15.7 (14.0–17.3) 14.5 (12.8–16.2) 15.0 (13.2–16.8) 0.593 16.7 (14.2–19.1) 16.0 (13.5–18.5) 14.3 (11.6–17.0) 0.478 MU head (n) 1.9 (1.6–2.3) 1.8 (1.5–2.1) 1.9 (1.7–2.2) 0.615 1.8 (1.5–2.1) 1.7 (1.4–2.1) 2.1 (1.8–2.6) 0.289 IMP totala(%) 95 (94–97) 91 (90–93) 93 (92–95) <0.001 Variationb(%) 98 (96–100) 96 (94–97) 95 (94–97) 0.121 Adaptabilityb(%) 98 (96–100) 87 (85–88) 91 (89–93) <0.001 Fluency (%) 95 (91–99) 89 (85–92) 91 (88–95) 0.045 Performance (%) 87 (86–88) 86 (85–87) 90 (89–91) <0.001

Bold denotes significant data.aAs the domain symmetry suffered from too strong a ceiling effect in the data (i.e. too many children

obtain-ing the maximum score of 100%), no reliable statistical model could be built. This ceilobtain-ing effect reflects the typically developobtain-ing nature of the participating infants.bThe models for variation and adaptability suffered to a lesser extent from the ceiling effect. Although normality

of the residuals was not met in all linear mixed-effect models, similar results were produced using different modelling approaches (i.e. modelling the raw scores as binomial counts using generalized estimating equation with logit link function); therefore, we were willing to trust the outcome of the models here presented. CI, confidence interval; DS, direction specificity; MU, movement unit.

Table III: Individual developmental trajectories of the different postural parameters when learning to walk in supported sitting position

Infant DS trunk DS neck DS trunk and neck Bottom-up Top-down Anticipatory activity trunk Anticipatory activity neck 1 ˅ 2 ˅ Λ 3 Λ – – – – – – 4 ˅ 5 ˅ Λ ⇈ – 6 ˅ – ˅ Λ – – – 7 Λ Λ Λ 8 – – – ⇈ – Λ ˅ 9 ˅ Λ 10 – ˅ – – ⇊ ⇊ ⇊ 11 ˅ 12 – – – – – – ˅ 13 Λ 14 ˅ ⇈ ˅ Λ ⇈ – – 15 ⇊ – – ⇈ ˅ – – 16 Λ ˅ 17 ⇊ – ˅ 18 ˅ ˅ ˅ Λ 19 ˅ ˅ – – 20 21 – ˅ ˅

The cut-off of 10% was arbitrarily chosen, as the postural control literature did not offer information for a scientifically-based alternative. Note that trajectory information is missing in seven infants because of missing data at T1 (n=3), at T2 (n=2), and drop-out after T0 (n=2). A corre-sponding table with data in the unsupported position was similar.˅, ‘dip’ trajectory consisting of a decrease of ≥10% of the mean of the parameter per session between T0 and T1 followed by an increase of≥10% between T1 and T2; Λ, ‘peak’ trajectory consisting of an increase of ≥10% between T0 and T1 followed by a decrease of ≥10% between T1 and T2; ⇊, ‘decreasing’ trajectory consisting of a decrease of ≥10%, both between T0 and T1 and between T1 and T2;⇈, ’increasing’ trajectory consisting of an increase of ≥10% both between T0 and T1 and between T1 and T2;–, no increase or decrease between the measurement moments, or changes <10%; a blank cell indicates that the data of the parameter were missing in one of the sessions, precluding the assignment of a trajectory. DS, direction specificity.

(8)

Nevertheless, Figure S1 and Table III suggest that learning to walk may be accompanied by changes in postu-ral control during seated reaching, although a different way for each infant. The latter suggestion fits the idea that motor development is characterized by variation, and that each infant uses different motor strategies to discover new skills and the best adaptations to environmental con-straints.29 Our findings correspond to those of Woollacott et al.’s30 study of children with cerebral palsy, where improvement of postural control after training was achieved in a different way in each child. Perhaps the influ-ence of learning to walk at group level is only observable in the net postural outcome reflected, for instance, in sway parameters,12,22 whereas this is achieved with different muscular strategies in the individual infant.

Our IMP data underscore the notion of reorganization of motor behaviour during the development of indepen-dent walking; the total IMP score and the fluency score decreased and the scores of the domain adaptability showed a dip at the time when the infants were able to produce their first independent steps, after which the per-formance scores improved. This result corroborates the findings of Corbetta et al. and the suggestions of Chen et al.12,20,22 We propose that when learning a new skill, the infant should learn anew how to adapt the new beha-viour to different situations, by controlling newly involved muscles and integrating different sensory information.

Most of the infants did not modulate EMG amplitude to reaching and head stability characteristics, not before, dur-ing, or after the development of independent walking. This is in line with the results of van der Fits et al.,11who indi-cated that modulation of EMG amplitude during reaching in sitting position emerges from 18 months onwards. Pre-sumably, the modulation of postural EMG activity is dependent on the situation or challenge during testing. Apparently, modulation of EMG activity during reaching in sitting position is not affected by learning to walk, but, for example, Hadders-Algra et al.31suggest that the capac-ity to modulate EMG activcapac-ity during external perturba-tions of a moveable platform was promoted by daily balance practice.

Both our head stability parameters remained stable dur-ing the development of independent walkdur-ing. Conceivably, the infants experienced no difficulties with head balance in our sitting position, and a possible reorganization of postu-ral control does not influence head balance in an unchal-lenging position.

Two strengths of the study are the longitudinal data col-lection during the development of independent walking and the use of the mixed-effects models; it allowed us to calculate individual trajectories over time, while taking repeated measures at different ages into account. Addition-ally, by correcting for age we could focus on the effect of the development of independent walking. However, inter-pretation of the data is limited owing to data loss, reducing our sample to a size that just lacked sufficient power. This

occurred especially at T1 and in the long-leg position. Although parents were instructed very carefully, it appeared difficult to get the parents to contact the research team at the correct time, allowing scheduling of the T1 assessment within a week of the infants’ first independent steps. What may be considered another limitation is the assessment of postural activity on one side of the body. However, previous research indicated that the postural muscles on the side contralateral to the reaching arm are less frequently activated than the muscles on the ipsilateral

side.11 This suggests that we adequately covered the

infant’s postural muscle activity.

In conclusion, this study demonstrated that while learn-ing to walk independently, infants show signs of reorgani-zation of their motor behaviour. However, they do not show consistent signs of a reorganization of their muscular postural control strategies during reaching in sitting posi-tion. Rather, the data suggest that every infant uses its own muscular postural control strategy during reaching while sitting in the transition period to independent walking. As we have suggested that our testing situation may have been an unchallenging situation for infants, we propose to fur-ther explore postural development during walking by assessing muscular postural control strategies in a similar way to the current study but in reaching while standing. Another option would be the evaluation of postural muscles strategies during the development of independent walking in larger groups of infants as this would allow for the exploration of possible latent clusters of specific pat-terns of development by, for example, factor analysis or latent class analyses. The existence of latent clusters may show that development of specific infants may be similar or that combinations of specific postural parameters develop following a certain track.

A C K N O W L E D G E M E N T S

We are thankful for the help of Saskia Dijkstra, Patricia Pekel, Johan Tiems, Siebrigje Hooijsma, and Anneke Kracht in the data collection and/or analysis. AGB received financial support from the Junior Scientific Masterclass. The funder was not involved in the design of the study, data collection, data analysis, manuscript preparation, or publication decisions. The authors have stated that they had no interests which might be perceived as posing a con-flict or bias.

S U P P O R T I N G I N F O R M A T I O N

Additional supporting information may be found online in the Supporting Information section at the end of the article.

Figure S1: Individual trajectories of postural parameters before, just after, and a month after independent walking.

Table SI: Reaching characteristics and adaptability items of the Infant Motor Profile during walking

Table SII: Correlation between kinematic parameters and elec-tromyography amplitude of direction-specific postural muscles at T2 in supported sitting position

(9)

REFERENCES

1. Soska KC, Adolph KE, Johnson SP. Systems in develop-ment: motor skill acquisition facilitates three-dimen-sional object completion. Dev Psychol 2010; 46: 129–38. 2. Sommerville JA, Decety J. Weaving the fabric of social

interaction: articulating developmental psychology and cognitive neuroscience in the domain of motor cogni-tion. Psychon Bull Rev 2006; 13: 179–200.

3. Rochat P. Self-sitting and reaching in 5- to 8-month-old infants: the impact of posture and its development on early eye-hand coordination. J Mot Behav 1992; 24: 210–20.

4. Out L, Van Soest AJ, Savelsbergh GJ, Hopkins B. The effect of posture on early reaching movements. J Mot Behav 1998; 30: 260–72.

5. van der Kooij H, Jacobs R, Koopman B, van der Helm F. An adaptive model of sensory integration in a dynamic environment applied to human stance control. Biol Cybern 2001; 84: 103–15.

6. Hadders-Algra M. The development of postural control. In: Hadders-Algra M, Brogren-Carlberg E, editors. Pos-tural control: A Key Issue in Developmental Disorders. London: Mac Keith Press, 2008: 22–73.

7. Forssberg H, Hirschfeld H. Postural adjustments in sit-ting humans following external perturbations: muscle activity and kinematics. Exp Brain Res 1994; 97: 515–27. 8. van Balen LC, Dijkstra LJ, Hadders-Algra M.

Develop-ment of postural adjustDevelop-ments during reaching in typi-cally developing infants from 4 to 18 months. Exp Brain Res 2012; 220: 109–19.

9. van der Heide JC, Otten B, van Eykern LA, Hadders-Algra M. Development of postural adjustments during reaching in sitting children. Exp Brain Res 2003; 151: 32–45.

10. van der Fits IB, Klip AW, van Eykern LA, Hadders-Algra M. Postural adjustments during spontaneous and goal-directed arm movements in the first half year of life. Behav Brain Res 1999; 106: 75–90.

11. van der Fits IB, Otten E, Klip AW, Van Eykern LA, Hadders-Algra M. The development of postural adjust-ments during reaching in 6- to 18-month-old infants. Evidence for two transitions. Exp Brain Res 1999; 126: 517–28.

12. Chen LC, Jeka J, Clark JE. Development of adaptive sensorimotor control in infant sitting posture. Gait Pos-ture 2016; 45: 157–63.

13. Barela J, Jeka JJ, Clark JE. The use of somatosensory information during the acquisition of independent upright stance. Infant Behav Dev 1999; 22: 87–102. 14. Boxum AG, van Balen LC, Dijkstra LJ, et al. Postural

adjustments in infants at very high risk for cerebral palsy before and after developing the ability to sit indepen-dently. Early Hum Dev 2014; 90: 435–41.

15. Cignetti F, Zedka M, Vaugoyeau M, Assaiante C. Inde-pendent walking as a major skill for the development of anticipatory postural control: evidence from adjustments to predictable perturbations. PLoS ONE 2013; 8: e56313. 16. Assaiante C, Woollacott M, Amblard B. Development of

postural adjustment during gait initiation: kinematic and EMG analysis. J Mot Behav 2000; 32: 211–26. 17. Pozzo T, Berthoz A, Lefort L. Head stabilization during

various locomotor tasks in humans. I. Normal subjects. Exp Brain Res 1990; 82: 97–106.

18. Cromwell RL, Newton RA, Carlton LG. Horizontal plane head stabilization during locomotor tasks. J Mot Behav 2001; 33: 49–58.

19. Ledebt A, Bril B, Wiener-Vacher S. Trunk and head stabilization during the first months of independent walking. NeuroReport 1995; 6: 1737–40.

20. Corbetta D, Bojczyk KE. Infants return to two-handed reaching when they are learning to walk. J Mot Behav 2002; 34: 83–95.

21. Heineman KR, Middelburg KJ, Bos AF, et al. Reliability and concurrent validity of the Infant Motor Profile. Dev Med Child Neurol 2013; 55: 539–45.

22. Chen LC, Metcalfe JS, Jeka JJ, Clark JE. Two steps for-ward and one back: learning to walk affects infants’ sit-ting posture. Infant Behav Dev 2007; 30: 16–25. 23. Piper MC, Darrah J. Motor Assessment of the

Develop-ing Infant. Philadelphia, PA: Saunders, 1994. 24. Hadders-Algra M, Heineman KR, Bos AF, Middelburg

KJ. The assessment of minor neurological dysfunction in infancy using the Touwen Infant Neurological Exam-ination: strengths and limitations. Dev Med Child Neurol 2010; 52: 87–92.

25. Piper MC, Pinnell LE, Darrah J, Maguire T, Byrne PJ. Construction and validation of the Alberta Infant Motor Scale (AIMS). Can J Public Health; 1992; 83(Suppl. 2): S46–50.

26. Staude G, Wolf W. Objective motor response onset detection in surface myoelectric signals. Med Eng Phys; 1999; 21: 449–67.

27. von Hofsten C. Structuring of early reaching move-ments: a longitudinal study. J Mot Behav 1991; 23: 280– 92.

28. Hedberg A, Schmitz C, Forssberg H, Hadders-Algra M. Early development of postural adjustments in standing with and without support. Exp Brain Res 2007; 178: 439–49.

29. Hadders-Algra M. Variation and variability: key words in human motor development. Phys Ther 2010; 90: 1823–37.

30. Woollacott M, Shumway-Cook A, Hutchinson S, Ciol M, Price R, Kartin D. Effect of balance training on muscle activity used in recovery of stability in children with cerebral palsy: a pilot study. Dev Med Child Neurol 2005; 47: 455–61.

31. Hadders-Algra M, Brogren E, Forssberg H. Training affects the development of postural adjustments in sit-ting infants. J Physiol 1996; 493(Pt 1): 289–98.

Referenties

GERELATEERDE DOCUMENTEN

Findings of this dissertation suggest that: 1) at group level, muscular postural control strategies improve with age rather than with functional performance; 2) when learning

Chapter 4: Early intervention and the development of postural adjustments during reaching in infants at risk of cerebral palsy: an in-depth analysis of a randomized

This was a randomized controlled trial comparing COPCA to traditional infant physiotherapy (TIP) consisting of a mix of NDT and functional approaches. From 2003 to

For each infant at each age, the following parameters were calculated: (1) percentage of direction-specific trials at neck and/or trunk level; direction-specificity meant

recruitment (middle panel), and anticipatory activation in any postural muscle (right panel) of infants at high risk (HR) for CP and infants with typical development (TD) at

Between-group analyses indicated that postural outcomes of the COPCA and TIP groups were similar. Within-group analyses revealed some differences: 1) infants who had

The present study suggested that postural control in terms of direction-specificity and recruitment order in infants at very high risk for CP does not change when the infant

Apparently, during development of independent walking, TD-infants get into the habit of consistently applying appropriate (direction-specific) postural adjustments even in