• No results found

Contrast sensitivity as a function of position on the retina

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Share "Contrast sensitivity as a function of position on the retina"

Copied!
8
0
0

Bezig met laden.... (Bekijk nu de volledige tekst)

Hele tekst

(1)

Contrast sensitivity as a function of position on the retina

Citation for published version (APA):

Rijsdijk, J. P., Kroon, J. N., & Wildt, van der, G. J. (1980). Contrast sensitivity as a function of position on the

retina. Vision Research, 20(3), 235-241. https://doi.org/10.1016/0042-6989(80)90108-X

DOI:

10.1016/0042-6989(80)90108-X

Document status and date:

Published: 01/01/1980

Document Version:

Publisher’s PDF, also known as Version of Record (includes final page, issue and volume numbers)

Please check the document version of this publication:

• A submitted manuscript is the version of the article upon submission and before peer-review. There can be

important differences between the submitted version and the official published version of record. People

interested in the research are advised to contact the author for the final version of the publication, or visit the

DOI to the publisher's website.

• The final author version and the galley proof are versions of the publication after peer review.

• The final published version features the final layout of the paper including the volume, issue and page

numbers.

Link to publication

General rights

Copyright and moral rights for the publications made accessible in the public portal are retained by the authors and/or other copyright owners and it is a condition of accessing publications that users recognise and abide by the legal requirements associated with these rights. • Users may download and print one copy of any publication from the public portal for the purpose of private study or research. • You may not further distribute the material or use it for any profit-making activity or commercial gain

• You may freely distribute the URL identifying the publication in the public portal.

If the publication is distributed under the terms of Article 25fa of the Dutch Copyright Act, indicated by the “Taverne” license above, please follow below link for the End User Agreement:

www.tue.nl/taverne

Take down policy

If you believe that this document breaches copyright please contact us at:

openaccess@tue.nl

providing details and we will investigate your claim.

(2)

CONTRAST SENSITIVITY AS A FUNCTION OF

POSITION ON THE RETINA

J. P. RLJSDIJK, J. N. KROON and G. J. VAN DER WILDT

Department of Biological and Medical Physics, Erasmus University. P.O. Box 1735. 3M)o DR Rotterdam. The Netherlands

(Received I Sepremher 1978)

Abstract-Contrast sensitivity has been measured as a function of the eccentricity along several mer- idians. The measurements were carried out with a two-dimensional sinusoidal grating (cross grating) presented in a surround with a luminance equal to the average stimulus luminance. The advantage of this stimulus over a one-dimensional grating is that it permits presentation of small-sized stimuli. in a surround of the same luminance, without discontinuities at the edges of the stimulus. The results appear to be comparable with contrast sensitivity data obtained with one-dimensional gratings, The decrease in sensitivity is found to be dependent on the meridian along which it is measured. Plots

of

equal sensitivity curves show that these curves are somewhat ellipsoidal.

1NTRODUCI’ION

Many investigations have been carried out on con- trast transfer by the human visual system. For example the spatial modulation transfer function (MTF) has been measured as a function of:

The luminance (van Nes 1967);

The number of periods in the grating (Savoy and McCann, 1975; Estevez and Cavonius, 1974; van der Wildt et ai.. 1976).

The orientation (Campbell et al., 1966).

In all these investigations the stimuli were pre- sented to the fovea. Measurements in which the stimuli were presented to the peripheral retina are mostly acuity measurements (Anstis, 1974; Berkley, 1975; Green, 1970) The only results we found in the literature giving information on the contrast sensi- tivity as a function of eccentricity are those of Bryng- dahl (1966). Hilz and Cavonius (1974) and Rovamo et al. (1978). Bryngdahl (1966) concluded from his results, obtained by measuring the subjective modu- lation depth of a one-dimensional grating, that the contrast sensitivity reaches a maximum at the peri- phery. Hib and Cavonius (1974), using an interference fringe method, showed that the sensitivity is maxi- mum in the fovea. For this reason we have measured the sensitivity for a sinusoidally modulated ‘stimulus. of which the width was as narrow as possible, i.e. one sinusoidal period of grating (Kroon et al., 1980). The stimulus height was 5”. The results obtained with this stimulus also showed a maximum sensitivity for fovea1 presentation. A disadvantage of this stimulus, however, was that because of its height, a part of the peripheral retina was always stimulated, even when the stimulus was presented foveally. So the maximum sensitivity for a foveally presented stimulus does not have to be caused by the fovea, but can be caused by a more sensitive peripheral part of the retina, situated above and below the fovea, which also is stimulated

because of the stimulus height of 5”. This means that if one wants to measure local contrast sensitivity, es- pecially around the fovea, a stimulus will have to be used which is limited in height as well as in width. One-dimensional gratings limited in height and width always show d&continuities at the upper and lower edges, if presented in a surround with a luminance equal to the average luminance of the 8rating. The discontinuities at the right and left side of the stimu- lus can be eliminated by starting the sinusoid at phase zero. To avoid these discontinuities at the upper and lower edges we have chosen to use a stimulus of which the luminance is modulated sinusoidally both in vertical and horizontal directions (cross gratings). The smallest ‘possible sinusoidal stimulus then is 1 x 1 period. We have measured the contrast sensi- tivity along several meridians as a function of eccen- tricity with this two-dimensional stimulus.

LVETHODS

Stimulus

Our stimulus was a two-Dimensions grating (referred to as a cross grating from now on). modu- lated sinusoidaliy in both the vertical and horizontal directions. The horizontal and vertical signals were multiplied The stimulus was presented on a TV monitor (Tektronix Picture mode 632 with phosphor WA D 6500). The stimulus surround was a circular field with a diameter of 15’. with a red fixation spot in the middle. The luminance of the surround was iOcd/m’, equal to the mean luminance of the stimu- lus. The viewing distance was 85 cm. The cross grat- ing was presented at different positions in this field but the fixation spot was always in the middle. An artificial pupil was not used. Two sine-wave gener- ators (wavetek 144) were used to provide the horizon- tal and vertical signals on the monitor. The width and the height of the stimulus were determined by pulse

(3)

2% 1. P. R~JSDIJK et ui.

generators (data pulse IOOA). A whole number of periods was always presented. The sine-wave gener- ators were started when the sine-wave crossed the zero level (background lumin~~) in both the hori- zontal and verticaf directions to avoid discontinuities at the edges. The measurements were carried out monocularly (right eye). A chin and forehead rest were used. We also performed me~urements with a one-dimensional grating. In this case. only the hori- zontal direction was modulated. The orientation was changed by rotating the TV monitor around an axis at right angles to the screen. The amplitude of the stimulus was sinusoidally modulated with a frequency of I Hz (.A = A,sinwt).

.M ensuring procedures

Two procedures were used.

I. The udjustment merhod. The subject decreased the modulation depth by 1 dB steps until the contrast threshold was reached. The average result of at least three adjustments are taken as threshold. The stan- dard error of mean was 0.7 dB.

2. Van BP&~ tracking. In this method the attenu- ation is controlled by a micro-processor. A more detailed description of this method is given by Keemink er al. (1979). The subject can cause the con- trast of the grating to fall by depressing a switch. As soon as the contrast is subthreshold. the subject releases the switch which causes the contrast to increase. When the grating is just visibie. the subject depresses the switch again. This process is repeated a dozen times. so that the modutation depth fluctuates around the subject’s threshold. The higher and lower contrast reversal values are averaged and this value is taken as the threshold. To avoid adaptation effects. the first four reversal values are not used. The thresh- old determined from the next eight reversal values is printed out automaticaliy. The presented threshold data are obtained by averaging the results of at least two measurements. The standard error of mean was 1 dB.

Experiments

We measured the MTF for one-dimensional grat- ings presented over the full stimulus field of IS” with different orientations, using Von Btkesy tracking, to see if it makes any difference if the threshold for cross gratings is determined by the frequencies present in

the cross grating other than in the horizonral dirsc- tion.

We needed this finding as a basis for comparison of the MTF for cross gratings and one-di~s~onal ,orat-

ings. for which purpose we measured the MTF for cross gratings also presented over the fuli stimulus field, again using Von Bikesy tracking. and compared the results with our measured values for one-dimen- sionai gratings (Kroon et al.. 1980). Using cross _erat- ings of limited dimensions (one period in the horizon- tal and one in the vertical direction). we measured the sensitivity as a function of the eccentricity of the stimulus (see Fig. I), using Von Bekisy nacking. To check for circular symmetry we measured the sensi- tivity in several directions using the adjustment method.

RESULTS

The results are obtained for two subjects, JPR and JNK. As there are no large differences between the results of both subjects, mostly the results of one sub- ject are given (JPR). As mentioned above, our two- dimensional stimulus was obtained by multiplying the horizontal and vertical sinusoidal signals_ Now Cari- son er al. (1977) found the same contrasr results with one-dimensional gratings and two-dimensional grat- ings obtained by adding the vertical and horizontal sinusoida signals. The difference between two-dimen- sional gratings obtained by addition and multipli- cation includes a difference in orientation of 45” (jsc Appendix). As we want to compare the results for two-dimensional and one-dimensional gratings. we checked whether the MTF for one-dimensional grat- ings depends on the orientation of the gratings by measuring the effect of orientation on the contrast threshold under the same conditions as for cross gat- ings. The results are given in Fig. 2.

We may conclude that our results do not depend on the orientation of the one-dimensional gratings. Now results published in the literature indicate that there is an orientation dependence for one-dimen- sional gratings. We wilt return to this point in the discussion.

Figure 3 gives the MTF measured with cross grat- ings presented over the full stimulus field of 15’ dia. The frequencies plotted as abscissae are those pre- sented in both horizontal and vertical directions. By

Fig. 1. Cross-section of the stimulus field in the horizontal direction. tricky. L = luminance. X = coordinate

(4)

Contrast sensitivity 237

I IO

sptiol frequency (c/d)

Fig. 1. The MTF for one-dimensional gratings. covering

the full 15 field. with different orientations. Dark surround.

way of comparison, the curve fitted to the results of Fig. 2 is reproduced here. We will return to the differ- ence between these results in the discussion.

The contrast sensitivity as a function of eccentricity, measured with a stimulus of 1 x I period, is given in Fig. 4.

The contrast sensitivity decreases monotonically with eccentricity for all spatial frequencies. We measured the sensitivity along several meridians to check whether or not the dependence on the eccen- tricity is the same in all directions. The results are given for two spatial frequencies in Figs 5 and 6. We see that the decrease in sensitivity as a function of eccentricity is monotonic in all directions. and

loo:l:\

1

j ,.P?. . , , ., ,,,

1

0.1 I IO

~potial frequency (c/d)

Fig. 3. The MTF for cross gratings (open circles) and for one-dimensional gratings (full line from Fig. 2).

Fig. 5. Contrast sensitivity along several meridians f, = 0.5 q’deg. Stimulus width Z x 1’. For symbols see

inset in figure.

0 1 2 3 4 5 6

eccentricity (deg)

Fig. 4. Contrast sensitivity as a function of eccentricity for a stimulus of one period in both horizontal and vertical

directions.

depends on the meridian along which the eccentricity is varied.

DlSCUSSlON

We may conclude from our results that the contrast threshold does not depend on the orientation of the grating. In the literature, however, results are given indicating that the contrast threshold does depend on the orientation of the gratings (Campbell er al., 1966; Berkley er al., 1975). If we examine these results more

1

f, = 0,5 c/d J.P.R.

IO’ I I . , , P , , , , 1

6 4 2 0 2 4 6

(5)

1:s J. P. RIJSDLIK YI ai 30 ’ f, = 4 c/‘d J .P.R.

11 ,

I

6 4 2 0 2 4 b eccentricity (deg}

%_a. 6. Contrast sensitivity along several meridians

i; = 6~ dep. Stimulus I x I‘. For symbols see inset in

figure.

closely. we can see that the dependence on the orien-

tation appears at spatial frequencies above 8-10 c/deg.

We did not measure at spatial frequencies above 10 c, deg. There is thus no real contradiction between our results and those published in the literature. The MTF measured with cross gratings is not the same as that for one-dimensional gratings. as we can see from Fig. 3. Carlson et nf. (1977) showed that the MTF for two-dimensional gratings obtained by adding the ver- tical and horizontal signals was equal to the MTF for one-dimensional gratings. As mentioned above. we generated our stimulus by multiplying the vertical and horizontal sinusoidal signals. One of the differ- ences between stimuli obtained by adding and multi-

spatial frequency (c/d)

/ i

0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

eccentricity (dcg)

Fig. 7. The %fTF for cross gratings. the frequencies and Fig. 9. Contrast sensitivity decrease as a function of eccen- modulation depth being measured at an angle of 45’ with tricity for cross gratings (A. A) and gratings (0 and l ). the horizontal. The full line is the same as in Fig. 2 (best fit The data of Rovamo rz ot. (1978) for one-dimensional grat-

for one-dimensional gratings). ings (0) are included by way of comparison.

f,=2c,‘d A J.N.K.. I

1

0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 eccentricity (deg)

Fig. 5. Contrast sensitivity decrease as a function of eccen- tricity for cross gratings@. A) and for one-dimensional gratings (D and n ). The data of Rovamo er al. (1978) for

one-dimensional gratings (e) are included by way of comparison. f,= 6 c/d A J.N.K. zi 6

s

.i)

10 3 : = * L\ _I.P.R. . J.N.K. 20 .

(6)

Contrast sensitivity

Fig. IO. Isocontrast sensitivity curves for j, = 0.5 c/deg. The data are obtained from Fig. 5.

plying the vertical and horizontal signals is a differ- ence in orientation. As we can see from the results of Fig. 2 the difference in orientation cannot cause the measured difference between the MTFs. In the Appendix we show that the stimulus obtained by multiplying can also be obtained by adding two sinus- oidal signals with frequencies a factor ,/z larger, modulation depth a factor 2 smaller and rotated through 45’. We can thus also characterize the cross gratings by the spatial frequencies at an angle of 4.5” with the horizontal if we multiply the frequencies in the horizontal direction by $ and divide the modu- lation depth by 2. Figure 7 shows the data for cross gratings corrected in this way, together with the curve for one-dimensional gratings from Fig. 2.

We see that there is good agreement between the results for one-dimensional and two-dimensional gratings. Our results are thus compatible with those of Carlson er nf. (1977). Because of this finding, we decided to characterize the cross gratings by the spa- tial frequencies and modulation depth at an angle of 45” with the horizontal, to facilitate comparison with one-dimensional gratings. Measuring with our two- dimensional “local” stimulus (1 x 1 period), we found that the contrast sensitivity decreases monotonically with eccentricity (Fig. 4). The contrast sensitivity is maximum in the fovea. For one-dimensional gratings, we also found a monotonic decrease in sensitivity with eccentricity. Our results obtained with

l The results for subjects JPR and JNK were obtained

with gratings 5” high and one period wide. The stimulus

was presented against a background of the same average luminance, 5’ high and 19’ wide. The measure’ments were carried out along the 0’ meridian (nasal). Rovamo et al.

(1978) used as stimulus the lower half of a circular field with a diameter of 2”. The background was dark. They measured the sensitivity along the 270” meridian.

(1 x 1 period) cross gratings (from Fig. 4) are com-

pared with results obtained with one-dimensional gratings in Figs 8 and 9 to see whether the depen- dence on eccentricity is the same for both cases. The data of Rovamo et al. (1978) for one-dimensional gratings are included in Figs 8 and 9 by way of com- parison.

There is reasonable agreement between our results obtained with one-dimensional and two-dimensional stimuli: and, taking the different stimulus conditions into account,* there is also reasonable agreement between our results and those of Rovamo er al. (1978). We used the two-dimensional stimuli to measure the contrast sensitivity as a function of position of the retina. Some qualitative remarks may be made about the results given in Figs 5 and 6. First, the slope of the curves is dependent on the spatial frequency. Over a range of 6” the decrease in sensitivity changes from a factor of 3 for f, = 0.5 c/deg to a factor of 16 for f, = 6 cideg. Secondly. the contrast sensitivity de- creases for all spatial frequencies more rapidly in the vertical direction than in the horizontal one.

Finally, for the results measured along the 45”/225’ and the 135’/315’ meridians the data are not sym- metrical with regard to the fovea. This is expressed in Figs 5 and 6 by the fact that in. the left-hand half of the figures the open circles .are situated above the closed ones, while in the right-hand half the open circles are underneath. This means that the contrast sensitivity in the upper half of the visual field is some- what lower than in the lower half. We can map the contrast sensitivity of the retina, on the basis of the results of Figs 5 and 6. Contours for constant contrast sensitivity based on these data are given in Figs IO and 11.

The full lines are drawn by eye through the data and represent curves of equal contrast sensitivity.

(7)

f. P. RIJ~~NJK rf nf.

Fig. 1 I. lsocontrast sensitivity curves forfi = 6 c deg. The data are obtained from Fig. 6

These maps provide a clear illustration of some of the Nes F. L. van (1968) Experimental studies in Spatiotem- remarks made above. The more rapid decrease in sen- poral contrast transfer by the human eye. Thesis sitivity in the vertical direction than in the horizontal (Utrecht Univ.) Bronder Offset, Rotterdam.

one gives somewhat ellipsoidal contours. Rovamo J., Virsu V. and Niitinen R. (1973) Cortical rmsg- n&cation factor uredicts the ohotopic contrast sensi-

tivity of peripheral vision. iJnr&e 27i. 5+-56.

ick,luHlrciyemenrs-We would like to acknowledge the Savoy R. L. and McCann J. J. (1975) Visibility of low-spa- helpful discussions with Professor G. van den Brink and tial-frequency sine-wave targets: dependence on the the technical asssistance of Mr J. B. P. van Deursen and number of cycles. J. opt. Sot. Am 65, 3-t3-350. Mr C. J. Keemink, Wildt G. J. van det Keemink C. J. and Brink G. van den

(1976) Gradient detection and contrast transfer by the human eye. Vision Res. 16, fO47-1053.

REFERENCES APPE;\iDIX

Anstis S. M. (1974) A chart demonstrating variations in acuity with retinal position. Vision Rrs. 14, 589-592. Berkley M. A., Kitterle F. and Watkins D. W. (1975) Grat-

ing visibility as a function of orientation and retinal eccentricity. Vision Res. 15, 239-244.

Bryn~dahI 0. (1966) Perceived contrast variation with eccentricity of spatial sine-wave stimuli. Vision Rrs. 6, 553-565.

Campbell F. W.. Kulikowski J. J. and Levinson J. (1966) The effect of orientation on the visual resolution of grat- ings. J. Pension. 181.427-436.

Carlson C. R.. Cohen R. W. and Gorog I. (1977) Visual processing of simple two-dimensional sinewave lumin- ance gratings. Vision Res. 17, 3.51-358.

Estevez 0. and Cavonius C. R. (t976) Low-frequency at- tenuation in the detection of gratings: sorting out the artefacts. Vision Res. 16 497-500.

Hilz R. and Cavonius C. R. (1974) Functional organization of the peripheral retina: sensitivity to periodic stimuli.

Vision Res. 14, 1333-1337.

Carlson er al. (1977) showed that the contrast sensitivity is the same for one-dimensional and two-dimensional grat- ings. They obtained their stimuli by adding the horizontal and vertical sinusoidal signals. We however multiplied these signals to obtain a stimulus of one sinusoidal period horizontally and one period vertically. Addition of these single-period signals would give a stimulus which was par- tially one-dimensional and partially two-dimensional. though under “full” field conditions there is no essential difference between the two stimuli. We will now show that the stimulus obtained by mu~tipl~ng can also be obtained bv adding two sinusoidal signals.

L ***..r.y = L(I + M(coslcf,cosYfJ (1) L mu,,..x,y = L(i + cM(cos .vI;I x cos .vf,,r (2)

= u 1 f M(f cos (-XI-s - VI-*) + + cos (.vf, f Yf,)))

Keemink C. J., Wildt G. J. van der and Deursen J. B. P. van (1979) Microprocessor-controlled contrast sensitivity measurements. Med. Biol. Eng. Comput. 17, 371-378. Kroon J. N., Rijsdijk J. P. and Wildt G. J. van der (1980)

Peripheral contrast sensitivitv for sinewave nratinns and sin& periods. Vision Res. Th\s issue. pp. 24c252.” 3Nes F. L. van and Bournan IM. A. (1967) Suatral Modula-

= L

! 1 -I- !$cosA(.v - Y) + cosi*(x + .t)) ) (3) Rotation of the axes through 45’ gives:

tion Transfer in the Human Eye. J. op;. Sot. Am S7.

401-406.

f = --k(_.Y + ‘) i5) I-!

(8)

We can now rewrite (3) as: L,,,,..,., =

L[

1 +

+.‘2f,(

7)

+

COS,Tf,

(

-7

>)I \’

2

Contrast sensitivity 241

= L

r

1 +

?(cos

\tif,x’ +

cos .,

Tf,y’)

1

17)

L L _I

This means that the stimulus obtained by multiplying two given signals can also be obtained by adding two signals with frequencies a factor \/2 larger, modulation depth a (6) factor 2 smaller and rotating the resulting stimulus through

Referenties

GERELATEERDE DOCUMENTEN

Een betrouwbare herkenning wordt bemoeilijkt door de vele kleuren van de vuilschaligheid, variaties in eikleur, eivorm en de aanwezigheid van kalk- en pigmentvlekken op de schil die

Het aantal zeedagen voor deze schepen lag in 2006 gemiddeld op 196 en de verdienste voor een opvarende op deze kotters lag met 46.000 euro op een 28% hoger niveau in vergelijking

Tongeren: Hondssstraat (prov. Limburg) A: Mozatekvloer van een Romeinse stads- woning. Mosaic floor of a Roman urban house. B: Vierkant paneel met cirkelvormig geome- trisch

BSVMM cannot be covered by existing convergence results, and in order to understand its convergence, it is necessary to exploit the special structure of the problem; thirdly, BSVMM

Voor ex-post evaluatie van in het verleden geformuleerd beleid waar veelal SMART beleidsdoelstellingen ontbreken en monitoring van de complete informatie niet voorhanden is kunnen

Sander Bax (De taak van de schrijver 2007) daarentegen plaatst terecht kritische nuances bij het gebruik van het begrip autonomie door Vaessens en door Ruiter en Smulders, en laat

Bij volwassen patiënten met eosinofiele oesofagitis, bij wie de behandeling met protonpompremmers niet of onvoldoende effectief is gebleken of onvoldoende wordt verdragen,

Hierdie opleidingsentrums word as voorbeelde gebruik omdat hierdie gevalle as illustrasie kan dien ten opsigte van die skakeling wat bewerkstellig word deur