• No results found

Influencers’ impact on students’ attitude toward alcohol and intention to consume alcohol: the moderating roles of parasocial interaction and sponsorship disclosure

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Share "Influencers’ impact on students’ attitude toward alcohol and intention to consume alcohol: the moderating roles of parasocial interaction and sponsorship disclosure"

Copied!
45
0
0

Bezig met laden.... (Bekijk nu de volledige tekst)

Hele tekst

(1)

1

Influencers’ impact on students’ attitude toward alcohol and

intention to consume alcohol: the moderating roles of parasocial

interaction and sponsorship disclosure

Zenna Guijt 10765387

Master’s Thesis

Graduate School of Communication

Research Master’s programme Communication Science University of Amsterdam

Supervisor: Eva van Reijmersdal

(2)

2 Abstract

The current study aimed to investigate the effect of influencers’ alcohol-related posts on

attitude toward alcohol and intention to consume alcohol. In addition, potential moderating

roles of parasocial interaction (PSI) and sponsorship disclosure were examined. Assumptions

were tested among 173 Dutch female students in an experimental 2 (alcohol vs. neutral) × 2 (sponsorship disclosure present vs. sponsorship disclosure not present) between-subjects

design. Results show no significant effects of alcohol-related posts on the dependent

variables. In addition, no moderating effect of sponsorship disclosure was found.

Contradictory findings were found regarding PSI: only a low level of PSI mitigated the

relation between alcohol-related posts and alcohol outcomes. These results highlight the

importance for further research on influencers’ alcohol-related posts.

Introduction

Organizations use social networking sites (SNSs) strategically to market their goods

and services (Carr & Hayes, 2014). By attempting to influence consumers, companies can

work together with someone who has power in these social networks, i.e. an influencer.

Influencers are regarded as internet personalities who have a large amount of followers on

social media and exert influence on their followers (Agrawal, 2016; Varsamis, 2018). By

making and sharing posts on social media, these ‘’ordinary individuals’’ have become

‘’online celebrities’’ (Lou & Yuan, 2019). They are generally perceived as experts in a certain niche, such as lifestyle, beauty or food (Lou & Yuan, 2019). Recently, brands such as Audi

and Samsung have observed the extensive impact and viral growth potential this influencer

marketing can bring (see Carr & Hayes, 2014). A novel report showed that 94% of marketers

who have used influencer marketing deemed these campaigns successful (Ahmad, 2018).

(3)

3

more affordable compared to more acclaimed celebrities (Hall, 2015). Furthermore,

influencers are generally already specialized in a particular niche (Lou & Yuan, 2019). This

implies that when influencers form partnerships with brands that align with this area of

expertise, the campaign is perceived as more trustworthy (Hall, 2016).

One of the social networking sites, Instagram, seems to be mostly beneficial for

influencer marketing, since it enables brands and products to be visually displayed and named

in the caption of the image (de Veirman, Cauberghe & Hudders, 2017). Previous research has

investigated influencer marketing on Instagram, in relation to concepts such as brand attitude

(e.g., de Veirman et al,. 2017) and purchase intention (e.g., Evans, Phua, Lim & Jun, 2017).

However, studies that examine the effects on individuals’ alcohol consumption are lacking,

yet partnerships between alcohol brands and influencers exist (see Medialogica, 2018). These

advertisements seem to reach youth the most, considering the use of social media among

different age groups. In the Netherlands, most of 18 to 25-year-olds (43%) use social media

for one to three hours a day (Statista, 2019). Longitudinal studies have frequently shown that

exposure to alcohol advertising serves as a powerful factor in whether youth will start

drinking alcohol, and the amount they will drink if they have already consumed alcohol

before (e.g., Smith & Foxcroft, 2009).

Among youth, students drink more excessively compared to individuals who do not

attend college (Hoffman, Pinkleton, Weintraub-Austin & Reyes-Velázquez, 2014). A study in

the Netherlands showed that approximately 53% of male students and 47% of female students

drink too much (Trubendorffer, n.d.). Since alcohol advertisers have now extended their reach

through social media and influencers, it seems crucial to investigate to what extent students

are influenced in this new era of digital communication. A large body of research has already

investigated the association of alcohol-related social media posts with drinking outcomes,

(4)

4

Campbell & Ellis, 2012). Previous literature has also researched alcohol marketing on social

media (e.g., Nicholls, 2012; Winpenny, Marteau & Nolte, 2013). However, no study has investigated the influence of exposure to influencers’ alcohol-related posts on alcohol

outcomes among students. Therefore, the aim of the current study is first of all to research the

extent to which influencers’ Instagram posts influence students’ attitude and intention

regarding alcohol. This will expand the existing body of knowledge on influencer marketing,

as these effects have not been studied yet, while youth are likely exposed to influencers’

alcohol-related posts.

Influencers are generally perceived as relatable since they share personal content and

interact with their followers (Abidin, 2016). This could engender parasocial interaction among influencers’ followers (De Veirman et al., 2017). In general, PSI is referred to as an unreal, one-sided relationship with an individual whom one knows well (Horton & Wohl, 1956). Since influencers’ endorsements are also usually highly personal, they could be regarded as influencers’ own attitudes and can therefore be very persuasive (Abidin, 2015). Therefore, the second aim of this study is to investigate the extent to which PSI moderates the relation

between alcohol-related posts and alcohol outcomes.Despite the likelihood that these feelings

occur with influencers (De Veirman et al., 2017), it has not been investigated yet.

The commercial intent of influencer marketing may not be clear to individuals as the

posts resemble editorial content (Boerman & Van Reijmersdal, 2016). In general, disclosures

make individuals more aware of the persuasive intent of the content and can activate their

persuasion knowledge (Boerman & Van Reijmersdal, 2016). This implies that people can feel

more sceptic toward the content (Nelson, Wood & Paek, 2009), process it more critically

(Shrum, Liu, Nespoli & Lowri, 2012) or attempt to avoid it (Cho & Cheon, 2004). Therefore,

the third and final aim of this study is to examine the extent to which a sponsorship disclosure

(5)

5

presence of a disclosure in Facebook posts and blogs has been researched before (e.g.,

Boerman, Willemsem & Van der Aa, 2017; Carr & Hayes, 2014; Hwang & Jeong, 2016),

similar studies regarding Instagram posts are scarce.

The outcomes of this study could raise awareness on how students can be influenced

in this new era of digital communication. Thus, it could benefit health authorities with making

policies and developing campaigns in relation to students and alcohol. In addition, it could

motivate social networking sites to possibly change it guidelines regarding alcohol-related

posts. The results could also provide valuable insights for influencers and brands, since it can

make them more aware of how students are affected by their content.

Theoretical Background

Attitude and intention regarding alcohol

Longitudinal research has shown that youth’s exposure to alcohol advertising is

related to their initial and continued alcohol consumption (e.g., Smith & Foxcroft, 2009). A systematic review on longitudinal studies published since 2008 showed that ‘young people who have greater exposure to alcohol marketing appear to be more likely subsequently to initiate alcohol use and engage in binge and hazardous drinking’ (Jernigan, Noel, Landon, Thornton & Lobstein, 2017, p. 7). One study among students found a positive association

between participants’ exposure to alcohol marketing on social media and their problematic

alcohol consumption (Hoffman et al., 2014). While no study has investigated exposure to

influencers’ alcohol-related posts on drinking outcomes, there is research on celebrity

endorsement and smoking. These studies indicate that young people are more likely to smoke

cigars and e-cigarettes when they have been exposed to celebrity endorsed smoking

(6)

6

Celebrities express their personalities when they endorse a brand, which affects opinions (Jin

& Phua, 2014; Salmon & Atkin, 2003). Compared to celebrities, influencers narrate their lives

in a highly personal way and interact with their followers (Abidin, 2016). Their endorsed

content is often also very personal (Abidin, 2015). Moreover, these posts are interwoven with

the ‘’regular’’ posts in which they narrate their lives, which makes it likely that they will be

seen as the influencers’ own attitudes and could therefore have relevant persuasion abilities

(Abidin, 2015). Therefore, it seems plausible that influencers’ alcohol-related posts affect students’ attitude toward alcohol and intention to consume alcohol. This leads to the following hypothesis:

H1. Influencers’ alcohol-related posts will have a more positive effect on a) attitude toward alcohol, and b) intention to consume alcohol, than more neutral posts.

Parasocial interaction and alcohol outcomes

Parasocial interaction (PSI) can be defined as a ‘seeming face-to-face relationship

between spectator and performer’ (Horton & Wohl, 1956, p. 215). The interaction is

‘one-sided, nondialectical, controlled by the performer, and not susceptible of mutual development’

(Horton & Wohl, 1956, p. 215). Generally, individuals who are engaged in a PSI believe that

they are a part of a direct two-way discourse as they imagine that the mediated persona is

communicating directly to him or her (Levy, 1979). This one-sided relationship stimulates

individuals to use media (Kim, 2005), which in turn fulfils their need to connect (Nordlund,

1978). Feelings of PSI can reach the extent where people start to perceive the mediated

persona as a ‘’real friend’’ (Stern, Russell & Russell, 2007). Previous research has shown associations between media usage and PSI with celebrities (Rubin, Perse & Powell, 1985),

implying that individuals who use media are also likely to feel a certain attachment to

(7)

7

with celebrities and social media use (Kim et al., 2015) and Twitter usage specifically (Kim &

Song, 2016). Compared to regular celebrities, influencers are perceived as accessible, credible

and intimate (Abidin, 2016). They can be relatable to their followers because they post

personal, often publicly inaccessible parts of their lives (Abidin, 2016; Schau & Gilly, 2003).

This could engender feelings of PSI (De Veirman et al., 2017). If a follower develops this

imagery bond with an influencer, he or she is also likely to perceive the influencer as

trustworthy (Labrecque, 2014).

Drawing from social cognitive theory (Bandura, 1986), people can learn by observing

others. Consequently, identifying oneself with media protagonists has a big impact on

individuals’ own opinions, values, beliefs and actions (Phua et al., 2018). A study on smoking habits found that if consumers perceive a celebrity endorser in an advertisement as a role

model, this can in turn positively affect attitudes and intentions toward smoking e-cigarettes

(Phua et al., 2018). In addition, previous studies have found associations between PSI and

purchase tendencies, where higher PSI resulted in greater tendencies (Kim et al., 2015; Xiang,

Zheng, Lee & Zhao, 2016). A study on thin media ideals found that PSI moderates the effect of exposure to a thin media ideal on women’s body image, indicating that participants who felt high PSI towards the media figure, experienced greater body satisfaction compared to

participants who felt less PSI (Young, Gabriel & Sechrist, 2012). The authors argue that

feeling this imagery bond with a media protagonist can increase feelings of similarity,

whereas an absence of PSI is more likely to lead to contrastive effects (Young et al., 2012). In

line with this, it is expected that people who feel high PSI toward an influencer will

experience more positive effects compared to people who feel less PSI. Therefore, the

(8)

8

H2. Parasocial interaction (PSI) will moderate the effect of alcohol-related posts on a)

attitude toward alcohol, and b) intention to consume alcohol, so that people with high PSI

will experience more positive effects than people who have less PSI.

Sponsorship disclosure and alcohol outcomes

Research has indicated that the commercial intent in non-traditional commercials such

as social media advertisements is more difficult to recognize compared to traditional

advertising formats (e.g., Van Noort, Antheunis & Van Reijmersdal, 2012). Similarly,

influencer marketing can be difficult to recognize for individuals, as the posts are made to

look like editorial content while they are in fact paid advertisements (Evans et al., 2017). The

paid relationship between the influencer and the sponsoring brand may not be obvious for the

followers (Evans et al., 2017). The ambiguity of the relationship as well as the number of

followers of the influencer can lead individuals to believe that the posts are based on the

influencer’s own attitudes (Evans et al., 2017). If content is not recognized as persuasive, it

draws more attention, is seen as more believable, and exerts a stronger influence on affective

reactions (e.g., Matthes, Schemer & Wirth, 2007). In order to make consumers more aware of

the commercial intent of the content, new official guidelines advocate that influencers have to

disclose the commercial nature of their content clearly (Hosie, 2018). The rules suggest to use

hashtags such as ‘’ad,’’ ‘’advertising,’’ or ‘’advert,’’ and avoid hashtags such as ‘’spon,’’ ‘’in association with,’’ or ‘’thanks to [brand] for making this possible,’’ as these are not clear enough (Hosie, 2018).

A number of studies has investigated the impact of sponsorship disclosure on

consumers (Boerman & Van Reijmersdal, 2016). Generally, disclosures increases consumers’ awareness about the persuasive intent of the message (e.g., Boerman, Van Reijmersdal &

(9)

9

about social media specifically (e.g., Boerman, et al., 2017; Evans et al., 2017). Also,

sponsorship disclosures can activate consumers’ persuasion knowledge (Boerman & Van

Reijmersdal, 2016), which can be explained with the persuasion knowledge model (PKM;

Friestad & Wright, 1994). The PKM posits that individuals are exposed to different

persuasive content throughout their lives, which helps them establish knowledge and an

understanding about persuasive intent (Friestad & Wright, 1994). Persuasion knowledge can

be regarded as the group of theories and notions about persuasion and its strategies that

individuals form in their lives (Friestad & Wright, 1994). Thus, people gain knowledge over

time from experience about what persuasive content entails and how to develop coping

strategies in order to deal with the persuasive message (Friestad & Wright, 1994). Examples

of these responses are increased scepticism (Nelson et al., 2009), critical processing (Shrum,

et al., 2012) avoidance (Cho & Cheon, 2004), and resistance (Fransen, Verlegh, Kirmani &

Smit, 2015). This implies that when individuals recognize the commercial intent of a

message, they may feel more sceptic, think more critically and try to avoid or resist the

persuasion attempt. Examples of these responses can also be found in studies regarding social media. For instance, a study about celebrities’ sponsored Facebook posts found that

respondents’ ability to recognize the posts as sponsored engendered critical and sceptical beliefs about the content. These beliefs in turn decreased the intention to share the message

(Boerman et al., 2017). In addition, a study on sponsorship disclosure in influencer

advertising on Instagram showed that a disclosure (compared to no disclosure) resulted in a

less favourable brand attitude. Advertising recognition and memorizing a disclosure message

combined also decreased the intention to share the content (Evans et al., 2017). Moreover, a

recent study about advertising on the Chinese platform Sinaweibo found a moderating

negative effect of sponsorship disclosure on the relation between the post’s number of

(10)

10

of the amount of likes on consumers’ positive brand attitude (Seo, Kim, Choi & Li, 2019). Since research generally points to more negative attitudes and intentions resulting from

disclosures, it is plausible that the same would occur for the current study. In addition, in line

with the research of Seo et al. (2019), I expect a moderating effect of sponsorship disclosure

on the relation between alcohol-related posts and alcohol outcomes. Since influencers’

endorsed content on its own is likely to have persuasive abilities due to its personal nature

(Abidin, 2015; Jin & Phua, 2014; Salmon & Atkin, 2003), it seems more plausible that effects

would be moderated instead of mediated by the presence of a sponsorship disclosure.

Therefore, the following hypothesis is formulated:

H3. Sponsorship disclosure will moderate the effect of alcohol-related posts on a) attitude

towards alcohol, and b) intention to consume alcohol, so that exposure to a sponsorship

disclosure will result in less positive effects compared to no sponsorship disclosure.

Sponsorship disclosure, parasocial interaction and alcohol outcomes

It is anticipated that an alcohol-related post will result in a more positive attitude

toward alcohol and greater intention to drink alcohol, compared to a more neutral post

(hypothesis 1). Furthermore, it is hypothesized that individuals who have high PSI toward the

influencer compared to individuals who feel less PSI, will experience more positive effects

(hypothesis 2). In addition, I expect that the presence of a sponsorship disclosure will result in

less positive effects, compared to the absence of a sponsorship disclosure (hypothesis 3).

Based on these hypotheses, it is expected that exposure to an alcohol-related post, in

combination with high PSI and the lack of a sponsorship disclosure will result in a more

positive attitude toward alcohol and a greater intention to consume alcohol. Consecutively,

(11)

11

H4. There is a three way interaction between an alcohol-related post, PSI and sponsorship

disclosure on a) attitude toward alcohol, and b) intention to consume alcohol, so that an

alcohol-related post in combination with high PSI and a lack of sponsorship disclosure will

result in more positive effects, compared to a more neutral post in combination with low PSI

and the presence of a sponsorship disclosure.

Method

Sample and Design

In total, 173 Dutch female students with an Instagram account participated in the study

(Mage = 21.23, SD = 2.21). Only women were recruited because the stimulus material included

a female influencer, which may not engender feelings of PSI in men and could therefore skew

the results. Participants who filled out the survey via the university’s website could earn

credits. The majority of the respondents (59) used Instagram five to seven times a day

(34.10%). The median also lies at the seventh point of the scale (five to seven times a day).

They spend 107.64 minutes on the platform daily, on average (SD = 274.98). In this study, we

adopted a 2 (alcohol vs. neutral) × 2 (sponsorship disclosure present vs. sponsorship

disclosure not present) between-subjects design. Participants were randomly assigned to one

of four conditions: an alcohol-related post with sponsorship disclosure, an alcohol-related post

without sponsorship disclosure, a neutral post with sponsorship disclosure, or a neutral post

without sponsorship disclosure.

Procedure

Participants were primarily recruited through the university’s lab website (lab.uva.nl),

(12)

12

Facebook and LinkedIn, which explicitly stated that only students could participate. This was

also stated in the fact sheet of the survey. Friends and acquaintances of whom the author knew

were students, have been personally asked to participate.

After giving their consent, participants could fill out the questionnaire. First, they were

asked brief questions about their Instagram usage, after which they were randomly assigned to

one of four conditions. Participants were made aware that they were going to be exposed to an

Instagram post and that they could continue the survey after viewing the post for at least 10

seconds. After being exposed to the stimulus material, participants were asked questions

about brand recognition and brand attitude. Subsequently, questions about their attitude

toward alcohol, intention to consume alcohol and previous alcohol consumptions were asked.

Next, PSI and attitude toward the influencer were measured. In addition, questions regarding

alcohol recognition, advertising recognition, disclosure memory were asked. Finally, they

were asked if they were familiar with the influencer and the brand(s) they had or had not seen

in the Instagram post. All participants were debriefed after completing the survey.

Pretest

The aim of the pretest was to determine the content of the stimulus material for the

experiment. Participants (N = 11, Mage = 22.91, SD = 1.81, 90.90% female) were presented

four altered Instagram posts, in which alcohol was removed from the photo, and were asked

how realistic they found these posts. The four original posts were selected from Instagram. In

order to be selected, the post had to meet the following criteria: 1) the post was shared by an

individual who is primarily known for being an influencer, instead of an actor or presenter for

example, and 2) the photo included a clean background so that the alcohol in the photo was

(13)

13

Participants were asked, using a scale ranging from 1 (unrealistic) to 7 (realistic), how

realistic they found the altered posts that originally included the brand Liefmans (M = 4.55;

SD = 1.44), Heineken (M = 3.00; SD = 1,76), KetelOne (M = 5.00; SD = 1.69) and Moët (M =

4.18; SD = 1.78). Since the four original posts depicted an alcohol brand, participants’ attitude

toward these brands was assessed. They were asked how they felt, using 7-point semantic

differentials: unattractive-attractive/unpleasant-pleasant/boring-interesting/not

nice-nice/negative-positive/bad-good (Bruner & Kumar, 2000) towards Liefmans (Cronbach’s α =

0.81, M = 5.56, SD = 0.69, N = 9), Heineken (Cronbach’s α = 0.74, M = 5.0, SD = 0.82, N =

11), KetelOne (Cronbach’s α = 0.95, M = 4.80, SD = 1.43, N = 5) and Moët (Cronbach’s α =

0.83, M = 4.52, SD = 0.99, N = 9). Furthermore, PSI and attitude were measured regarding

three Dutch influencers who were ranked high in the list of ‘’most influential Dutch

influencers’’ (De Media 100, 2019). To measure PSI, participants were presented ten items

and asked to rate them on a scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree;

based on the Revised Parasocial Interaction Scale, Rubin & Perse, 1987). Based on factor

analyses, two subscales were created for PSI towards Anna Nooshin (Cronbach’s α = 0.84, M

= 1.89, SD = 0.79; Cronbach’s α = 0.72, M = 3.00, SD = 1.32, N = 7). Similarly, two

subscales were created for PSI towards Monica Geuze (Cronbach’s α = 0.95, M = 2.19, SD =

1.39; Cronbach’s α = 0.70, M = 2.61, SD = 0.71, N = 6) and Enzo Knol (Cronbach’s α = 0.85,

M = 1.43, SD = 0.61; Cronbach’s α = 0.85, M = 2.36, SD = 1.22, N = 7). To assess attitude,

participants were asked how they felt, using 7-point semantic differentials:

unpleasant-pleasant/boring-interesting/not nice-nice/negative-positive/bad-good (Bruner & Kumar,

2000), towards Anna Nooshin (Cronbach’s α = 0.78, M = 4.67, SD = 0.72, N = 7), Monica

Geuze (Cronbach’s α = 0.88, M = 3.73, SD = 1.12, N = 6) and Enzo Knol (Cronbach’s α =

(14)

14

Based on the results of the pretest, the post which depicted the brand Liefmans was

chosen, because the altered post was considered fairly realistic. Participants reported a slightly

higher score regarding the post in which a bottle of KetelOne was removed. However, the

photo included a cocktail with syrup and whipped cream, so it could also be perceived as a

dessert. This could interfere with the results of the study. Unfortunately, little to no feelings of

PSI toward the three influencers were expressed by the participants. Therefore, the influencer

was chosen based on attitude. Since the influencer would not be present in the picture of the

stimulus material, it might be less likely that PSI would occur. However, it may be more

likely if the influencer is well-liked. Participants in the pretest expressed the most positive

attitude toward Anna Nooshin.

Stimuli

Participants were exposed to a fictitious Instagram post by Anna Nooshin

(@annanooshin). The photo included a platter of snacks and two filled glasses (one of which

is a red-coloured drink) on a white background. In the alcohol conditions, the photo included

a bottle of Liefmans, which was also referred to in the text of the post. The alcohol with

sponsorship disclosure condition included the hashtag ‘’advertisment’’ and the phrase ‘’paid

partnership with Liefmans.’’ The neutral conditions did not show the bottle of Liefmans nor

references to alcohol in the text. To avoid that participants thought the posts contain alcohol,

the red-coloured drink was referred to as cassis in the texts. Similarly, the neutral with

sponsorship disclosure condition included the hashtag ‘’advertisement’’ and the phrase ‘’paid

(15)

15 Measures

Alcohol Attitude and Intention

Attitude towards alcohol was measured using 7-point semantic differentials with the

statement ‘’if I were to drink alcohol the following week, I would find that

unpleasant-pleasant/not nice-nice/unwise-wise/harmless-harmful/negative-positive/bad-good’’ (Cronbach’s α = 0.89, M = 4.50, SD = 1.03; based on TBP; Azjen, 1991). Intention to consume alcohol was measured by asking participants to rate the statements ‘’I plan to drink

alcohol during the following week’’ and ‘’I intent to drink alcohol during the following

week’’ (based on TBP; Azjen, 1991) on a scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7

(strongly agree; M = 3.94, SD = 2.00).

Parasocial Interaction (PSI)

PSI was assessed based on the Revised Parasocial Interaction Scale (Rubin & Perse,

1987). Rubin & Perse’s scale originally measured PSI with a television soap opera character.

Therefore, the ten items were slightly altered in order to make them more accurate for the

present study. Participants were asked to rate statements such as ‘’Anna Nooshin feels like a

friend to me’’ and ‘’I look forward to seeing Anna Nooshin on Instagram,’’ on a scale ranging

from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree). A factor analysis showed three components

with an eigenvalue higher than 1. As a result, two subscales were made, with the first

including items ‘’Anna Nooshin feels like a friend to me,’’ ‘’I look forward seeing Anna

Nooshin on Instagram,’’ ‘’If I would see Anna Nooshin on another Instagram account, I would follow this account,’’ ‘’Anna Nooshin seems to understand the kinds of things I want to know’’ and ‘’I miss seeing Anna Nooshin if she does not post anything’’ (Cronbach’s α =

(16)

16

saw a story about Anna Nooshin in a newspaper or magazine, I would read it,’’ ‘’I would like

to meet Anna Nooshin in person,’’ ‘’I feel sorry for Anna Nooshin when she makes a

mistake’’ and ‘’I find Anna Nooshin to be attractive’’ (Cronbach’s α = 0.71, M = 3.26, SD =

1.18; Bruner & Kumar, 2000). Since the first subscale seems to depict attachment toward the

influencer, it was called the ‘’attachment subscale of PSI.’’ Items of the second subscale seem

to show admiration and was therefore named ‘’the admiration subscale of PSI.’’ Item 2 (‘’I

see Anna Nooshin as a natural, down-to-earth person’’) served as a component on its own and

was therefore not included in either of the subscales.

Manipulation Checks

To assess whether participants noticed a brand in the stimulus material, they were

firstly asked if they saw a brand in the Instagram post (0 = no or I am not sure and 1 = yes).

Secondly, they were asked ‘’which brand did you see in the post?’’. All participants were

presented with different brands to choose from including an option ‘’none of these brands.’’

This was then recoded into 0 = ‘’I do not remember’’ or ‘’none of these brands’’ and 1 = did

report a brand. In addition, to measure if participants acknowledged alcohol in the stimulus

material, the question ‘’was there alcohol present in the post?’’ was posed (0 = no or I am not

sure and 1 = yes). In order to assess whether participants noticed the persuasive intent of the

stimulus material, advertising recognition and disclosure memory were measured. Advertising

recognition was measured by asking the participants to answer the following statement: ‘’indicate to what extent you think the Instagram post you just saw was an advertisement,’’ on a scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree; M = 5.63; SD = 1.70;

Boerman et al., 2012). This was followed by an open ended question: ‘’why do you think that

the post was or was not an advertisement?’’ (Wojdynsk & Evans, 2016), which was later

(17)

17

disclosure present). Participants’ disclosure memory was assessed by asking them whether or

not they had seen one or more of the following terms or hashtags in the post: #sponsored,

#adv, paid partnership with Liefmans, paid partnership with Fanta, #collaboration, paid

partnership with Hero, #advertisement. This was then recoded into 0 = did not report a

disclosure and 1 = did report a disclosure.

Control Variables

To measure participants’ familiarity with the brands, for each brand they were asked whether they were familiar with it before participating in the study (0 = no or I am not sure

and 1 = yes). Most participants reported to be familiar with Liefmans (74.00%) as well as

Hero (83.20%). The same question was posed about Anna Nooshin. Most participants

(87.90%) reported to be familiar with the influencer. Additionally, these 152 participants were

asked whether they follow Anna Nooshin on Instagram (0 = no or I am not sure and 1 = yes).

Most of these participants indicated that they did not (69.10%). Furthermore, participants’

attitude toward the alcohol brand Liefmans was assessed, using 7-point semantic differentials:

unattractive-attractive/unpleasant-pleasant/boring-interesting/not

nice-nice/negative-positive/bad-good (Cronbach’s α = 0.94, M = 4.91, SD = 1.04; Bruner & Kumar, 2000). The

same scale was used to assess participants’ attitude toward Hero (Cronbach’s α = 0.94, M =

4.60, SD = 0.95; Bruner & Kumar, 2000). In order to establish participants’ attitude toward

Anna Nooshin, the same questions were posed about the influencer (Cronbach’s α = 0.88, M

= 4.39, SD = 1.00; Bruner & Kumar, 2000). The first item (unattractive-attractive) was

removed, since participants already answered this question in the PSI scale. Moreover, the

survey included questions about participants’ average Instagram usage (‘’how much do you

use Instagram on average?’’ 0 = less than once a month, 1 = monthly, 2 = once a week, 3 =

(18)

18

a day, 7 = eight times a day or more; median = five to seven times a day) and daily Instagram

usage (‘’on average, how much hours and/or minutes do you spend on Instagram daily?’’; M

= 107.64; SD = 274.98; Sheldon, Rauschnabel, Antony & Car, 2017). Finally, participants’

alcohol consumption was measured by asking them to estimate the amount of alcoholic drinks

they consumed for each day in the previous week (M = 1.15; SD = 1.16, based on the Daily

Drinking Questionnaire; DDQ; Collins, Parks & Marlatt, 1985).

Results

Manipulation Checks

Crosstabs showed a significant difference for brand recognition, χ2(1) = 52.98, p <

0.001. The majority of the participants (74.60%) who were exposed to a brand indicated

correctly that they noticed a brand. Regarding the condition without a brand, most of the

participants (88.40%) correctly stated that they did not notice a brand. In addition, there was a

significant difference between groups regarding brand memory, χ2(1) = 58.31, p < 0.001.

Most of the participants (75.40%) who were part of the conditions with brand indicated that

they saw a brand. The majority of the participants (90.70%) who were part of the condition

without a brand reported to have not seen a brand. Regarding alcohol recognition, there was

significant difference between conditions, χ2(1) = 59.52, p = 0.00. The majority of the

participants (74.70%) within the alcohol conditions reported to have noticed alcohol. In the

neutral conditions, 72 participants (83.70%) indicated that they did not see alcohol. With

respect to the closed question that measured advertising recognition, a significant difference

between participants in disclosure conditions (M = 6.11; SD = 1.33) and conditions without

disclosure (M = 5.18; SD = 1.88), F(1, 171) = 13.90, p < 0.001 was found. Also, a significant

difference was found between conditions regarding the open-ended question, χ2(1) = 9.16, p =

(19)

19

that there was a disclosure present. However, only 26 participants (29.20%) who were part of

the conditions without a disclosure correctly reported that there was not a disclosure present,

whereas 63 participants (70.80%) in these conditions incorrectly reported that there was a

disclosure present. With respect to disclosure memory, a significant difference was found

between conditions, χ2(1) = 96.06, p < 0.001. Most of the participants (72.60%) who were

part of the disclosure conditions checked one or more of the disclosures. In the conditions

without a disclosure present, the majority of the participants (89.90%) did not report a

disclosure.

Randomization

Analyses of variance and chi-square analyses showed that respondents between the

alcohol and the neutral conditions did not differ regarding age, F(1, 171) = .00, p = 0.994,

average Instagram usage, F(1, 171) = .66, p = 0.417, daily Instagram usage, F(1, 171) = .48, p

= 0.490, familiarity with Liefmans, χ (1) = 0.05, p = 0.827, familiarity with Hero, χ(1) = 0.33,

p = 0.685, familiarity with the influencer, χ2(1) = .07, p = 0.820, whether they followed the

influencer on Instagram, χ2(1) = .40, p = 0.600, or alcohol consumption, F(1, 171) = .23, p =

0.633, demonstrating successful randomisation.

Analysis

To test hypotheses 1 and 3, a MANOVA was conducted with alcohol condition

(alcohol vs. neutral) and disclosure condition (sponsorship disclosure present vs. sponsorship

disclosure not present) as independent variables. Attitude toward alcohol and intention to

(20)

20

Multivariate tests showed no significant multivariate effect from alcohol-related posts,

Wilks’ lambda = 0.96, F(3, 167) = 2.24, ns. Sponsorship disclosure also did not have a multivariate effect, Wilks’ lambda = 0.99, F(3, 167) = 0.61, ns. In addition, no interaction

effect was found from alcohol-related posts and sponsorship disclosure, Wilks’ lambda =

0.99, F(3, 167) = 0.60, ns.

Regarding hypothesis 1a, no significant effects were found, F(1, 169) = 2.60, p =

0.109, ηp2= 0.015 and 1b, F(1, 169) = .12, p = 0.733, ηp2= 0.001, indicating that exposure to an alcohol-related post did not result in a more positive attitude toward alcohol nor a greater

intention to consume alcohol, compared to exposure to a more neutral post.

With respect to hypothesis 3a and 3b, the analysis showed no moderation effect from

disclosure on the relationship between post and attitude toward alcohol, F(1, 169) = 0.04, p =

0.847, ηp2< 0.001, and intention to consume alcohol, F(1, 169) = 0.05, p = 0.827, ηp2< 0.001. This means there were no significant differences in alcohol outcomes between respondents

who saw a sponsorship disclosure and respondents who did not see a sponsorship disclosure.

Table 1

Descriptive Statistics for attitude toward alcohol and intention to consume alcohol by conditions (N = 173)

Alcohol Neutral

Mean SD Mean SD

Attitude toward alcohol 4.63 0.97 4.38 1.07

Intention to consume alcohol 3.98 1.99 3.89 2.03 ___________________________________________________________________

(21)

21

In order to test hypotheses 2 and 4, Hayes’ PROCESS macro (Hayes, 2012) was used, with respectively Model 1 and Model 3. Regarding hypothesis 2a, results demonstrated that

the attachment subscale of PSI was not a significant moderator of the relation between

alcohol-related posts and attitude toward alcohol (b = -0.24, SE = 0.19, p = 0.199). A

significant moderation effect was found from the admiration subscale of PSI (b = -0.28, SE =

0.13, t = -2.12, p = 0.035). The conditional interaction effect of the admiration subscale of

PSI was positive and significant until value 3.00 (b = 0.598, SE = 0.23, t = 1.98, p = 0.049),

indicating that only for individuals who reported a low level of PSI, an alcohol-related post

resulted in a more positive attitude toward alcohol.

With respect to hypothesis 2b, findings indicated no significant moderation effect from

the attachment subscale of PSI on the relation between alcohol-related posts and intention to

consume alcohol (b = -0.10, SE = 0.37, p = 0.785). A significant moderation effect was found

from the admiration subscale of PSI (b = -0.55, SE = 0.26, t = -2.10, p = 0.037). The

conditional interaction effect of the admiration subscale of PSI was positive and significant

only at value 1.00 (b = 1.32, SE = 0.66, t = 1.99, p = 0.048), demonstrating that only for

participants who experienced low feelings of PSI, an alcohol-related post led to a higher

intention to consume alcohol. Additionally, marginal negative significant effects were found

from value 5.25 (b = -1.01, SE = 0.60, t = -1.67, p = 0.096), which implies that for

participants who experienced high feelings of PSI, exposure to an alcohol-related post led to a

lower intention to consume alcohol. However, this effect is only marginally significant.

Regarding hypothesis 4a, findings demonstrated that there is no significant three way

interaction between alcohol-related p, sponsorship disclosure and the attachment subscale of

PSI on attitude towards alcohol (b = 0.50, SE = 0.39, p = 0.199). Similar results were found

regarding the admiration subscale of PSI (b = -0.10 , SE = 0.27, p = 0.714). This implies that

(22)

22

present and who experienced high feelings of PSI did not have a more favourable attitude

toward alcohol compared to participants who saw a more neutral post with a sponsorship

disclosure and who felt low feelings of PSI.

With respect to hypothesis 4b, no significant three way interaction alcohol-related

posts, sponsorship disclosure and the attachment subscale of PSI on intention to drink alcohol

was found (b = 0.07, SE = 0.76, p = 0.924). Similar results were found regarding the

admiration subscale of PSI (b = 0.26, SE = 0.53, p = 0.629). This means that an

alcohol-related post without a sponsorship disclosure present in combination with high feelings of PSI

did not result in a stronger intention to consume alcohol compared to a more neutral post with

a sponsorship disclosure present in combination with low feelings of PSI.

Discussion

The aim of this study was to examine to what extent influencers’ alcohol-related Instagram posts influence students’ attitude and intention regarding alcohol. The study also

aimed to investigate to what degree sponsorship disclosure and PSI moderated these relations.

Assumptions were tested among 173 Dutch female students in an experimental design.

Results of the study firstly show that the expected influence from alcohol-related posts

has not been found: exposure to influencer’s alcohol-related posts did not result in a more

positive attitude nor a greater intention to consume alcohol, in comparison to more neutral

posts. An explanation for this unexpected finding could be that participants only saw one post.

This may not have been enough exposure in order to affect alcohol outcomes. Another

explanation may be that the average alcohol consumption among participants was low. They

may not be interested in alcohol enough in order to be affected by an alcohol-related post. In

(23)

23

advertisements in general. These studies show associations between exposure to alcohol

marketing and alcohol consumption among students (e.g., Smith & Foxcroft, 2009). However,

the relation between influencer marketing and alcohol outcomes may not be as

straightforward. Other factors may play a significant role regarding the persuasive abilities of

the content, such as the influencer’s perceived popularity (see De Veirman et al., 2017) or

advertising recognition (see Evans et al., 2017).

Second, significant moderating effects from PSI on the relations between

alcohol-related posts on alcohol outcomes were partly found. That is, only the admiration subscale of

PSI was found to cause significant effects. Interestingly, this subscale had a reversed effect:

only low feelings of PSI mitigated the influence of alcohol-related posts, so that it resulted in

a more positive attitude toward alcohol and a higher intention to consume alcohol, compared

to more neutral posts. A possible explanation for these contradictory findings could be the

perceived ‘’fit’’ between the influencer and the brand. Anna Nooshin does not (regularly)

share alcohol-related messages nor does she seem to endorse any alcohol brands. Liefmans

may therefore not align with her values. Participants who reported low feelings of PSI toward

Anna Nooshin may not be aware of this, whereas participants who experienced high feelings

of PSI are likely more familiar with the influencer’s values.

Third, no moderation effect of sponsorship disclosure was found. This means that a

disclosure did not mitigate a potential effect of alcohol-related posts on alcohol outcomes. A

clarification for this unanticipated finding may be that the majority of participants who were

in the conditions without a disclosure present believed that the post they saw to be an

advertisement. Thus, a disclosure did not have an effect. Influencers’ posts were still regarded

(24)

24

Fourth, the expected three-way interaction between alcohol-related posts, sponsorship

disclosure and PSI on alcohol outcomes was not found. The same clarifications given for the

unexpected results of hypotheses 1, 2 and 3 could explain this unexpected finding.

Limitations and Future Research

Similar to other studies, this research has not been conducted without its limitations.

First of all, results of this study may be hard to generalize to other female students, as students

in general drink more (Hoffman et al., 2014; Trubendorffer, n.d.) compared to the low

average alcohol intake of the participants in the current study. Second, participants were

exposed to only one post. This may not be sufficient in order to affect individuals’ attitudes

and intentions. Future research could investigate whether showing more posts results in

different findings. Third, comments were removed from the stimulus material, as they could

have driven attention away from the picture and text. While this improves internal validity, it

reduces the ecological validity of the study. Moreover, this could influence the outcomes of

the study as people may be affected by perceptions of others. Fourth, there exist other

variables besides PSI and sponsorship disclosure that could mitigate (i.e., moderate) or

explain (i.e., mediate) the potential persuasive effects of these posts. Prior studies that

investigated influence from endorsers of brands on perceptions regarding these brands,

showed that certain characteristics of celebrity endorsers could positively affect the

successfulness of the advertisement (Bergkvist, Hjalmarson & Magi, 2016). For instance, the ‘’fit’’ between the influencer and the endorsed brand may be important, as suggested by the ‘’match-up hypothesis’’ (e.g., Till & Busler, 2000). Influencers are often specialized in a certain area (Lou & Yuan, 2019), which can make a campaign perceived as more credible if it

entails a brand that aligns with this particular niche (Hall, 2016). Source credibility, which can

(25)

25

of a message’’ (Ohanian, 1990), can influence individuals’ attitudes (Pornpitakpan, 2004). An

important determinant of source credibility is perceived popularity (Jin & Phua, 2014), which

individuals can derive from ‘’cues’’ on social media (Utz, 2010). Examples of these cues are

the user’s amount of friends (e.g., Tong, Van der Heide, Langwell & Walther, 2008; Utz,

2010) or followers (e.g., De Veirman et al., 2017; Jin & Phua, 2014). Future studies should

explore whether these factors influence the effects of exposure to influencers’ alcohol-related

posts.

Theoretical and Practical Implications

Despite its limitations, this study contributes to the literature in several ways. It is the

first study to research influencer marketing in relation to alcohol outcomes. The

nonsignificant and contradictory results demonstrate the need for an improved theoretical

framework. Due to the growing popularity of influencer marketing and the potential harmful

effects alcohol advertising can have on young people’s drinking tendencies, it is relevant to investigate the effects of alcohol-related posts on alcohol outcomes further.

An important finding of the study was the negative moderation of the admiration PSI

subscale on the relation between alcohol-related posts and alcohol outcomes. This contradicts

prior research that have shown positive associations between PSI and smoking attitudes and

intentions (Phua et al., 2018) and purchase tendencies (Kim et al., 2015; Xiang et al., 2016).

In addition, one study found a positive moderated effect of PSI on the relation between

exposure to a thin media ideal and body satisfaction (Young et al., 2012). Presumably, the

contradictory findings can be explained by the lack of fit between the influencer and the

endorsed brand. The values of the endorsed brand and the ones of the influencer in the current

study plausibly did not align. Participants with high PSI reported reactions that may be more

(26)

26

participants with low PSI. The findings then support the notion that high PSI results in

responses similar to the media protagonist, while low PSI causes contrastive effects (Young et

al., 2012). However, these are assumptions and they should be tested further before making

any conclusive statements.

Furthermore, this study contributes to our understanding of advertising recognition on

Instagram. Previous research indicates that the presence of a sponsorship disclosure can

increase people’s awareness about the persuasive intent of a message (e.g., Boerman et al., 2012; Boerman et al., 2017; Campbell et al., 2013; Evans et al., 2017). However, the current

study showed no differences regarding advertising recognition between participants in

conditions with and without a disclosure. This suggests that without the presence of a sponsorship disclosure, influencers’ post are still likely to be perceived as advertisements.

At a practical level, the results first of all indicate that PSIs play an important role in a campaign’s persuasive abilities. It seems crucial that the values of the influencer align with the values of the endorsed brand or product. An advice for brands would therefore be to form

partnerships with influencers that align with its values and vice versa. Secondly, findings

indicate that Instagram’s new influencer guidelines (see Hosie, 2018) may not be used

sufficiently by influencers. If they would be used correctly, users would not still perceive an

Instagram post without a proper disclosure as an advertisement. An advice for the social

network could be to make the use of disclosures more salient. For instance, by notifying a

user about their disclosure policy anytime they post anything. For influencers, the results

indicate that they should attempt to be more transparent about their endorsements. Users

(27)

27 References

Abidin, C. (2015). Communicative intimacies: Influencers and perceived

interconnectedness. Ada, 8, 1-16. Retrieved from https://adanewmedia.org/issues/

Abidin, C. (2016). Visibility labour: Engaging with influencers’ fashion brands and

#OOTD advertorial campaigns on Instagram. Media International Australia, 161(1), 86–100.

doi:10.1177/1329878X16665177

Agrawal, A.J. (2016). Why Influencer Marketing Will Explode in 2017. Retrieved

from https://www.forbes. com/sites/ajagrawal/2016/12/27/why-influencer-marketing-

will-explode-in-2017/#3bfaf85c20a9

Ahmad, I. (2018). The Influencer Marketing Revolution. Retrieved from https://

www.socialmediatoday.com/news/the-influencer-marketing-revolution-infographic/517146/

Ajzen, I. (1991). The theory of planned behavior. Organization Behavior and Human

Decision Processes, 50, 179–211. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0749-5978(91)90020-T

Ajzen, I., & Fishbein, M. (1980). Understanding attitudes and predicting social

behavior. Englewood Cliffs. NJ: Prentice Hall.

Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action: A social cognitive

theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ, US: Prentice-Hall, Inc.

Bergkvist, L., Hjalmarson, H., & Mägi, A. W. (2016). A new model of how celebrity

endorsements work: attitude toward the endorsement as a mediator of celebrity source and

endorsement effects. International Journal of Advertising, 35(2), 171–184.

doi:10.1080/02650487.2015.1024384

Boerman, S. C., & van Reijmersdal, E. A. (2016). Informing consumers about

(28)

28

De Pelsmacker (ed.), Advertising in new formats and media: Current research and

Implications for marketers (pp. 115–146). Emerald Group Publishing Limited.

Boerman, S. C., Van Reijmersdal, E. A., & Neijens, P. C. (2012). Sponsorship

disclosure: Effects of duration on persuasion knowledge and brand responses. Journal of

Communication, 62(6), 1047–1064. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-2466.2012.01677.x

Boerman, S. C., Willemsen, L. M., & Van Der Aa, E. P. (2017). “This post is

sponsored”: Effects of sponsorship disclosure on persuasion knowledge and electronic word of mouth in the context of Facebook. Journal of Interactive Marketing, 38, 82–92.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intmar.2016.12.002

Carr, C. T., & Hayes, R. A. (2014). The effect of disclosure of third-party influence on

an opinion leader's credibility and electronic word of mouth in two-step flow. Journal of

Interactive Advertising, 14(1), 38–50. https://doi.org/10.1080/15252019.2014.909296

Campbell, M. C., Mohr, G. S., & Verlegh, P. W. (2013). Can disclosures lead

consumers to resist covert persuasion? The important roles of disclosure timing and type of

response. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 23(4), 483–495.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcps.2012.10.012

Cho, C. H., & Cheon, H.J. (2004). Why do people avoid advertising on the

internet?. Journal of advertising, 33(4), 89–97.

https://doi.org/10.1080/00913367.2004.10639175

De Media 100. (2019). DeInfluencer50. Retrieved from

http://www.demedia100.nl/deinfluencer50.html

de Veirman, M., Cauberghe, V., & Hudders, L. (2017). Marketing through Instagram

(29)

29

attitude. International Journal of Advertising, 36(5), 798–828.

https://doi.org/10.1080/02650487.2017.1348035

Evans, N. J., Phua, J., Lim, J., & Jun, H. (2017). Disclosing instagram influencer

advertising: The effects of disclosure language on advertising recognition, attitudes, and

behavioral intent. Journal of Interactive Advertising, 17(2), 138–149.

https://doi.org/10.1080/15252019.2017.1366885

Fransen, M. L., Verlegh, P. W., Kirmani, A., & Smit, E. G. (2015). A typology of

consumer strategies for resisting advertising, and a review of mechanisms for countering

them. International Journal of Advertising, 34(1), 6–16. doi:10.1080/02650487.2014.995284

Friestad, M., & Wright, P. (1994). The persuasion knowledge model: How people

cope with persuasion attempts. Journal of Consumer Research, 21(1), 1–31.

doi:10.1086/209380

Hall, J. (2015). Build Authentic Audience Experiences through Influencer Marketing.

Retrieved from https://www.forbes.com/sites/johnhall/2015/12/17/buildauthentic-

audience-experiences-through-influencer-marketing/# 589d25fa4ff2.

Hall, K. (2016). The Importance of Authenticity in Influencer Marketing. Retrieved

from https://www.sproutcontent.com/blog/the-importance-ofauthenticity-

in-influencer-marketing.

Hayes, A. F. (2012). PROCESS: A versatile computational tool for observed variable

mediation, moderation, and conditional process modeling [White paper]. Retrieved from

http://www.afhayes.com/ public/process2012.pdf

Hoffman, E. W., Pinkleton, B. E., Weintraub Austin, E., & Reyes-Velázquez, W. (2014). Exploring college students’ use of general and alcohol-related social media and their associations with alcohol-related behaviors. Journal of American College Health, 62(5), 328–

(30)

30

Horton, D., & Richard Wohl, R. (1956). Mass communication and para-social

interaction: Observations on intimacy at a distance. Psychiatry, 19(3), 215–229.

https://doi.org/10.1080/00332747.1956.11023049

Hosie, R. (2018). There's now an official guide for social media influencers posting

adverts. Retrieved from

https://www.independent.co.uk/life-style/social-media-influencers-adverts-guide-asa-law-instagram-cma-a8559406.html

Hwang, Y., & Jeong, S. H. (2016). “This is a sponsored blog post, but all opinions are my own”: The effects of sponsorship disclosure on responses to sponsored blog

posts. Computers in Human Behavior, 62, 528–535. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2016.04.026

Jernigan, D., Noel, J., Landon, J., Thornton, N., & Lobstein, T. (2017). Alcohol

marketing and youth alcohol consumption: a systematic review of longitudinal studies

published since 2008. Addiction, 112, 7–20. https://doi.org/10.1111/add.13591

Jin, S. A. A., & Phua, J. (2014). Following celebrities’ tweets about brands: The impact of twitter-based electronic word-of-mouth on consumers’ source credibility

perception, buying intention, and social identification with celebrities. Journal of

Advertising, 43(2), 181–195.https://doi.org/10.1080/00913367.2013.827606

Kim, J. K. (2005). Parasocial interaction with favorite television characters. Korean

Journal of Broadcasting and Telecommunication Studies, 19, 255–285

Kim, H., Ko, E., & Kim, J. (2015). SNS users' para-social relationships with

celebrities: social media effects on purchase intentions. Journal of Global Scholars of

Marketing Science, 25(3), 279–294. https://doi.org/10.1080/21639159.2015.1043690

Kim, J., & Song, H. (2016). Celebrity's self-disclosure on Twitter and parasocial

relationships: A mediating role of social presence. Computers in Human Behavior, 62, 570–

(31)

31

Labrecque, L. I. (2014). Fostering consumer–brand relationships in social media

environments: The role of parasocial interaction. Journal of Interactive Marketing, 28(2),

134–148. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intmar.2013.12.003

Levy, M. R. (1979). Watching TV news as para‐social interaction. Journal of

Broadcasting & Electronic Media, 23(1), 69–80. https://doi.org/10.1080/08838157909363919

Lou, C., & Yuan, S. (2019). Influencer Marketing: How Message Value and

Credibility Affect Consumer Trust of Branded Content on Social Media. Journal of

Interactive Advertising, 1–16. https://doi.org/10.1080/15252019.2018.1533501

Nelson, M. R., Wood, M. L., & Paek, H. J. (2009). Increased persuasion knowledge of

video news releases: Audience beliefs about news and support for source disclosure. Journal

of Mass Media Ethics, 24(4), 220–237. https://doi.org/10.1080/08900520903332626

Nicholls, J. (2012). Everyday, everywhere: alcohol marketing and social media—

current trends. Alcohol and alcoholism, 47(4), 486–493. https://doi.org/10.1093/alcalc/ags043

Nordlund, J. E. (1978). Media interaction. Communication Research, 5(2), 150–175.

doi:10.1177/009365027800500202

Matthes, J., Schemer, C., & Wirth, W. (2007). More than meets the eye: Investigating

the hidden impact of brand placements in television magazines. International Journal of

Advertising, 26(4), 477–503. https://doi.org/10.1080/02650487.2007.11073029 Medialogica. (2018). Alcoholreclame op Instagram. Retrieved from

https://www.human.nl/medialogica/kijk/online/alcoholreclame.html

Moreno, M. A., Christakis, D. A., Egan, K. G., Brockman, L. N., & Becker, T. (2012).

Associations between displayed alcohol references on Facebook and problem drinking among

college students. Archives of pediatrics & adolescent medicine, 166(2), 157–163.

(32)

32

Ohanian, R. (1990). Construction and validation of a scale to measure celebrity

endorsers' perceived expertise, trustworthiness, and attractiveness. Journal of

advertising, 19(3), 39–52. https://doi.org/10.1080/00913367.1990.10673191

Phua, J., Jin, S. V., & Hahm, J. M. (2018). Celebrity-endorsed e-cigarette brand

Instagram advertisements: effects on young adults’ attitudes towards e-cigarettes and smoking intentions. Journal of health psychology, 23(4), 550–560.

https://doi.org/10.1177/1359105317693912

Pornpitakpan, C. (2004). The persuasiveness of source credibility: A critical review of

five decades’ evidence. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 34(1), 243–81. doi:10.1111/j.1559-1816.2004.tb02547.x

Ridout, B., Campbell, A., & Ellis, L. (2012). ‘Off your Face (book)’: alcohol in online social identity construction and its relation to problem drinking in university students. Drug

and alcohol review, 31(1), 20–26. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1465-3362.2010.00277.x

Rubin, A. M., Perse, E. M., & Powell, R. A. (1985). Loneliness, para-social

interaction, and local television news viewing. Human Communication Research, 12, 155–

180. doi:10.1111/j.1468-2958.1985.tb00071.x

Salmon, C. T., & Atkin, C. (2003). Using media campaigns for health promotion.

In T.L. Thompson et al. (Eds), Handbook of health communication (pp.449–472). Mahwah,

New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc., Publishers.

Schau, H.J., & Gilly, M.C. (2003). We are what we post? Self-presentation in personal

web space. Journal of Consumer Research, 30(3), pp 385–404. doi:10.1086/378616

Seo, Y., Kim, J., Choi, Y. K., & Li, X. (2019). In “likes” we trust: likes, disclosures and firm-serving motives on social media. European Journal of Marketing.

(33)

33

Shrum, L. J., M. Liu, M. Nespoli, and T.M. Lowrey. 2012. Persuasion in the

marketplace: How theories of persuasion apply to marketing and advertising. In

J. Dillard and L. Shen (Eds.), The Persuasion Handbook (pp. 314–30). Thousand Oaks,

CA: Sage

Statista (2019). Netherlands: hours spent per day on social media, by age group 2017.

Retrieved from

https://www.statista.com/statistics/861834/hours-per-day-on-social-media-in-the-netherlands-by-age-group/ (accessed March 1, 2019).

Smith, L. A., & Foxcroft, D. R. (2009). The effect of alcohol advertising, marketing

and portrayal on drinking behaviour in young people: systematic review of prospective cohort

studies. BMC public health, 9(51), 1–11. https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2458-9-51

Stern, B. B., Russell, C. A., & Russell, D. W. (2007). Hidden persuasions in soap

operas: Damaged heroines and negative consumer effects. International Journal of

Advertising, 26(1), 9–36. https://doi.org/10.1080/02650487.2007.11072994

Sterling, K. L., Moore, R. S., Pitts, N., Duong, M., Ford, K. H., & Eriksen, M. P.

(2013). Exposure to celebrity-endorsed small cigar promotions and susceptibility to use

among young adult cigarette smokers. Journal of environmental and public health, 2013.

http://dx.doi.org/10.1155/2013/520286

Till, B. D., & Busler, M. (2000). The match-up hypothesis: Physical attractiveness,

expertise, and the role of fit on brand attitude, purchase intent and brand beliefs. Journal of

advertising, 29(3), 1–13.doi: 10.1080/00913367.2000.10673613

Tong, S. T., Van Der Heide, B., Langwell, L., & Walther, J. B. (2008). Too much of a

good thing? The relationship between number of friends and interpersonal impressions on

Facebook. Journal of computer-mediated communication, 13(3), 531–549.

(34)

34

Trubendorffer (n.d.). Binge drinken | alcoholprobleem onder studenten. Retrieved

from https://www.trubendorffer.nl/alcoholprobleem-onder-studenten/

Utz, S. (2010). Show me your friends and I will tell you what type of person you are:

How one's profile, number of friends, and type of friends influence impression formation on

social network sites. Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication, 15(2), 314–335.

https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1083-6101.2010.01522.x

Van Noort, G., Antheunis, M. L., & Van Reijmersdal, E. A. (2012). Social

connections and the persuasiveness of viral campaigns in social network sites: Persuasive

intent as the underlying mechanism. Journal of Marketing Communications, 18(1), 39–53.

https://doi.org/10.1080/13527266.2011.620764

Varsamis, E. (2018). Are Social Media Influencers the Next-Generation Brand

Ambassadors?. Retrieved from https://www.forbes.com/sites/theyec/2018/06/13/aresocial-

media-influencers-the-next-generation-brandambassadors/# 2d8b9e82473d.

Winpenny, E. M., Marteau, T. M., & Nolte, E. (2013). Exposure of children and

adolescents to alcohol marketing on social media websites. Alcohol and Alcoholism, 49(2),

154–159. https://doi.org/10.1093/alcalc/agt174

Xiang, L., Zheng, X., Lee, M. K., & Zhao, D. (2016). Exploring consumers’ impulse

buying behavior on social commerce platform: The role of parasocial

interaction. International Journal of Information Management, 36(3), 333–347.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijinfomgt.2015.11.002

Young, A. F., Gabriel, S., & Sechrist, G. B. (2012). The skinny on celebrities:

Parasocial relationships moderate the effects of thin media figures on women’s body image. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 3(6), 659–666.

(35)

35 Appendix A: Stimulus Materials

Condition 1: Alcohol post with sponsorship disclosure.

(36)

36 Condition 3: Neutral post with sponsorship disclosure.

(37)

37 Appendix B: Survey

Informatieblad

Beste student,

Je bent uitgenodigd deel te nemen aan een onderzoek dat wordt uitgevoerd onder

verantwoordelijkheid van onderzoeksinstituut ASCoR, onderdeel van de Universiteit van Amsterdam. ASCoR doet wetenschappelijk onderzoek naar media en communicatie in de samenleving. Het onderzoek waarvoor ik je medewerking heb gevraagd, is getiteld ‘Instagramgebruik onder studerende vrouwen.’ Aan dit onderzoek kunnen vrouwelijke studenten met een Instagram account deelnemen. In de online survey zul je vragen

beantwoorden over je Instagramgebruik. Het doel van het onderzoek is om meer te weten te komen over Instagramgebruik onder studenten en de effecten hiervan. Het invullen van de vragenlijst zal ongeveer 10 minuten duren. Omdat dit onderzoek wordt uitgevoerd onder de verantwoordelijkheid van ASCoR, Universiteit van Amsterdam, heb je de garantie dat:

1. Je anonimiteit is gewaarborgd en dat je antwoorden of gegevens onder geen enkele

voorwaarde aan derden zullen worden verstrekt, tenzij je hiervoor van tevoren uitdrukkelijke toestemming hebt verleend.

2. Je zonder opgaaf van redenen kunt weigeren mee te doen aan het onderzoek of je deelname voortijdig kunt afbreken. Ook kun je achteraf (binnen zeven dagen na deelname) je

toestemming intrekken voor het gebruik van je antwoorden of gegevens voor het onderzoek. 3. Deelname aan het onderzoek geen noemenswaardige risico’s of ongemakken voor je met zich meebrengt, geen moedwillige misleiding plaatsvindt, en je niet met expliciet

aanstootgevend materiaal zult worden geconfronteerd.

Voor meer informatie over dit onderzoek en de uitnodiging tot deelname kun je te allen tijde contact opnemen met Zenna Guijt, per adres: Universiteit van Amsterdam,

postbus 15791, 1001 NG Amsterdam; 020-525 3680; zenna.guijt@student.uva.nl. Mocht je naar aanleiding van je deelname aan dit onderzoek toch klachten of opmerkingen hebben over het verloop van het onderzoek en de daarbij gevolgde procedure, dan kun je contact opnemen met het lid van de Commissie Ethiek namens ASCoR, per adres: ASCoR secretariaat,

(38)

020-38

525 3680; ascor-secr-fmg@uva.nl. Een vertrouwelijke behandeling van je klacht of opmerking is daarbij gewaarborgd.

Ik hoop je hiermee voldoende te hebben geïnformeerd en bedank je bij voorbaat hartelijk voor je deelname aan dit onderzoek dat voor mij van grote waarde is.

Met vriendelijke groet,

Zenna Guijt

Informed consent

Ik verklaar hierbij op voor mij duidelijke wijze te zijn ingelicht over de aard en methode van het onderzoek, zoals uiteengezet in de uitnodigingsmail voor dit onderzoek.

Ik stem geheel vrijwillig in met deelname aan dit onderzoek. Ik behoud daarbij het recht deze instemming weer in te trekken zonder dat ik daarvoor een reden hoef op te geven. Ik besef dat ik op elk moment mag stoppen met het onderzoek.

Als mijn onderzoeksresultaten gebruikt worden in wetenschappelijke publicaties, of op een andere manier openbaar worden gemaakt, dan zal dit volledig geanonimiseerd gebeuren. Mijn persoonsgegevens worden niet door derden ingezien zonder mijn uitdrukkelijke toestemming. Als ik meer informatie wil, nu of in de toekomst, dan kan ik me wenden tot Zenna

Guijt, per adres: Universiteit van Amsterdam, postbus 15791, 1001 NG Amsterdam; 020-525 3680; zenna.guijt@student.uva.nl. Voor eventuele klachten over dit onderzoek kan ik me wenden tot het lid van de Commissie Ethiek namens ASCoR, per adres: ASCoR secretariaat, Commissie Ethiek, Universiteit van Amsterdam, postbus 15791, 1001 NG Amsterdam; 020-525 3680; ascor-secr-fmg@uva.nl.

(39)

39

Dank je wel voor je deelname. Je zult eerst wat korte vragen over jezelf beantwoorden.

A1. Heb je een Instagram account?

Nee

Ja

A2.Wat is je geslacht? Man

Vrouw

Zeg ik liever niet

A3.Wat is je leeftijd? ____

A4. Hoe vaak gebruik je gemiddeld Instagram? (hiermee wordt bedoeld de applicatie openen of naar de website gaan)

Minder dan een keer per maand

Maandelijks

Een keer per week

Meerdere keren per week

Een keer per dag

Twee tot vier keer per dag

Vijf keer tot zeven keer per dag

Acht keer per dag of vaker

A5. Gemiddeld genomen, hoeveel uren en/of minuten gebruik je Instagram dagelijks? (Mocht je geen uren en/of minuten Instagram gebruiken, vul dan het getal 0 in.)

Aantal uur ____

(40)

40 M

o

Op de volgende pagina zul je een Instagram post te zien krijgen. Neem de tijd om deze post goed te bekijken. Na 10 seconden kun je doorklikken en een aantal vragen beantwoorden over de post.

C1. Zag je een merk in de Instagram post die je zojuist gezien hebt? Nee

Ja

Ik weet het niet meer

C2. Welk merk zag je in de post? Fanta

Liefmans Heineken

Hero

Ik weet het niet meer

Ik heb geen merk in de post gezien

Referenties

GERELATEERDE DOCUMENTEN

TWENTENAREN hebben op dit moment geen stem in dit debat!.. En de WGR dan?  Centrale spelers: raadsleden  Raadslid taak: behartiging belang gemeente en lokale bevolking

Forty-one wounds (22.8%) were assessed as infected by the complete expert panel when culture results from wound biopsy were provided (in addition to clinical information) versus

Considering the literature on the influences on alcohol consumption amongst youth, a theoretical model has been proposed to investigate possible influences of

The means, standard deviations, minimums, maximums, and Cronbach’s Alpha were calculated for each of the research variables predictors (loneliness &amp; tolerance to

H1: Participants will indicate a higher purchase intention when they are exposed to a post by an influencer, who mentions and shows an alcoholic drink in this post (as opposed to

As low mental health conditions are associated with increased alcohol consumption, this study aims at exploring changes in the alcohol consumption patterns of students before

Keywords: Influencer marketing, influencer, social media, Instagram, sponsorship disclosure, product placement, source credibility, purchase intention, systematic literature

Het positieve verband tussen een hoge mate van uncertainty avoidance en een directere communicatiestijl, en de relatie tussen individualistische waarden en precieze communicatie