• No results found

Emotions that make your ad go viral! : the effect of positive and negative ad-evoked emotions by viral advertising on consumers’ intention to share, brand attitude and attitude towards the advertisement

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Share "Emotions that make your ad go viral! : the effect of positive and negative ad-evoked emotions by viral advertising on consumers’ intention to share, brand attitude and attitude towards the advertisement"

Copied!
41
0
0

Bezig met laden.... (Bekijk nu de volledige tekst)

Hele tekst

(1)

Emotions that make your ad go viral!

The effect of positive and negative ad-evoked emotions by viral

advertising on consumers’ intention to share, brand attitude and

attitude towards the advertisement.

Master of Science (M.Sc.)

University of Amsterdam

Graduate School of Communication

Author: Navina Wabnitz

Student number: 10373195

Supervisor: Dr. Sophie Boerman

College Year: 2014

Master Communication Science

Track Persuasive Communication

Master’s Thesis

(2)

1

Abstract

This study aimed to narrow down the influential role of ad-evoked emotions in viral

advertising, especially in the most used viral advertising tool of viral video ads. Some

researchers suggest that the emotions that get evoked with the viewer during exposure to a

viral advertisement is what likely drives consumers’ intention to share which ultimately

causes the advertisement to go viral, consumers’ attitude towards the brand which is promoted

in the viral ad and it also their attitude towards the viral ad itself. Next to loads of viral video

ads that evoke positive emotions with the viewer, there are still some viral video ads designed

by marketers that evoke mainly negative emotions. This study hypothesized that there is a

difference in effect between positive and negative evoked emotions and that positive

ad-evoked emotions have a more positive effect on consumers’ intention to share, brand attitude and attitude towards the ad. An experiment was conducted where two viral video ads were

compared: One that evoked positive emotions and one that evoked negative emotions with the

participants. As hypothesized, results indicated that positive ad-evoked emotions have a more

positive effect on consumers’ intention to share, brand attitude and attitude towards the ad

compared to negative ad-evoked emotions. To control the viral success of viral video ads,

marketers should create video ads that evoke primarily positive emotions with the viewer.

Introduction

Consumers are constantly exposed to attempts from companies to gain their attention. This

causes a growing selectivity (MindComet, 2006) and companies are realizing that new

marketing strategies are required to be able to overcome consumers’ selective burden. One

effective strategy that has established itself in the media landscape is to increase consumers’

involvement in the company’s marketing processes (Vargo & Lusch, 2004). Increased

(3)

2 viral advertising has shown to be an appropriate tool for providing exactly this active

participation (Trusov, Bucklin, & Pauwels, 2009).

The term ‘viral advertising’ was first introduced by Knights in 1996 and refers to a marketing strategy that focusses on spreading commercial information about a brand’s

product or the brand itself from one user to another through the internet like a virus. This can

be a viral video, image or written text. To get spread, viral advertising intends to affect

peoples’ intention to share the advertisement with others through different social media

channels. As a consequence of peoples’ sharing behavior, the ad gets viral and reaches a huge

amount of potential customers in a short time (Lindgren & Vanhamme, 2005). However, if

viral success lies in the power of affecting peoples’ intention to share, what are the features

that drives consumers to pass the advertisement on to their peers? Some researchers suggested

that the emotions that get evoked with the viewer when being exposed to a viral ad involves

people in a process of social sharing of these emotions to search for social agreement and

emotional confirmation (Dobele, Toleman, & Beverland, 2005; Phelps, Lewis, Mobilio,

Perry, & Raman, 2004).

In real life, there is a huge difference between viral ads regarding the emotions they

evoke with the viewer. Most of them intend to evoke positive emotions as fun, pleasure and

inspiration, but some others intend to evoke negative emotions like fear, shame and anger. At

first sight, negative ad-evoked emotions seem to be less effective when positively influencing consumers’ intention to share compared to positive ad-evoked emotions. Still, some marketers create viral video ads that evoke mainly negative emotions with the consumers during

exposure. We assume that this can result either from a lack of knowledge that negative

ad-evoked emotions are not effective in positively influencing peoples’ intention to share or from

the fact that negative ad-evoked emotions actually are more effective. To clarify the

difference in effect between positive and negative ad-evoked emotions in the context of viral

(4)

3 ad-evoked emotions in the context of the current most extensively used viral advertising tool

of viral video ads. However, before people actually share an advertisement, they form an

attitude towards the advertisement and an attitude towards the brand promoted in the ad (Lutz,

1977; Olson, Portner, & Bell, 1982; Wright, 1973). Because these variables are found to be

relevant in the process of intention formation, the variables attitude towards the ad and brand

attitude were also taken into account in the present study. Therefore, the main purpose was:

To what extent do positive ad-evoked emotions and negative ad-evoked emotions by viral

video ads have a positive effect on consumers’ intention to share, brand attitude and attitude

towards the ad and is this effect mediated by their attitude towards the advertisement?

Theoretical background

The difference in effect between positive and negative ad-evoked emotions on intention to

share, brand attitude and attitude towards the ad in viral advertising appears to be an

under-researched phenomenon. First, previous research focused on particular ad-evoked emotions

by investigating the effect per emotion as fear, surprise, inspiration, sadness and amusement.

However, the researchers made no comparison between these particular ad-evoked emotions

to investigate whether some of them are more effective than others in positively influencing

consumers’ intention to share, brand attitude and attitude towards the ad. Second, most of previous research focused on ad-evoked emotions by traditional advertising as radio and

television ads and not by viral advertising as viral video ads.

The next sections will narrow down the relevant concepts for this study, starting with

viral video ads. After, the independent variable of ad-evoked emotions will be defined. Here,

the categorization of positive and negative ad-evoked emotions will be explained based on

Lang's Limited Capacity Model of Motivated Mediated Message Processing (LC4MP).

(5)

4 relationship to evoked emotions will be discussed based on previous research on

ad-evoked emotions to explain the formulated hypotheses.

Viral videos

Previous studies on ad-evoked emotions focused mostly on ad-evoked emotions in traditional

advertising. Contrary, this study aimed to test ad-evoked emotions in the currently very

relevant marketing field of viral advertising. One of the extensively used viral advertising

tools is the viral video ad (MindComet, 2006). Viral video ads refer to online video

advertisements that get spread over the internet within a viral marketing campaign

(MindComet, 2006). Poels and Dewitte (2006) suggested viral video ads to be an appropriate

medium to measure consumers’ motivations affected by emotional stimuli. According to

Poels and Dewitte (2006) viral video ads have the potential to engage more complex and

intense emotional processes than viral images or viral written content because of their high

emotional arousal, integrating audio and image that is able to evoke strong emotions with the

viewer. Based on the recommendation of Poels and Dewitte (2006), the present study found

viral video ads to be the most relevant and appropriate viral advertising tool to measure

ad-evoked emotions and their effect on peoples’ motivations towards an ad after exposure.

Positive and negative ad-evoked emotions

Van Raaij (1984) described ad-evoked emotions as "primary affective reactions to an

advertisement” (p.78) and compared ad-evoked emotions to the emotional affect the viewer

encounters when being exposed to an advertisement. The term ‘affect’ is used by many

researchers when referring to ad-evoked emotions as for example by Bagozzi, Gobinath and

Nyer (1999) who tested emotional affect to measure the feelings that get evoked by television

ads by different health organizations. Because of the suggested similarity, the present study

used the terms emotional affect and ad-evoked emotions interchangeably. Within emotional

(6)

5 in line with the former suggested categories of negative and positive ad-evoked emotions.

Here, negative affect is defined as a personality variable that involves the experience

of negative emotions (Watson & Clark, 1984). In line with this, negative affect refers to the

experience of negative emotions evoked by an advertisement. Negative affectivity subsumes a

variety of negative emotions, including anger, contempt, disgust, guilt, and fear and

nervousness. Positive affect is defined as a characteristic that describes how humans

experience positive emotions (Watson & Clark, 1984). In line with this, positive affect refers

to the experience of negative emotions evoked by an advertisement. Those with high positive

affectivity are typically enthusiastic, energetic, confident, active, and alert.

Previous research on ad-evoked emotions tested the effect per ad-evoked emotion as

amusement, fun, and inspiration for example that are all of positive nature and ad-evoked

emotions as fear, sadness, irritation and dissatisfaction for example that are all of negative

nature. Previous research thus indirectly tested both categories of positive and negative

ad-evoked emotions but did not explicitly assigned these to two categories to make them

comparable. Assigning these ad-evoked emotions to a positive and negative category is also

suggested by the extensively applied theoretical framework about the cognition motivated

processing of emotional media content: Lang's Limited Capacity Model of Motivated

Mediated Message Processing (LC4MP) (Lang & Bradley, 2010). This model argues that

humans’ emotions and cognition are acting dependently of each other. This helps the human

mind to evaluate the motivational significance of encountered stimuli in messages as

advertising and helps creating appropriate responses to these messages or ads as for example

to share them (Lang & Bradley 2010).

According to Lang (2009) media content contains a range of motivationally significant

stimuli that activates peoples’ motivations as for example peoples’ sharing intentions. Lang

(2009) describes media content as a series of sensory information and this sensory

(7)

6 respond to the media content. Here, Lang (2009) describes two motivational systems, the

appetitive motivational system and the aversive motivational system. The appetitive

motivational system affects peoples’ response to perceived pleasantness of stimuli in media

content and results in approach-related or proactive behavior and the aversive motivational

system is activated in response to perceived unpleasantness which results in defensive,

avoidance-related or reactive behavior (Lang & Bradley, 2010).

Earlier, Watson and Tellegen (1985) described the relationship between perceived

pleasantness and perceived unpleasantness of media content and positive and negative

emotions. They argued that when perceiving pleasant media content, people engage a state of

positive affectivity which evokes positive emotions and the viewer respond with

approach-related behavior or proactive behavior, thus having the intention to share the media content

with others. Contrary, when perceiving unpleasant media content people engage a state of

negative affectivity which automatically evokes negative emotions and the viewer respond

with defensive, avoidance-related behavior or reactive behavior, thus having the intention not

to share the media content with others.

According to Lang and Bradley (2010) there are thus two emotional categories being

evoked by media content like an advertisement: Positive and negative ad-evoked emotions.

The authors also suggested that positive ad-evoked emotions are more effective in positively

influencing peoples’ intention to share as these causes proactive behavior. Based on the model

of LC4MP framework by Lang (2009) we expected that also in the present research positive

ad-evoked emotions have a more positive effect on people’s intention to share, brand attitude

and attitude towards the ad compared to negative ad-evoked emotions. Therefore, we decided

investigate the difference in effect between ad-evoked emotions on consumers’ intention to

share, brand attitude and attitude towards the ad by comparing the two categories of positive

(8)

7

Ad-evoked and the dependent variables

The dependent variables intention to share, brand attitude and attitude towards the ad are

suggested by Lutz (1977), Olson et al. (1982) and Wright (1973) within the cognitive

response model to measure consumers’ responses after exposure to an advertisement. This

study used these terms in order to measure the effect that ad-evoked emotions by viral video

ads have on consumers’ attitudes and intention towards the ad. The researchers argued that

peoples’ exposure to commercial content as an advertisement results in certain cognitive

responses: First, in attitude formation towards the advertisement itself (in the present study, ad

attitude), after in attitude formation towards the brand (brand attitude) which is promoted in

the ad and in behavioral intentions towards the ad (intention to share the ad).

Ad-evoked emotions and intention to share.Real life campaigns show that viral advertising effectively overcomes consumers’ selective burden as consumers show to extensively share these ads with others. The feature that makes them share the viral ad is suggested to be the

emotional connection which is built between the viewer and the viral ad during exposure

(Holbrook & Batra, 1987). Thus, the emotions that get evoked with the viewer by the viral ad

during exposure seems to influence consumers’ intention formation. Intention is assumed by Fishbein and Ajzen (1975) “to capture the motivational factors that influence behaviors; it is an indication of an individual’s willingness and readiness to behave” (p. 105). Thus, an

individual’s intention to share an advertisement as a viral video determines his or her behavior to actually share the ad with others. Previous research suggested a relationship between the

emotions that get evoked by a viral advertisement and the intention to share this ad with

others via multiple social media channels.

In their study about users’ forwarding behavior of e-mails, Phelps et al. (2004) tested

the effect for both particular positive and negative emotions and showed that "messages that

(9)

8 forwarded to others" (p. 335). Both positive and negative ad-evoked emotions seem to have

an effect on peoples’ intentions towards the ad. However, these researcher did not compare

positive and negative evoked emotions to investigate whether positive and negative

ad-evoked emotions differ significantly in their effect on consumers’ intentions to forward the

message to their peers. Also Lindgreen and Vanhamme (2005) tested the effect for different

particular ad-evoked emotions. They found that people forward messages to others when

experiencing the emotion of fun and surprise. They conclude that "emotions that are evoked

with the viewer are the key drivers of viral messages" (p. 98).Based on previous findings, we

expected the emotions evoked by a viral ad to be the driver of consumers’ intention to share

the ad with others on social media channels (e.g. Facebook).

Dobele et al. (2007) first made an attempt to explain the underlying processes of the

relationship between ad-evoked emotions and the intention to share by discussing the process

of social sharing of emotions. According to Dobele et al. (2007), consumers want to share the

experienced emotions socially with others. They become social emotions. Dobele et al. (2007)

asked the participants in their study why they have the intention to share the ad and the

answer was ‘because sharing them connected emotionally’. Thus, by sharing the ad people also share the emotions that the advertisement evokes with them. Therefore, the whole sharing

process becomes not only a process of sharing the ad with others but also a process of a social

sharing of emotions (Dobele et al., 2005). According to these findings, ad-evoked emotions

appear to have an effect on consumers’ intention to share and ultimately make these ads go

viral.

Previous research found evidence for the relationship of ad-evoked emotions and the

intention to share. However, those studies investigated the effect for particular positive and

particular negative ad-evoked emotions on the intention to share without comparing these two

categories to investigate whether one is more effective. Based on the discussed research and

(10)

9 emotions in their effect on intention to share. We hypothesized that positive ad-evoked

emotions by viral video ads have a more positive effect on consumers’ intention to share

compared to negative ad-evoked emotions. The following hypothesis was tested:

Hypothesis 1: Positive ad-evoked emotions by viral video ads have a more positive effect on consumers’ intention to share compared to negative ad-evoked emotions.

Ad-evoked emotions and brand attitude. Before forming an intention to share the ad,

people form a certain attitude towards the brand which is promoted in the ad (Lutz, 1977;

Olson et al., 1982; Wright (1973). According to Schwarz and Clore (2007), consumers

evaluates advertisements by analyzing their emotions during exposure and interpreting what

these emotions mean for the brand in the ad. This results in an attitude valence which is

defined as the degree of positivity or negativity with which an attitude object (in the current

context, a brand) is evaluated (Schwarz & Clore, 2007).

The relationship of ad-evoked emotions and brand attitude is already found in the

1980s where an influential series of experimental lab studies showed that emotions evoked by

various print, television and radio ads influence consumers' attitude formation towards the

brand in the ad (Aaker, Stayman, & Hagerty, 1986; Batra & Ray, 1986; Edell & Burke, 1987;

Holbrook & Batra, 1987). Contrary to the discussed research on ad-evoked emotions and

intention to share, these studies did make a comparison between positive and negative

ad-evoked emotions and their effect on brand attitude. Participants in these studies reported more

favourable brand attitudes after exposure to the ad that intended to evoke positive emotions

compared to ads that intended to evoke unpleasant emotions with the viewer.

Brown, Homer and Imman (1988) supported these influential series of studies by

making a meta-analysis on the effect of ad-evoked emotions and brand attitude. They found

(11)

10 research on this relationship was around r = .35 to .40. Cohen (1988) considered this effect to

be ‘medium to large’.

However, these studies are all conducted in a time when traditional marketing in the

form of radio and television ads were the main way marketers communicated to their

customers. Thus, previous studies tested the effect of ad-evoked emotions in the context of

traditional advertising, not viral advertising. Therefore, the present study aimed to broaden the

effect of positive and negative ad-evoked emotions on brand attitude besides television and

print ads to viral ads like viral video ads. As shown in previous research about ad-evoked

emotions and brand attitude, also in this study we expected positive ad-evoked emotions by

viral ads to have a more positive effect on consumers’ attitude towards the brand compared to

negative ad-evoked emotions. The following hypothesis was tested:

Hypothesis 2: Positive ad-evoked emotions by viral video ads have a more positive effect on consumers’ brand attitude compared to negative ad-evoked emotions.

Ad-evoked emotions and the attitude towards the ad. The third cognitive response during

exposure to advertising is the attitude towards the ad itself (Lutz, 1977; Olson et al., 1982;

Wright, 1973). Attitude towards the ad is defined as “predisposition to respond in a favorable

or unfavorable manner to a particular advertising stimulus during a particular exposure

occasion” (Lutz, 1985, p. 21).

The relationship of ad-evoked emotions and attitude towards the ad is suggested by

Edell and Burke (1987) and Holbrook and Batra (1987) who found the effect of ad-evoked

emotions in traditional advertisements as print, radio and television ads. Contrary to previous

research on evoked emotions, Royo-Vela and Marcela (2005) later tested various

ad-evoked emotions and their effect on peoples’ attitude towards the ad in the context of viral

ads. He investigated the effect of the ad-evoked emotion of joy, fun, irritation, satisfaction and

(12)

11 that the ad-evoked emotions joy, fun and satisfaction did elicit more positive attitudes towards

the ad compared to the emotions irritation and fear.

These findings indicate that positive ad-evoked emotions are likely to have a more

positive effect on attitude towards the ad compared to negative ad-evoked emotions.

However, these researchers tested the effect per particular ad-evoked emotion and did not

compare those to conclude whether some do have a more positive effect on consumers’

attitude towards the ad than others. In line with the previous study by Royo-Vela and Marcela

(2005), we expected positive ad-evoked emotions to have a more positive effect on

consumers’ attitude towards the advertisement compared to negative ad-evoked emotions: Hypothesis 3: Positive ad-evoked emotions by viral video ads have a more positive effect on consumers’ attitude towards the advertisement compared to negative ad-evoked emotions.

Besides being an outcome variable, attitude towards the ad is found by previous studies to act

as a mediator in the relationship of ad-evoked emotions on brand attitude and the intention to

share. First, the mediating role of attitude towards the ad in the relationship of ad-evoked

emotions and brand attitude is explained in the work of Mitchell and Olson (1981) and Shimp

(1981). The researchers found that attitude towards the ad treated as affective reaction

towards an ad (Lutz, 1985), can also mediate attitude towards the brand. These studies

supports the mediating role of attitude towards the ad on brand attitude, however they did not

test this relationship in the context of viral advertising but traditional advertising. In the

present study, we intended to indicate that this mediating role exists also in the context of the

more recent viral advertising. Therefore, we retested the mediating role in the context of viral

ads. In line with previous research, we expected attitude towards the ad also in the context of

viral ads to be a mediator in the relationship of ad-evoked emotions and brand attitude. The

(13)

12 Hypothesis 4: The effect of ad-evoked emotions by viral video ads on consumers’ brand attitude is mediated by consumer’s attitude towards the ad.

Second, the mediating role of attitude towards the ad in the relationship of ad-evoked

emotions and intention to share can be traced to the earlier mentioned cognitive response

model by Lutz (1977), Olson et al. (1982) and Wright (1973). In terms of attitude towards the

ad as predictor of intention to share, this model suggests that peoples’ exposure to an

advertisement first evokes a cognitive response with the viewer, which will affect attitude

formation, and the attitude will in return result in the formation of intentions. This process

results in a chain of reactions: cognitive response → attitude → intention. Applied to the

present study, we expected participants to form an attitude towards the advertisement first

after exposure which results in the intention formation to share or not to share the viral video

ad with others. However, this mediating role was only tested in traditional advertising, not in

the context of viral advertising. Based on the cognitive response model, we hypothesized

attitude towards the ad to act as a predictor of consumers’ intention to share a viral video ad.

The following hypothesis was tested:

Hypothesis 5: The effect of ad-evoked emotions by viral video ads on consumers’ intention to share is mediated by consumer’s attitude towards the ad.

Method

Design

The proposed hypotheses were addressed with an experiment that incorporated a one factor

between subjects design. To measure the difference in effect between positive and negative

ad-evoked emotions by viral video ads on the dependent variables intention to share, brand

attitude and attitude towards the ad, the emotions in a real life viral video ad were

(14)

13 video ad which was presumed to evoke positive emotions with the viewer and the original one

where the emotions have been manipulated. After manipulation, this version was presumed to

evoke negative emotions with the viewer. These two viral videos ads were compared to

investigate the difference in effect of positive and negative ad-evoked emotions on the

mentioned dependent variables.

Sample

For this experiment, we considered Dutch and German male and female Facebook users with

the age of 18 to 30 years. These participants are all members of Generation Y, those who were

born between 1982 and 2001 (Sheahan, 2005). This generation makes up 22.5 % (CBS, 2014)

of the Dutch and 20.4% (BMI, 2014) of the German population and therefore gives a broad reflection of the Dutch and German society’s attitude and beliefs about the research topic. Second, previous studies found this generation to be the major drivers and consumers of

social media and therefore viral video ads (Berthon, Pitt, & Des Autels, 2011). Consequently,

this generation’s consumers are all familiar with viral video ads on social network sites and

know how to share them. Third, by considering only German and Dutch users we aimed to

measure users’ attitudes and intention that are exclusively evoked by the emotions in the viral

video ad and not by other influential factors. German and Dutch participants were expected to

be almost unfamiliar with the chosen viral video ad and the source, the brand John Lewis.

John Lewis is a national British brand that has no stores outside the UK. Therefore, the participants’ emotions were expected to be evoked by the advertisement itself and not influenced by prior prejudices that participants would have when being familiar with the

brand John Lewis. In the UK, where the John Lewis video ad was launched, people showed to

share it, so we expected that Germans and Dutch would do likewise in this study.

In total 123 respondents participated in the experiment. 31 participants were excluded

(15)

14 participants were excluded from analysis because 3 of them indicated to have already seen the

viral video ad before participating in this experiment and 3 of them did not fit in the age group

of 18 to 30 years. Another 2 indicated to have an ‘other’ country of origin besides Germany or

The Netherlands and were therefore deleted from the data set. This left a sample of 84

participants with an age ranging from 18 to 30, and a mean age of 22.85 (SD = 2.66). 47.6%

of the participants were male and 52.4% were female, 34.5% of them were of German and

65.5% of Dutch origin. Most of the respondents were highly educated, with 31% enrolled in a

university and 58.3% enrolled in a university of applied sciences. The participants were split

up into two groups referring to the two conditions, the ‘positive ad-evoked emotions’ condition (n = 40) and the ‘negative ad-evoked emotions’ condition (n = 44).

Procedure

In this experiment, we used an online questionnaire in which the viral video ad was

integrated. Participants were contacted via the social network site Facebook. With the

Facebook option of private messaging, it was possible to control for a specific sample that

met the conditions for age and country of origin and consider different participants for the

main research and the pretest. The online questionnaire was created with the help of the

online survey tool Qualtrics. A group of 326 participants were invited to participate in this

research (see content of this private message with invitation for research in Appendix 2).

These users were sent a brief invitation with the referring Qualtrics link asking to fill in the

questionnaire. When participants clicked on the link, they were transferred to the Qualtrics

website and randomly assigned to one of the two conditions, thus to one of the two videos. On

the Qualtrics website, they first visited a page displaying the factsheet of this study in which

they got informed about the privacy policy of their personal data and the ethical rights of

being a participant in this scientific experiment (see the factsheet in Appendix 3). The next

(16)

15 to declare that they understand the terms and agree to the described research policy of the

present study (see the informed consent in Appendix 4). After the privacy policy agreement,

an introductory text explained that the participant will first get to see a video advertisement of

two minutes. Here, it was mentioned that this video is an advertisement by the British

department store John Lewis. Both video names were changed and uploaded on Vimeo to

hide information as amount of views and the real title which could influence participants’

emotions.

All participants, independently of the group, filled in the same questionnaire

measuring the later discussed dependent variables and the control variables. In the last part of

the questionnaire, the participants were thanked for participating in this experiment by

disclosing the research purpose of ad-evoked emotions in the context of viral video ads.

Besides, they were told about the opportunity to get informed about the research results by

contacting the researcher by mail. Moreover, they were invited to write down any questions

and comments about the research in the given text box.

Stimulus material

For the viral video ad, it was chosen for the John Lewis Christmas advertisement of 2013

because of various reasons (see screenshots of the videos in Appendix 1). First, there existed

two versions of this advertisement; the original version itself and a parody to this original.

Both versions were similar in audio and design but were presumed to evoke different

emotions. The original video ad was presumed to evoke positive emotions and the parody was

presumed to evoke negative emotions with the participants. Second, both versions received

high views on YouTube (original: 13 million views; parody: 8 million views, 03.05.14) as

consequence of a high rate of sharing on social media sites where the YouTube video was

embedded by users. Therefore, this video advertisement fulfilled the research purpose as the

(17)

16 within a viral marketing campaign. This made the video an appropriate stimulus material

within this research as this aimed to measure ad-evoked emotions in the context of viral

advertising. Both videos started with the same plot, telling the story of two best friends, a bear

and a hare, spending time together all year long until winter time when the bear starts his

winter sleep. From this scene on, the plot of the two versions continues in different directions.

The original video ad was created by John Lewis in November 2013 and launched on

television and on the brand’s social media sites (YouTube, Facebook and Twitter) as a part of

a marketing campaign to stimulate their Christmas business. From the turning scene, where

the two videos start to differentiate, the original employed positive emotions showing the bear

waking up from his winter sleep seeing the Christmas tree and is happy and excited to

experience his first joyful Christmas celebration as a surprise organized by his friends.

The parody was created by an anonymous source in November 2013 right after the

launch of the original video on YouTube. From the turning scene, the second part of this

video displayed a cruel and sad plot showing a bloody hare hunt where the hare together with

others was shot to serve as food within the Christmas dinner.

Pretest

A pretest was conducted to check whether the original video ad really evoked positive

emotions and the parody really evoked negative emotions to compare positive and negative

ad-evoked emotions in their effect on intention to share, brand attitude and attitude towards

the ad. As discussed before, the emotional affect was determined to measure ad-evoked

emotions. Therefore, the extensively used PANAS Scale by Tellegen, Watson and Clark

(1988) was applied. The PANAS scale consists of two separate scales: One measuring

Positive Affect (PA) and the other measuring Negative Affect (NA) (see PANAS scale in

Appendix 5.1). Both these scales, the PA and the NA, contain 10 items that describe feelings

(18)

17 disagree, 4 = neutral, 7=strongly agree) according to their subjective emotional state after being exposed to one of the two viral videos. 20 participants (10 participants per video), who

did not take part in the final experiment, were approached to take part in the pretest. The 10

items of each affect scale, the PA and the NA, were tested to composite a reliable factor scale

through Cronbach’s α. A principal components factor analysis with Varimax rotation indicated that all 10 separate items of the PA measure one construct, only one components

had an eigenvalue above 1 (EV = 9.32), also indicated by an inflexion in the scree plot. The

factor explained 93.14% of the variance. The PA scale was found to be reliable (α = .99; M =

4.07, SD = 1.13). The principal components factor analysis with Varimax rotation for the NA

indicated that all 10 separate items measured one construct, only one component had an

eigenvalue above 1 (EV = 9.40), also indicated by an inflexion in the scree plot. The factor

explained 93.60% of the variance. Also, the NA scale was found to be reliable (α = .99; M =

3.66, SD = 1.04).

An independent-samples t-test was conducted to determine whether the manipulation

of emotion worked. The results indicated that there was a significant difference in scores in

the positive group (M = 5.90, SD = 1.47) and the negative group (M = 2.23, SD = 1.23) in the

PA scale, t(18) = 20.59, p<.001, 95% CI [3.30, 4.05] and also there was a significant

difference in scores in the positive group (M = 1.60, SD = 1.02) and the negative group (M =

5.71, SD = 1.12) in the NA scale, t(18) = 25.33, p<.001, 95% CI [-3.77, 4.45].

The pretest showed that the manipulation of emotion was successful. The original

video ad did significantly evoke more positive emotions than negative emotions with the

participants and the parody did significantly evoke more negative than positive emotions. The

two videos were thus comparable to measure the difference in effect between positive and

negative ad-evoked emotions on intention to share, brand attitude and attitude towards the ad.

Therefore, we named the original video ad in the following main research positive video and

(19)

18

Measurement

The online questionnaire created in Qualtrics contained four scales, measuring the three

dependent variables intention to share, attitude towards the brand and attitude towards the ad,

the control variables and the emotional affect checking for the manipulation of emotion. To

note, all questions in the survey were formulated in English in order to be understood by both

Dutch and German participants.

Intention to share. The first dependent variable in the questionnaire was measured with a

scale by Derbaix and Vanhamme (2003). They authors measured the intention to share by the

word-of-mouth that is created as a result of users’ sharing behavior. The three items ‘How

ready are you to share this video ad with your friends, family and colleagues (e.g. on

Facebook)?‘, ‘How likely is it that you are going to share this video ad with your friends,

family and colleagues (e.g. on Facebook)?’ and ‘Do you have the intention to share this video

ad with your friends, family and colleagues (e.g. on Facebook)?’ belonging to this scale, the

participants had to rate on a 7 point Likert scale (1= absolutely not, 4= neutral, 7=

absolutely). These three items were tested to composite a reliable factor scale through

Cronbach’s α. A principal components factor analysis with Varimax rotation indicated that all the separate items measured one construct. Only one component had an eigenvalue above 1

(EV = 2.88), also indicated by an inflexion in the scree plot. The factor explained 96.14% of

the variance. The sharing intention scale was found to be reliable (α = .98; M = 3.16, SD =

1.25). No items were excluded to improve the scales reliability (see scale for intention to

share in Appendix 5.1).

Brand attitude. To measure the second dependent variable, a scale designed by Spears and

Singhs (2004) was used in the questionnaire. Participants had to indicate their overall feeling

about the department store brand John Lewis promoted in the viral video ad by rating the

(20)

19 ‘bad’-‘good’, ‘unpleasant’-‘pleasant’, ‘unfavorable’-‘favorable’ and ‘unlikeable -‘likeable’. A principal components factor analysis with Varimax rotation indicated that the 5 items

measured one construct. Only one component had an eigenvalue above 1 (EV = 4.81), also

indicated by an inflexion in the scree plot. The factor explained 96.14% of the variance. The

brand attitude scale was found to be reliable (α = .99; M = 3.89, SD = 0.98). No items were

excluded in order to improve the scales reliability (see scale for attitude towards the brand in

Appendix 5.2).

Attitude towards the ad. In order to measure the mediator variable, participants’ attitude

towards the advertisement, an adapted scale which was designed by Mitchell and Olson

(1981) was found to be convenient for this research. Participants had to indicate their overall

feeling about the video ad with the help of the four items ‘bad’-‘good’, ‘dislike’-‘like’,

‘irritating’-‘not irritating’ and ‘uninteresting’-‘interesting’ on a 7 point semantic differential scale. A principal components factor analysis with Varimax rotation indicated that all four

items belonging to the scale measured one construct. Only one component had an eigenvalue

above 1 (EV = 3.32), also indicated by an inflexion in the scree plot. The factor explained

82.99% of the variance. The brand attitude scale was found to be reliable (α = .93; M = 4.02,

SD = 1.12) (see scale for attitude towards the ad in Appendix 5.3).

Control variables. To control for the set characteristics of the target group, the questionnaire

included the following control variables: age, gender, level of education, and familiarity with

the John Lewis advertisement. In order to control for age, an open question was posed (‘what

is your age?’). Participants were asked to fill in their age in numbers. Gender was measured with one question (‘what is your gender?’). Participants had to choose between ‘male’ of ‘female’. After, the participants were asked for their level of education by indicating their latest education diploma on four given levels (no degree, high school, university and

(21)

20 with the advertisement before participating in this research, the participants had to indicate

whether they were ever being confronted to the advertisement (‘Where you familiar with the

advertisement before seeing it in this study?’). They indicated their familiarity with ‘Yes’ or

‘No’. To be sure about the participants were exposed to the manipulation of emotion located in the second part of the video, they had to state whether they were watching the video till the end (‘Yes’ or ‘No’). Besides, it was controlled for participants’ country of origin by giving the choices: The Netherlands, Germany and Other.

Ad-evoked emotions. In order to check for the ad-evoked emotions by the positive and

negative video, the PANAS scale measuring the emotional effect as used in the pretest was

implemented in the questionnaire (see PANAS scale in Appendix 5.4). The items ‘proud’ and ‘attentive’ of PA had to be omitted in order to let all items load on one component. A

principal components factor analysis with Varimax rotation indicated that the left 8 separate

items of the PA measured one construct, only one components had an eigenvalue above 1

(EV = 6.83), also indicated by an inflexion in the scree plot. The factor explained 85.41% of

the variance. The PA scale was found to be reliable (α = .98; M = 3.69, SD = 0.98). With the

NA the item ‘ashamed’ had to be omitted in order to let all items load on one component. The

principal components factor analysis with Varimax rotation indicated that the left 9 separate

items measured one construct, only one component had an eigenvalue above 1 (EV = 7.71),

also indicated by an inflexion in the scree plot. The factor explained 85.62% of the variance.

The NA scale was found to be reliable (α =.98; M = 3.25, SD = 0.83).

Results

Randomization check

To be sure of two similar samples that were comparable, it was necessary that the two groups

in this experiment did not differ significantly in characteristics such as age, gender, country of

(22)

21 differences between the experimental groups with respect to age for the positive group and the

negative group, F(1,82) = .000, p =.988. Chi-square analyses showed that there were no

differences between the two experimental groups with respect to country of origin, x²(1) =.69,

p =.406, neither with respect to gender, x²(1) = .17, p =.677 and there were no differences between the groups also with respect to level of education, x²(3) = .590, p =.899.

Manipulation check

An independent-samples t-test was conducted to determine whether the manipulation of

emotion worked in this experiment and the positive video did evoke more positive emotions

with the participants during exposure compared to the negative video and the negative video

did evoke more negative emotions compared to the positive video. The results demonstrate

that there was a significant difference in scores in the positive video group and the negative

video group in the PA scale, t(82) = 8.01, p<.001, 95% CI [-4.14, -2.50] and also there was a

significant difference in scores in the positive video group and the negative video group in the

NA scale, t(82) = 10.07, p<.001, 95% CI [2.66, 3.97]. The positive video that was presumed

to evoke positive emotions with the participants scored significantly higher (M = 5.12, SD =

2.10) in the PA scale than the negative video that was presumed to evoke negative emotions

with the participants (M = 1.80, SD = 1.69). The negative video that was presumed to evoke

negative emotions with the participants scored significantly higher (M = 4.69, SD = 2.02) in

the NA scale than the positive video that was presumed to evoke positive emotions with the

participants (M = 1.38, SD = 0.54). In line with the results of the pretest, the manipulation of

emotion was also successful in the main research. Therefore, all conclusions in this

experiment can be derived from comparing both groups.

Hypothesis testing

To test the proposed hypothesis, various Analyses of Variance had been conducted. First,

(23)

22 differences in main effect of positive and negative ad-evoked emotions on the dependent

variables intention to share, brand attitude and attitude towards the ad between the two

groups. The covariates age, gender, country of origin and level of education were integrated in

this measurement to consider the effects of the covariates on the dependent variables. The

multivariate results were significant for ad-evoked emotions, Pillai’s Trace = .794, F(3,76) =

97.71, p<.001, indicating a difference in intention to share, brand attitude and attitude towards

the ad between the positive and negative group. Results of the univariate F tests for the three

dependant variables were significant, as shown in detail below. The mean scores with

standard deviations for the positive and negative ad-evoked emotions per dependant variable

are shown in Table 1. Second, a mediation analysis using the regression analysis PROCESS

by Hayes (2012) was conducted to test the indirect effect of ad-evoked emotions on the

dependent variables intention to share and brand attitude via the mediator variable attitude

towards the ad.

Intention to share. The first hypothesis tested the main effect of ad-evoked emotions on the

intention to share between the two groups, stating that participants in the positive group are

more intended to share the ad compared to subjects in the negative group. The univariate F

tests demonstrated that there was a significant difference between the positive group and the

negative group for intention to share, F(1,78) = 115.80, p<.001. Participants who had seen the

positive video scored higher on intention to share (M= 5.07, SD= 2.11) than those who had

seen the negative video (M= 1.42, SD= 0.73). Based on these results, H1 was supported.

Positive ad-evoked emotions by viral video ads are found to have a more positive effect on

the intention to share compared to negative ad-evoked emotions.

Brand attitude. The second hypothesis tested the main effect of ad-evoked emotions on

attitude towards the brand between the two groups, stating that the participants in the positive

(24)

23 univariate F tests showed that there was a significant difference between the positive group

and the negative group for brand attitude, F(1,78) = 292.04, p<.001. Participants who had

seen the positive video indicated to have a more positive attitude towards the brand (M= 6.01,

SD= 0.98) than those who had seen the negative video (M= 1.96, SD= 1.12). Therefore, H2 is supported. Positive ad-evoked emotions by viral video ads are found to have a more positive

effect on brand attitude compared to negative ad-evoked emotions.

Attitude towards the ad. The third hypothesis tested the main effect of ad-evoked emotions

on attitude towards the ad, stating that participants in the positive group have a more positive

attitude towards the ad after exposure than participants in the negative group. The univariate F

tests showed there was a significant difference between the positive group and the negative

group for attitude towards the advertisement, F(1,78) = 213.80, p<.001. Participants who had

seen the positive video scored higher on attitude towards the ad (M= 5.94, SD= 1.01) than

those who had seen the negative video who intended to have a less positive attitude towards

the ad after exposure (M= 2.26, SD= 1.23). Hence, H3 is supported. Positive ad-evoked

emotions by viral video ads were found to have a more positive effect on attitude towards the

ad compared to negative ad-evoked emotions.

Table 1. A comparison of means (with standard deviations) for positive and negative ad-evoked emotions for the dependent variables intention to share, brand attitude and attitude

towards the ad.

Positive emotion Negative emotion

Intention to share 5.07 (2.11)a 1.42 (.73)a

Attitude towards the brand 6.01 (.98)a 1.96 (1.12)a

Attitude towards the ad 5.94 (1.01)a 2.26 (1.23)a

(25)

24

Mediation effect. The fourth and fifth hypothesis tested the mediation effect of ad-evoked

emotions on the two dependent variables brand attitude and the intention to share via the

mediator attitude towards the advertisement. In order to test whether the direct effect of

ad-evoked emotions on the two dependant variables is mediated by attitude towards the ad, a

mediation analysis was conducted. The mediation model (shown in Figure 1) was tested using Hayes’ PROCESS macro (Hayes, 2012). In the analyses for this experiment, 1,000 bootstrap samples were used to estimate the bias-corrected bootstrap confidence intervals (BCBCI).

Figure 1 represents the tested mediation model with ad-evoked emotions as the

independent variable, attitude towards the ad as the mediator, and brand attitude and intention

to share as the dependent variables. The paths in this figure correspond to the results in Table

2. The c-path in the model includes the direct effect of ad-evoked emotions on brand attitude

and intention to share, independent of the effect of the mediator (c′), and the total effect of

ad-evoked emotions on brand attitude and intention to share (c), which is the sum of the direct

effect and the indirect effect via the mediator (Hayes, 2012).

Figure 1. Tested mediation model: Effect of ad-evoked emotions on intention to share and brand attitude via attitude towards the ad.

The results in Table 2 show a significant effect of ad-evoked emotions on attitude towards the

ad (a) for both the model with brand attitude and intention to share as dependent variable (ba1

= 3.67, p<.001; ba2 = 3.67, p<.001). Second, there was a significant effect of attitude towards

a b c, c’ Ad-evoked emotions Attitude towards the ad Intention to share Brand attitude

(26)

25 the ad on both intention to share (bb1 = .77, p< .001) and brand attitude (bb2 = .70, p< .001).

The direct effect (c’) of emotion on brand attitude showed to be not significant, meaning that the effect of ad-evoked emotions was not significant without mediated by

attitude towards the ad (bc’2 = 1.45, p =.004). In line with this, bootstrapping showed

significant indirect effect via the mediator attitude towards the ad (indirect effect 2.58, SE =

.31, 95% BCBCI [2.021, 3.320]). Attitude towards the ad is thus the mediator in this effect.

Hence, H4 is supported, the effect of ad-evoked emotions on brand attitude is mediated by

attitude towards the ad. The direct effect (c’) of ad-evoked emotions on the intention to share

showed to be not significant (bc’1 =.76, p =.05), meaning that the direct effect of ad-evoked

emotions on intention to share is mediated by the attitude towards the ad. In line with this,

bootstrapping showed that the indirect effect of ad-evoked emotions on intention to share via

attitude towards the ad was significant in this experiment (indirect effect 2.81, SE = .58, 95%

BCBCI [1.830, 4.110]). Attitude towards the ad is thus the mediator in this effect. This

relationship supports H5, the effect of ad-evoked emotions on the intention to share is only

significant when mediated by attitude towards the ad.

Table 2. Mediation: Effect of ad-evoked emotions on brand attitude and intention to share via attitude towards the ad.

a b c (total) c’ (direct) Indirect effect (95% BCBCI) Intention to share (1) 3.67 (.25)*** .77 (.12)*** 3.57 (.33)*** .76 (.51)+ 2.81 (.58) [1.830; 4.110] Brand attitude (2) 3.67 (.25)*** .70 (.07)*** 4.03 (.23)*** 1.45 (.30)+ 2.58 (.31) [2.021; 3.320]

Note: Unstandardized b-coefficients (with boot SE between parentheses) correspond to the paths in Figure 1; BCBCI =

Bias-corrected 1,000 bootstrap confidence interval; unstandardized b-coefficient for intention to share= b1 and for brand attitude=

(27)

26 To summarize, the results supported H1, H2, H3, H4 and H5: Compared to negative

ad-evoked emotions by viral video ads, positive ad-ad-evoked emotions showed to have a more

positive effect on intention to share, brand attitude and attitude towards the ad. Moreover,

ad-evoked emotions by viral video ads showed to influence both intention to share and brand

attitude via the mediator attitude towards the ad. This supports the mediation effects as

proposed with H4 and H5.

Discussion

The present study aimed to investigate the difference in effect between positive and negative

ad-evoked emotions by viral video ads on consumers’ intention to share and brand attitude

and the role attitude towards the advertisement plays in this relationship. A model was

proposed in which positive ad-evoked emotions have a more positive effect on the intention to

share, brand attitude and attitude towards the ad compared to negative ad-evoked emotions.

Results indicated that this model was supported. Participants who were exposed to the

positive video scored significantly higher on brand attitude, intention to share and attitude

towards the ad compared to participants who were exposed to the negative video.

Two goals were set for this study. First, we aimed to complete the literature in the field

of ad-evoked emotions and viral advertising. Most of previous research investigated the effect

of ad-evoked emotions in traditional advertising like in radio, television and print ads as most

of the previous research was conducted between the 1970’s and the 2000’s (Aaker, Stayman,

& Hagerty, 1986; Batra & Ray, 1986; Edell & Burke, 1987; Holbrook & Batra, 1987). This

study aimed to fill in that gap and examined the effect of ad-evoked emotions in the nowadays

very relevant marketing tool of viral advertising. This was successful. The present study

examined the effect of ad-evoked emotions by the most extensively used viral advertising

tool, the viral video ad. Second, most of previous research on ad-evoked emotions as the

(28)

27 ad-evoked emotion and made no comparison between ad-evoked emotions. Contrary, we

aimed to compare the effect of positive and negative ad-evoked emotions on consumers’

intention to share, brand attitude and attitude towards the ad. Also this was successful,

demonstrating that both positive and negative ad-evoked emotions were found to have an

effect on intention to share, brand attitude and attitude towards the ad. Here, positive

ad-evoked emotions were found to have a significantly more positive effect compared to negative

ad-evoked emotions. Results demonstrate that the effect of ad-evoked emotions on both brand

attitude and the intention to share is mediated by the attitude towards the ad. Based on the

results, the research question of this study was successfully answered.

To make sure that when comparing the two viral videos, positive and negative

ad-evoked emotions were compared, this study pretested the presumed ad-ad-evoked emotions of

the two viral videos. Here, we found that the presumed positive video, which was the original

John Lewis ad, really evoked positive emotions with the viewer and the presumed negative

video, which was the John Lewis parody, really did evoke negative emotions. Consequently,

the two viral videos were comparable in terms of positive and negative ad-evoked emotions.

The relationship between ad-evoked emotions and the intention to share as presumed

based on the discussed LC4MP by Lang and Bradley (2010) was also found in this study. The

researchers argued that perceived pleasantness of an ad results in a state of positive affectivity

which evokes positive emotions with the viewer that ultimately results in proactive behavior.

In this study, this was shown by participants’ behavior of having the intention to share the

positive video. Contrary, perceived unpleasantness in an ad make people engage in a state of

negative affectivity which evokes negative emotions and results reactive behavior. In this study, this was shown by participants’ behavior of not having the intention to share the negative video.

In terms of the relationship of ad-evoked emotions and brand attitude, previous

(29)

28 emotions (Aaker, Stayman, & Hagerty, 1986; Batra & Ray, 1986; Edell & Burke, 1987;

Holbrook & Batra, 1987). The findings of this study support the assumptions of earlier

research, but were first in testing this effect in the context of viral ads whereas previous

studies focused on traditional ads as print, television and radio ads. Therefore, this study

provides a more current insight in how to positively affect consumers brand attitude with the

help of viral advertising.

In terms of the relationship of ad-evoked emotions and attitude towards the ad,

previous research tested the effects of ad-evoked emotions on attitude towards the ad either in

the context of traditional ads as television and radio ads (Edell & Burke, 1987; Holbrook &

Batra, 1987) or did test them in viral ads but then per ad-evoked emotion and made no

comparison between these ad-evoked emotions as in the study of Royo-Vela and Marcela

(2005). Therefore, results of this research completed the literature by comparing positive and

negative ad-evoked emotions by viral video ads in their effect on consumer’s attitude towards

the ad.

Besides investigating attitude towards the ad as outcome variable, this study tested this

variable as mediator in the effect of ad-evoked emotions on both brand attitude and the

intention to share. Results were in line with the cognitive response model by Lutz (1977),

Olson et al. (1982) and Wright (1973) who suggested that during exposure to an

advertisement there is always a process of cognitive responses taking place, starting with

attitude formation towards the ad and ending in intention formation towards the ad. However,

the present research completed the literature by testing this mediation effect of attitude

towards the ad within viral advertising instead of traditional advertising as the previous

research did. The mediating role of attitude towards the ad in the relationship of ad-evoked

emotions and brand attitude was also supported with the results of this study as Mitchell and

Olson (1981) and Shimp (1981) suggested beforehand. Still, these researchers tested

(30)

29 succeeded in completing the literature by testing the effect within viral advertising, especially

within the relevant viral ads of viral videos.

The practical implications of the present findings for designing viral video ads in real

life are significant. As discussed, with the findings of this study, marketers get more insight in

how to strengthen a positive brand attitude and how to enhance the intention to share and

attitude towards the ad of their consumers. By evoking positive emotions with the viewer

through their viral video ads, first marketers can control the viral success of their

advertisements as positive ad-evoked emotions were found to positively affect consumers’

intention to share significantly. Second, they can enhance their brand’s image as positive

ad-evoked emotions were found to positively affect consumers’ brand attitude. The mediating

effect of attitude towards the ad on intention to share and brand attitude shows marketers that

when evoking positive ad-evoked emotions with their consumers, they are able to affect not

only consumers’ direct attitude towards the ad, but automatically their intention to share and

brand attitude as indirect effect. As real life cases demonstrate, there are still some viral video

ads circulating recently that evoke mainly negative emotions (MindComet, 2006). The

findings of the present study first provide marketers with the needed information that there is

actually a difference between positive and negative ad-evoked emotions in their effect on

consumers’ intention to share, brand attitude and attitude towards the ad and that positive

ad-evoked emotions are significantly more effective here. By considering these findings,

marketers know in future how to design their viral video ads in order to make them go viral

and successfully enhance their consumers’ attitude towards their brand.

The limitations of this experiment are multidimensional. First, participants of this

experiment were mainly students with a higher level of educational (78% university of

applied science; 13% university) between 18 and 30 years old which is the prototypical

audience of Generation Y as discussed in the method section. However, the field of social

(31)

30 as assumed in this study, also Generation Z should be considered. This generation showed to

be even more active on social media sites and will therefore be the future consumers (Pillai,

Brakenhoff, & Müller, 2010). All consumers that were born after 1990 belong to this

Generation. Comparing to Generation Y, this Generation’s users form attitudes and intentions

even more via social network channels compared to Generation Y (Igel & Urquhart, 2012).

Here lies the chance of viral marketing as social media channels are the place where viral

marketing campaigns try to influence consumers’ attitudes and intentions. Therefore, future

research should also consider Generation Z users as they will be the future main target group

on social media platforms and therefore the target group of viral advertising.

A second limitation of this study addresses this study’s stimulus material. The pretest

and manipulation check demonstrated that the two viral videos were significantly different in

terms of the emotions they evoke and therefore appropriate to test the difference in effect

between positive and negative ad-evoked emotions. The original video ad did evoke

significantly more positive emotions compared to the parody which did evoke significantly

more negative emotions. These viral videos were thus comparable and the measurement of the

difference in effect was successful based on the chosen stimulus material. However, there was

also a relevant limitation. The parody was designed by an anonymous source and put on

YouTube right after launching of the original John Lewis ad. Both videos went viral on the

internet, thus both video were viral videos. However, the source of the parody was neither a

commercial one nor was the parody part of a viral marketing campaign as the original video

ad was. Therefore, the parody was not an advertisement and did not fit in the context of this

research which was viral advertising. Still, we used this parody in this study because it was

found to evoke negative ad-evoked emotions and had a similar plot as the original John Lewis

ad. Ideally, future research should compare ad-evoked emotions by two viral video ads that

are part of a viral marketing campaign in their effect on intention to share, brand attitude and

(32)

31

References

Aaker, D. A., Stayman, D. M., & Hagerty, M. R. (1986). Warmth in advertising:

Measurement, impact and sequence effects. Journal of Consumer Research, 12(4),

365–381.

Bagozzi, R. P., Gobinath, M., & Nyer, P. U. (1999). The role of emotions in marketing.

Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, 27(2), 184-211.

Batra, R. & Ray, M. L. (1986). Affective responses mediating acceptance of advertising.

Journal of Consumer Research, 13(2), 234–249.

Berthon, P., Pitt, L., & Des Autels, P. (2011). Unveiling videos: consumer-generated ads as

qualitative inquiry. Psychology and Marketing, 28(10), 1044–1060.

BMI (2014). Demographie. Retrieved from http://www.bmi.bund.de/DE/Themen/Gesellschaft

Verfassung/Demografie/demografie_node.html.

Brown, S. P., Homer, P. M., & Imman, J. J. (1998). A meta-analysis of relationships between

ad-evoked feelings and advertising responses. Journal of Marketing Research, 26(1),

69-83.

CBS (2013). Dutch Demography Day 2013. Abstracts. Retrieved from

http://www.nvdemografie.nl/sites/default/files/BookOfAbstracts2013_0.pdf

Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Hillsdale: Lawrence

Erlbaum.

Derbaix, C. & Vanhamme, J. (2003). Inducing word-of-mouth by eliciting surprise: A pilot

investigation. Journal of Economic Psychology, 24(1), 99 −107.

Dobele, A., Toleman, D., & Beverland, M. (2005). Controlled infection! Spreading the brand

message through viral marketing. Business Horizons, 48(2), 143−149.

Edell, J. A. & Burke, M. C. (1987). The power of feelings in understanding advertising

(33)

32 Fishbein, M. & Aijzen, L. (1975). Belief, attitude, intention, and behavior: An introduction to

theory and research. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.

Hayes, A. F. (2012). PROCESS. A versatile computational tool for observed variable

mediation, moderation, and conditional process modelling (white paper). Retrieved

from http://www.afhayes.com/public/process2012.pdf

Holbrook, M. B. & Batra, R. (1987). Assessing the role of emotions as mediators of

consumer responses to advertising. Journal of Consumer Research, 14(3), 404–420.

Igel, C. & Urquhart, V. (2012). Generation Z, meet cooperative learning. Middle School

Journal, 43(4), 267-282.

Knight, M. (1996). Web 2.0 [web technologies]. Communications Engineer, 5(1), 234.

Lang, A. (2009). The limited capacity model of motivated mediated message processing.

The Sage Handbook of Media Processes and Effects, Nabi, L.R. & Oliver, M.B. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications, 193-204.

Lang, P. J. & Bradley, M. (2010). Emotion and the motivational brain. Biological

Psychology, 84(3), 437-50.

Lindgreen, A. & Vanhamme, J. (2005). Viral marketing: The use of surprise. In I.C. Clarke

& T. B. Flaherty (Eds.). Advances in electronic marketing. Hershey, PA: Idea Group.

Lutz, R. J. (1977). Integrating cognitive structure and cognitive response approaches to

monitoring communications effects. Advances in Consumer Research, 4, 363-371.

Lutz, R. J. (1985). Affective and cognitive antecedents of attitude toward the ad: A conceptual

framework. In L. F. Alwitt & A. A. Mitchell (Eds.), Psychological processes and

advertising effects; Theory, research and application. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence

Erlbaum Associate.

MindComet (2006). Viral Marketing: Understanding the concepts and benefits of viral

marketing. Retrieved from http://cmginteractive.com/uploads/viral_marketing.pdf.

(34)

33 advertising effects on brand attitude? Journal of Marketing Research, 18, 318-332.

Olson, D. H., Portner, J., & Bell, R. Q. (1982). FACES II: Family adaptability and cohesion

evaluation scales. Minnesota: Family Social Science, University of Minnesota. Phelps, P. H., Lewis, R., Mobilio, L., Perry, D., & Raman, N. (2004). Viral marketing or

electronic word-of-mouth advertising: examining consumer responses and motivations

to pass along email. Journal of Advertising Research, 44(4), 333–348.

Pillai, R. S., Brakenhoff, G. J., & Müller, M. (2010). Anomalous behavior in the third

harmonic generation z response through dispersion induced shape changes and

matching χ(3). Applied Physic Letter, 89, 23-78.

Poels, K. & Dewitte, S. (2006). How to capture the heart? Reviewing 20 years of emotion

measurement in advertising. Journal of Advertising Research, 46(1), 18-37.

Royo-Vela, A. & Marcela, K. (2005). Emotional and informational content of commercials:

Visual and auditory circumplex spaces, product information and their effects on

audience evaluation. Journal of Current Issues and Research in Advertising, 27,

13-38.

Schwarz, N. & Clore, G. L. (2007). Feelings and phenomenal experiences. In E. T. Higgins,

& A. W. Kruglanski. Social psychology: Handbook of basic principles. New York:

Guilford.

Sheahan, P. (2005). Generation Y: Surviving (and thriving) with Generation Y at work.

Hardy Grant Publishing: Victoria, Australia.

Shimp, T. A. (1981). Attitude toward the ad as a mediator of consumer brand choice. Journal

of Advertising, 10(2), 9-15.

Spears, N. & Singh, S. N. (2004). Measuring attitude toward the brand and purchase

intentions. Journal of Current Issues & Research in Advertising, 26(2), 53–66.

Tellegen, A., Watson, D., & Clark, L. A. (1988). Development and validation of brief

Referenties

GERELATEERDE DOCUMENTEN

De natuurdoeltypen die in de vier studiegebieden voorkomen zijn ingedeeld in kritische en minder kritische natuurdoeltypen voor de aspecten ruimte, water en milieu. Tabel

Als laatste redmiddel zijn de planten helemaal droog gezet (ongeveer 10 dagen geen water vanaf 8 oktober) en daarna verzadigd, maar ook dit heeft geen effect gehad en er is

This thesis examines the anticipated and real social impact of Dakpark in Rotterdam within the context of Nature-Based Solutions and their effects on people and the

To investigate the impacts of these developments on catchment-wetland water resources, the Soil and Water Assessment Tool (SWAT) was applied to the Kilombero Catchment in

We conclude that when the impact time scale of the drop on the substrate (drop diameter/impact velocity) is of the order of the thermal time scale or larger, the cooling effect

[r]

Table 3: Top URLs and Hashtags in User Groups By URL Bias Liberal URL Users Conservative URL Users Neutral URL Users.. Top

In a previous study, we showed that healthy people were able to control an active trunk support using four different control interfaces (based on joystick, force on feet, force