• No results found

Rethinking inhibition theory: explaining forgetting without inhibition - References

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Share "Rethinking inhibition theory: explaining forgetting without inhibition - References"

Copied!
10
0
0

Bezig met laden.... (Bekijk nu de volledige tekst)

Hele tekst

(1)

UvA-DARE is a service provided by the library of the University of Amsterdam (https://dare.uva.nl)

UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository)

Rethinking inhibition theory: explaining forgetting without inhibition

Jakab, E.

Publication date

2010

Link to publication

Citation for published version (APA):

Jakab, E. (2010). Rethinking inhibition theory: explaining forgetting without inhibition.

General rights

It is not permitted to download or to forward/distribute the text or part of it without the consent of the author(s) and/or copyright holder(s), other than for strictly personal, individual use, unless the work is under an open content license (like Creative Commons).

Disclaimer/Complaints regulations

If you believe that digital publication of certain material infringes any of your rights or (privacy) interests, please let the Library know, stating your reasons. In case of a legitimate complaint, the Library will make the material inaccessible and/or remove it from the website. Please Ask the Library: https://uba.uva.nl/en/contact, or a letter to: Library of the University of Amsterdam, Secretariat, Singel 425, 1012 WP Amsterdam, The Netherlands. You will be contacted as soon as possible.

(2)

179

References

Anderson, J. R. (1976). Language, memory, and thought. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Anderson, J. R. (1981). Interference: The relationship between response latency and response accuracy. Journal of Experimental

Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 7, 326-343.

Anderson, J. R. (1983a). The architecture of cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Anderson, J. R. (1983b). A spreading activation theory of memory.

Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 22, 261-295.

Anderson, J. R. (1990). The adaptive character of thought. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates

Anderson, J. R. (1993). Rules of the mind. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Anderson, M. C. (2003). Rethinking interference theory: Executive control and the mechanism of forgetting. Journal of Memory and

Language, 49, 415-445.

Anderson, M. C. (2005). The role of inhibitory control in forgetting unwanted memories: A consideration of three methods. In N. Otha, C. M. MacLeod & B. Uttl, (eds.), Dynamic Cognitive

Processes (pp. 301-331). Tokyo, Japan: Springer-Verlag.

Anderson, M. C., & Bell, T. (2001). Forgetting our facts: The role of inhibitory processes in the loss of propositional knowledge.

Journal of Experimental Psychology, General, 130, 544-570.

Anderson, M. C., Bjork, E. L., & Bjork, R. A. (2000). Retrieval-induced forgetting: Evidence for a recall-specific mechanism.

Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 7, 522-530.

Anderson, M. C., Bjork, R. A., & Bjork, E. L. (1994). Remembering can cause forgetting: Retrieval dynamics in long-term memory.

(3)

References

180

Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory & Cognition, 20, 1063-1087.

Anderson, M. C., Green, C., & McCulloch, K. C. (2000). Similarity and inhibition in long-term memory: Evidence for a two-factor model.

Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 26, 1141-1159.

Anderson, M. C., & McCulloch, K. C. (1999). Integration as a general boundary condition on retrieval-induced forgetting. Journal of

Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 25,

608–629.

Anderson, M. C., & Spellman, B. A. (1995). On the status of inhibitory mechanisms in cognition: Memory retrieval as a model case.

Psychological Review, 102, 68-100.

Atkinson, R. C., & Shiffrin, R. M. (1968). Human memory: A proposed system and its control processes. In K. W. Spence, & J. T. Spence (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation: Advances in

research and theory. Vol. 2. New York: Academic Press.

Basden, D. R., & Basden, B. H. (1995). Some tests of the strategy disruption interpretation of part-list cuing inhibition. Journal of

Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 21,

1656-1669.

Basden, D. R., Basden, B. H., & Galloway, B. C. (1977). Inhibition with part-list cuing: Some tests of the item strength hypothesis.

Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 3, 100-108.

Bäuml, K. H. (1996). Revisiting an old issue: Retroactive interference as a function of the degree of original and interpolated learning. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 3, 380-384.

Bäuml, K. H. (1997). The list-strength effect: Strength-dependent competition or suppression? Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 4, 260-264.

(4)

181 Bäuml, K. (1998). Strong items get suppressed, weak items do not: The role of item strength in output interference. Psychonomic Bulletin

& Review, 5, 459-463.

Bjork, E. L., & Bjork, R. A. (1996). Continuing influences of to-be-forgotten information. Consciousness and Cognition, 5, 176-196.

Blaxton, T. A. (1989). Investigating dissociations among memory measures: Support for a transfer appropriate processing framework. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning,

Memory, and Cognition, 15, 657-668.

Camp, G., Jakab, E. & Raaijmakers, J. G. W. (2010). Dutch category

norms. Manuscript in preparation.

Camp, G., Pecher, D., & Schmidt, H. G. (2007). No retrieval-induced forgetting using item-specific independent cues: Evidence against a general inhibitory account. Journal of Experimental

Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 33, 950-958.

Camp, G., Pecher, D., Schmidt, H. G., & Zeelenberg, R. (2009). Are independent probes truly independent? Journal of Experimental

Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35, 934-942.

Ciranni, M. A., & Shimamura, A. P. (1999). Retrieval-induced forgetting in episodic memory. Journal of Experimental

Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 25, 1403-1414.

Crowder, R. G. (1976). Principles of learning and memory. Hillsdale, N.J: Erlbaum.

Debner, J. A. & Jacoby, L. L. (1994). Unconscious perception: attention, awareness, and control. Journal of Experimental

Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 20, 304-17.

Dodd, M. D., Castel, A. D., & Roberts, K. E. (2006). A strategy disruption component to retrieval-induced forgetting. Memory

(5)

References

182

Evett, L. J. & Humphreys, G. W. (1981). The use of abstract graphemic information in lexical access. Quarterly Journal of

Experimental Psychology, 33, 325-350.

Forster, K., Booker, J., Schacter, D. L., & Davis, C. (1990). Masked repetition priming: Lexical activation or novel memory trace?

Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 28, 341-345.

Forster, K. I., & Davis, C. (1984). Repetition priming and frequency attenuation in lexical access. Journal of Experimental

Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 10, 680-698.

Gillund, G., & Shiffrin, R. M. (1984). A retrieval model for both recognition and recall. Psychological Review, 91, 1-67.

Glover, S., & Dixon, P. (2004). Likelihood ratios: A simple and flexible statistic for empirical psychologists. Psychonomic Bulletin &

Review, 11, 791-806.

Hudson, P. T. W. (1982). Preliminary category norms for verbal items

in 51 categories in Dutch. Internal Report / Vakgroep

Psychologische Funktieleer. Katholieke Universiteit Nijmegen, The Netherlands.

Humphreys, M. S., Wiles, J., & Bain, J. D. (1992). Direct access: Cues with separate histories.In meyer, D.E., & Kornblum, s. (Eds.),

Attention and Performance XXIV: A Silver Jubilee, 489-508. MIT

Press, Cambridge, MA.

Jacoby, L. L., & Dallas, M. (1981). On the relationship between autobiographical memory and perceptual learning. Journal of

Experimental Psychology: General, 110, 306-340.

Jakab, E. & Raaijmakers, J. G. W. (2009). The role of item strength in retrieval induced forgetting. Journal of Experimental

Psychology: Learning, Memory & Cognition, 35, 607-617.

Jakab, E. & Raaijmakers, J. G. W. (2010). The modulating effect of

target item strength in retrieval induced forgetting. Manuscript

(6)

183 Johnson, S. K., & Anderson, M. C. (2004). The role of inhibitory

control in forgetting semantic knowledge. Psychological

Science, 15, 448-453.

Keppel, G. (1966). Association by contiguity: Role of response availability. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 71, 624-628. Keppel, G. (1968). Retroactive and proactive inhibition. In T.R. Dixon

& D. L. Horton (Ed.), Verbal behavior and general behavior

theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ.: Prentice-Hall.

Major, J. C., Camp, G., & MacLeod, C. M. (2008). Encoding strength

does not modulate retrieval-induced forgetting: Evidence inconsistent with an inhibition account. Manuscript submitted for

publication.

Malmberg, K. J., & Shiffrin, R. M. (2005). The “One-Shot” hypothesis for context storage. Journal of Experimental Psychology:

Learning, Memory & Cognition, 31, 322-336.

McGeoch, J. A. (1932). Forgetting and the law of disuse. Psychological

Review , 39, 352–370.

McGeoch, J. A. (1942). The psychology of human learning. New York: Longmans, Green.

Melton, A. W., & Irwin, J. McQ. (1940). The influence of degree of interpolated learning on retroactive inhibition and the overt transfer of specific responses. The American Journal of

Psychology. 53, 173-203.

Melton, A. W., & von Lackum, W. J. (1941). Retroactive and proactive inhibition in retention: evidence for a two-factor theory of retroactive inhibition. The American Journal of Psychology. 54, 157-173.

Mensink, G. J. M., & Raaijmakers, J.G.W. (1988). A model of interference and forgetting. Psychological Review, 95, 434-455. Merikle, P. M., Joordens, S., & Stolz, J. A. (1995). Measuring the

relative magnitude of unconscious influences. Consciousness

(7)

References

184

Morris, C. D., Bransford, J. D., & Franks, J. J. (1977). Levels of processing versus transfer appropriate processing. Journal of

Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior,16, 519-533.

Müller, G. E., & Pilzecker, A. (1900). Experimentelle Beiträge zur Lehre vom Gedächtnis. Zeitschrift für Psychologie,

Ergänzungsband, 1, 1-300.

Perfect, T, J., Stark, L., Tree, J. J., Moulin, C. J. A., Ahmed, L., & Hutter, R. (2004). Transfer appropriate forgetting: The cue-dependent nature of retrieval-induced forgetting. Journal of

Memory and Language, 51, 399-417.

Postman, L., Stark, K., & Fraser, J. (1968). Temporal changes in interference. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behaviour,

7, 672-694.

Postman, L., & Underwood, B. J. (1973). Critical issues in interference theory. Memory and Cognition, 1, 19-40.

Raaijmakers, J. G. W., & Shiffrin, R. M. (1981). Search of associative memory. Psychological Review, 88, 93-134.

Racsmány, M., & Conway, M. A. (2006). Episodic inhibition. Journal

of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory & Cognition, 32, 44-57.

Ratcliff, R., Clark, S. E., & Shiffrin, R. M. (1990). The list-strength effect: I. Data and discussion. Journal of Experimental

Psychology: .Learning, Memory and Cognition, 16, 163-178.

Roediger III, H., L. (1973).Inhibition in recall fro cueing with recall targets. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 12, 644-657.

Rouder, J. N., Speckman, P. L., Sun, D., Morey, R. D., & Iverson, G. (2009). Bayesian t–tests for accepting and rejecting the null hypothesis. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 16, 225-237. Rubin, D. C., & Wallace, W. T. (1989). Rhyme and reason: Analyses of

dual retrieval cues. Journal of Experimental Psychology:

(8)

185 Rundus, D. (1971). Analysis of rehearsal processes in free recall.

Journal of Experimental Psychology, 89, 63-77.

Scarborough, D. L., Cortese, C., & Scarborough, H. S. (1977). Frequency and repetition effects in lexical memory. Journal of

Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 3, 1 - 17.

Schneider, W., Eschman, A., & Zuccolotto, A. (2002). E-Prime User’s

Guide. Pittsburgh: Psychology Software Tools Inc.

Schütz, K., Schendzielarz, I., Zwitserlood, P. & Vorberg, D. (2007). Nice wor_ if you can get the wor_: Subliminal semantic and form priming in fragment completion. Consciousness and

Cognition, 16, 520-532.

Shiffrin, R. M. (1976). Capacity limitations in information processing, attention, and memory. In W. K. Estes (Ed.), Handbook of

learning and cognitive processes. Vol. 4. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Shivde, G., & Anderson, M. C. (2001). The role of inhibition in meaning selection: Insights from retrieval-induced forgetting. In D. S. Gorfein (Ed.), On the consequences of meaning selection:

Perspectives on resolving lexical ambiguity (pp. 175–190).

Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

Slamecka, N. J. (1968). An examination of trace storage in free recall.

Journal of Experimental Psychology, 76, 504-513.

Slamecka, N. J. (1969). Testing for associative storage in multitrial free recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 81, 557-560. Slamecka, N. J., & Graf, P. (1978). The generation effect: Delineation

of a phenomenon. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human

Learning and Memory, 4, 592-604.

Spear, N. E., Ekstrand, B. R., & Underwood, B. J. (1964). Association by contiguity. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 67, 151-161.

(9)

References

186

Storm, B. C., Bjork, E. L., & Bjork, R. A. (2007). When intended remembering leads to unintended forgetting. The Quarterly

Journal of Experimental Psychology, 60 (7), 909-915.

Storm, B. C., Bjork, E. L., Bjork, R. A., & Nestojko, J. F. (2006). Is retrieval success a necessary condition for retrieval-induced forgetting? Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 13, 1023-1027. Storms, G. (2001). Flemish category norms for exemplars of 39

categories: A replication of the Battig and Montague (1969) category norms. Psychologica Belgica, 41, 145-168.

Tenpenny, P. L. (1995). Abstractionist versus episodic theories of repetition priming and word identification. Psychonomic

Bulletin & Review, 2, 339-363.

Thorndike, E. L. (1914). The psychology of learning. NewYork: Teachers College Press.

Underwood, B. J. (1948a). Retroactive and proactive inhibition after five and forty-eight hours. Journal of Experimental Psychology,

38, 29-38.

Underwood, B. J. (1948b). ‘Spontaneous recovery’ of verbal associations. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 38, 429-439. Verde, M. F. (2009). The list-strength effect in recall: relative-strength

competition and retrieval inhibition may both contribute to forgetting. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning,

Memory & Cognition, 35, 205-220Wagenaar, W. A. (1969).

Note on the construction of digram-balanced latin squares.

Psychological Bulletin, 72, 384-386.

Wagenmakers, E.-J. (2007). A practical solution to the pervasive problems of p values. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14, 779-804.

Wallace, W. P. (1970). Consistency of Emission Order in Free Recall.

(10)

187 Williams, C. C., & Zacks, R. T. (2001). Is retrieval-induced forgetting an inhibitory process. American Journal of Psychology, 114, 329-354.

Wood, G., & Underwood, B. J. (1967). Implicit Responses and Conceptual Similarity. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal

Referenties

GERELATEERDE DOCUMENTEN

If you believe that digital publication of certain material infringes any of your rights or (privacy) interests, please let the Library know, stating your reasons.. In case of

The Wadge game GW ( A, B) has two Players and is normally defined in such a way that Player II has a winning strategy if and only if A ≤W B.. In this thesis, however, it will

As the game progresses, Player II may extend these finite sequences and may increase the number of rows she is using.. In the limit, Player II’s task is to produce an infinite

The Wadge game can be viewed as the restricted tree game in which Player II is required to produce φ such that dom(φ) is linear; for the eraser game, we require that dom( φ) is

Please Ask the Library: https://uba.uva.nl/en/contact, or a letter to: Library of the University of Amsterdam, Secretariat, Singel 425, 1012 WP Amsterdam, The Netherlands.. You will

In that regard, the enhancement of memory processes during the early stages of responding to a stressor can be viewed as logical and salutary.” However, one of the

High cortisol levels at the time of testing were associated with slow WM performance at high loads, and with impaired recall of moderately emotional, but not of

Oei and colleagues found that stress impaired accuracy in the Sternberg paradigm specifically at high loads during present-target trials, whereas Schoofs and