• No results found

Brand versus public organization message effectiveness : the effect of source (brand vs. public) on consumer attitude towards sustainability communication

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Share "Brand versus public organization message effectiveness : the effect of source (brand vs. public) on consumer attitude towards sustainability communication"

Copied!
42
0
0

Bezig met laden.... (Bekijk nu de volledige tekst)

Hele tekst

(1)

Brand versus public organization message effectiveness

The effect of source (brand vs. public) on consumer attitude towards sustainability

communication

Mattia Falciani

12358401

Master thesis

Graduate School of Communication

MSc Persuasive & Corporate Communication

Supervisor: M. L. Fransen

(2)

1

Abstract

Consumers’ increasing interest in sustainability has resulted in a vast majority of brands engaging in Corporate Social Responsibility communication to the general public. Equally, governments and

public organizations regularly promote sustainability campaigns as well, to advocate those policies

and urge citizens towards environmental-friendly behaviours.

This study compares the effects of those two different types of message source, brand and public

organization, on attitudes toward the message and recycling intention. A sustainability message sent

by a public organization source resulted higher in credibility, than a brand source message. No

significant direct effect of message source on attitude towards the message and recycling intention

was found, however analysis found a mediation effect of credibility. Furthermore, an exploratory moderation for participant’s cynicism was found non-significant. Individuals seem to value credibility over likeability of a message source, when evaluating sustainability communication.

Brands engaging in this type of communication should ensure to have a sufficiently strong

reputation and may consider partnering with public organizations to achieve better effectiveness

results. Based on these findings, implications and recommendations for future research are

provided.

(3)

2

Introduction

In recent years we have experienced an increasing attention of mass media for environmental

issues. The myriad of scientific studies regarding climate change, global warming, plastic pollution

and similar, has gained broad space across printed and broadcast media, making all these issues a

wider public problem (Qader & Zainuddin, 2011). New protest movements and social enterprises

surged with the aim of tackling environmentally damaging actions, while public opinion became

more aware and attentive to the precarious conditions of the planet.

Governments have a strategic role in environmental issues, designing the national environmental

policies that regulate the use of resources and the correct disposal of waste, to which citizens and

enterprises alike must adhere. On the other side, national authorities are also subjected to external

influence. For example, to comply with international environmental objectives, such as the Kyoto

Protocol (1997) or the Paris Agreement (2015), or to align with further supranational directives, like

the European Union regulations. The big challenges that both scientific research and public opinion

are posing on public sector organizations impose governments to put in place concrete measures

that prompt citizens to change their behaviours in terms of sustainability and environmental

consciousness. Leveraging on its persuasive power, policymakers make use of advertising

campaigns in a similar fashion to businesses: they inform, involve and drive societal change across

the public (Wooden, 2008), but aim towards environmental challenges.

The effectiveness of an advertising message is related to various factors, however likeability and

credibility of the message source are considered two remarkable elements (Yilmaz et al., 2011).

Both the source characteristics in fact have an impact on how messages are processed by

individuals. A likeable source tends to increase the attention and purchase intention of the public,

while a credible source tends to increase message influence on the public (Yilmaz et al., 2011;

(4)

3

Businesses make use advertising to persuade consumers, changing their intentions towards the

ultimate goal of purchasing their products or services (Skard & Thorbjørnsen, 2014). However, as

the attention for sustainability and environmental-related causes translates also to consumers,

brands make large use of it in their corporate or commercial communications. Corporate Social

Responsibility has become overall popular among private companies, because of its proven effectiveness in sustaining a corporation’s prosperity over time (Öberseder et al., 2013). In some cases, the effective communication of a brand’s sustainability efforts or mission has been key for the company growth (e.g. Patagonia, The North Face, Lush, etc.). Nevertheless, the surge of these

types of communication, and relative market positioning, have sparked concern among consumers,

who are found to be especially sceptical of CSR-related campaigns. Customers do not always trust

the motives behind businesses’ environment-friendly actions (Becker-Olsen et al., 2006). For some

individuals, a cynicism attribute may play an interesting role in this sceptical attitude towards

sustainability messages. Cynicism is a personality trait consisting of disbelief towards others, which

may explain individuals’ reactions to external communication (Mohr et al., 1998), and therefore to messages communicated from a brand or a public organization source.

Public organizations, moreover, suffer from a systemic negative reputation, resulting in low

perceptions of reliability, efficiency and trust (Wæraas & Byrkjeflot, 2012; Goodsell, 2004). In a

simple way, environmental campaigns from governments or more in general public organizations,

do not have substantial echo among the public. So, businesses and public sectors engage in

communicating eco-friendly initiatives; despite different goals, they both promote sustainable

actions among the public, for institutional reasons on one side, and reputational one on the other.

This allows the emergence of interesting possibilities. Advertising practitioners, marketeers,

communication specialists and policy makers alike could benefit from understanding what drives

message source effectiveness. As climate and environment are critical future challenges for

(5)

4

in driving a sustainability-related behaviour change. Commercial partnerships and brand alliances

have resulted overtime in effective marketing opportunities for brands pursuing specific positive associations, benefiting of “spill over” effects for example (Simonin & Ruth, 1998). Similar partnerships could be envisioned for sustainability communication, ideally teaming up positive

elements of public and private.

Scholars haven’t yet extensively investigated the potentials and pitfalls of sustainability

communicated by these two sources. While studies on CSR have filled the journals of business

management and marketing communications in recent years, there is a lack of insight into the

results and effects of similar messages, coming from a public source. Some literature analysed

the opportunities of a public-private collaboration to enhance some public services (Guzmán &

Sierra, 2012), however to our knowledge no prior experimental study has investigated the

different effect of public and private message source on individuals exposed to this type of

communication. This research contributes to provide academic insight into an aspect of

sustainability communication that so far has received limited attention.

Therefore, this study aims to explore the influence of different message sources (brand vs. public

organization) on individuals’reaction towards sustainability-related message. Hence, the following

research questions were formulated.

RQ1 What is the effect of the source of a sustainability-related message (brand vs. public sector

organization) on attitudes towards the message and recycling intention? Do source credibility and

likeability mediate these effects?

RQ2: Is the effect of source on credibility and likability moderated by participants’ level of

(6)

5

Theoretical Background

In this section, the general notion of CSR will be examined first. Then, a brief

introduction of the concepts of attitude toward the message and recycling intention

will be presented, and finally, the source effect theory will be explained.

Furthermore, the focus on the effects of source credibility and source likeability will

follow, along with the explanation of the concept of cynicism.

Corporate Social Responsibility

Today's concept of corporate social responsibility was developed primarily during the 1960s in the

USA with the notion that corporations have responsibilities that go beyond their legal obligations

(Brønn & Vrioni, 2001). Enderle and Tavis (1998) describe corporate social responsibility as the

practice or policy of a corporation's social involvement beyond its legal requirements for the benefit

of the general society. Visser (2011) describes CSR as a method utilized by business to assemble,

not consume, a wide range of assets (economic, natural, human, etc.). This sense of acknowledging

the industrial footprint, taking responsibility for the resources used and the commitment to give

something back to communities, society and environment is common to many concepts of

sustainability. However, corporate sustainability is a complex and multidimensional approach that

has generated a varied lexicon and a rich stream of literature.

Despite the popular evidence that companies communicating their social responsibility get

rewarded by consumers (Öberseder et al., 2013; Ellen et al., 2000; Sen & Bhattacharya, 2001),

studies also showed that reactions to CSR activities are mediated by many factors (Yoon et at.,

(7)

6

CSR effectiveness (Yoon et at., 2006), as well as perceived congruence between consumers’ and

company’s characteristics in response to brand’s CSR initiatives (Sen & Bhattacharya, 2001). Sen & Bhattacharya (2001) examined the effects of CSR and new product information on a sample

of MBA students, finding out that the positive effect of CSR initiatives on company evaluation is

mediated by consumers’ perception of coherent relationship between personal and company characteristics. Consumers tend to have a stronger identification with the brand, and consequent

support towards it, when they recognize the brand’s CSR efforts as in line with the individual’s

self-concepts (Ahearne et al., 2005). However, it has been largely proven that factors related to the

company, the CSR cause and the consumer’s general belief may decrease consumer’s willingness to purchase a product (Sen & Bhattacharya, 2001), when exposed to a CSR campaign. Therefore,

findings on CSR effects remain mixed and changing depending on multiple factors.

Recycling intention and attitude toward the message

In the field of CSR, messages are not usually aimed at promoting purchases, but rather at

encouraging behaviour change, towards more environmentally friendly behaviours (Hartmann et al.,

2005). Recycling in particular, is a sustainable behaviour that aims at reducing human impact on the

environment (Wan et al., 2017). It also represents one of the most addressed issues from

governments around the world. Institutions have launched various schemes and plans to address the

problem and promote correct recycling practices among people (Wan et al. 2017; Wilson et al.,

2012). Therefore, a great majority of individuals are familiar with recycling and with

communication campaigns promoting its correct process.

As not every advertisement is designed to create an immediate purchase behaviour, increase in sales

is not the only measure of advertisement effectiveness. Cognitive and affective dimensions have

become more and more important to researchers and practitioners (Yilmaz et al., 2011; Lutz et al.,

(8)

7

popular in the academic world (Bergkvist & Rossiter, 2008). Attitude toward the ad is defined as “a predisposition to respond in a favourable or unfavourable manner to a particular advertising

stimulus during a particular exposure occasion” (MacKenzie & Lutz, 1989, p. 49). Extensive research has demonstrated AAD to be an important mediator of advertising response (MacKenzie &

Lutz, 1989; MacKenzie et al., 1986). There is also strong support that AAD together with brand

attitude may influence repeated purchasing behaviour (Lutz et al., 1983; MacKenzie et al., 1986).

Source effects

Companies that want to communicate their CSR efforts often do so “quietly”, in a challenging effort to balance internal stakeholders needs and potentially negative external reactions (Skard &

Thorbjørnsen, 2014). On the other side, public organizations that promote sustainability campaigns are indissolubly linked to their image of “public sector” institution. The difference in the source of a sustainability-related message may influence consumer evaluation and their level of trust towards

this type of communication.

Message source has been largely studied in several academic fields, from Communication Science

to Psychology and Consumer Behaviour, encompassing the different effects that it produces on the

persuasion process. Literature regarding source effects has been pioneered by Kelman (1961) who

proposed three psychological processes through which a message source can lead to attitude

change: internalization, identification and compliance. An individual may internalize the influence

of the message source as a consequence of perceiving the promoted behaviour as congruent with the

own values (Kelman, 1961). Such process of acceptance can only take place if the individual

considers the communication source as truthful and genuine, or in other words only if the

communication source possesses credibility, as Wilson & Sherrell (1993) point out. The process of

identification instead has source attractiveness (also named source likeability, as referred by Yilmaz

(9)

8

audience are more likely to accept and embrace opinions of attractive (or likeable) sources,

compared to unattractive sources. Finally, Kelman (1961) theorizes an influence process based on

compliance, when the message source act as an agent with “means control”. That is, the case in

which message source has the power of punishing the audience if the promoted opinion change

does not take place. However, this last process is generally seen as not relevant by researchers in

Communication Science (Wilson & Sherrell, 1993), because of the difficulty in recreating similar

conditions in a study. Hence, source credibility and source likeability, the influencing agents of the respective processes of internalization and identification proposed by Kelman’s framework,

represent the two main dimensions studied in the source effects theory.

Effects of source credibility

Source credibility concerns the communicator’s trustworthiness and expertise as it is perceived by the public (Kelman & Hovland, 1953; Yilmaz et al., 2011). Some researchers suggested that source

credibility represents a multidimensional construct, encompassing various dimensions such as

expertise, reliability, goodwill (Giffin, 1967), safety, qualification, dynamism (Berlo et al., 1969)

and finally also trustworthiness (Goldsmith et al., 2000). Others instead have studied it as a

comprehensive construct (Wilson & Sherrell, 1993; Yilmaz et al., 2011).

In persuasive communication, studies on source credibility have often been connected to attribution

theory: researchers have focused on the reason that consumers attribute to explain why a

communicator promotes a certain position or opinion through a message (Yilmaz et al., 2011). For

example, Eagly & Chaiken (1975) have demonstrated how individuals tend to disagree with

messages that are perceived as low credibility and attributed as containing bias elements.

Source credibility has been demonstrated to affect the success of firms's CSR initiatives, on par

with corporate reputation (Skard & Thorbjørnsen, 2014). Dahlén & Lange argue that consumers

(10)

9

communication efforts (as cited in Skard & Thorbjørnsen, 2014). In the case of CSR

communications, Strahilevitz (2003) points out that companies perceived as unethical do not benefit

from CSR positive effects on reputation while Osterhus (1997) demonstrated that trust in a

company, together with its involvement in the social activity, has a positive effect on CSR.

Research suggests that the negative role of bad reputations in consumer perception of CSR activities

is caused by the impression that companies are involved in these activities solely for commercial

reasons, to “clean” their negative image (Skard & Thorbjørnsen, 2014; Du et al., 2010). The authors explain that consumers attribute a higher amount of self-serving motives to CSR activities from a

company with poor reputation.

To the extent of our knowledge however, no studies have investigated individuals’ attitude towards sustainability communication promoted by public organizations. Research in the field of public

management has shown that many governments and public sector organizations in general have a

bad image among the citizens (Wæraas & Byrkjeflot, 2012). According to the authors, bad

reputation of public organizations is mainly linked with the type of services that such

administrations provide to citizens on a daily base, and not directly on their level of trustworthiness.

Furthermore, a public organization has no commercial purposes: it is run by a government, it

operates with money from taxpayers and it provides good and services for the benefit of the

community (BBC, 2020). Based on the above discussions, we expect that a public organization

source will be evaluated as more credible than a brand source. The first hypothesis is proposed.

H1 A message promoting an environmentally sustainable behaviour posted by a public sector organization will result in higher perceptions of credibility than a message posted by a brand.

Furthermore, Belch and Belch argued that when consumers trust the source of a message as

credible, they perceive positively both the ad and the brand (as cited in Yilmaz et al., 2011).

(11)

10

credibility levels, in obtaining the consumer’s attention, resulting in positive attitude toward the ad and towards the brand (Goldsmith et al., 2000). Generally, AAD has been studied as a mediator

between advertising influence on brand attitude and on purchasing intention (Lutz et al., 1983;

MacKenzie et al., 1986). This is also based on the extensive conceptual and empirical research

demonstrating a higher repurchase intention rate as consequence of ad attitude (Yilmaz et al., 2011).

Based on the mentioned studies, and on similar others that position AAD as antecedent of

behavioural measures of effectiveness (MacKenzie & Lutz, 1989) we hypothesize that attitude

toward the message may result in a similar effect on a similar behavioural intention dimension,

recycling intention. Therefore, according to the above discussions, the third hypothesis is proposed.

H3 There is a positive relationship between credibility and (a) attitudes towards the message and (b) behavioural intention.

Effects of source likeability

Source likeability is defined as “the ability to create a pleasant and enjoyable perception” (Yilmaz

et al., 2011, p. 892) among the communication target audience. Sanders describes it as the

communicator’s capacity of establishing a positive experience, especially in connection with the individual’s personal emotions (as cited by Yilmaz et al., 2011). Consistent research in the past has studied source likeability primarily on the physical attractiveness dimension, looking at physical

appearances of testimonials, endorsers or spokespersons for example (Chaiken, 1979; Joseph,

1982). While many likeability theories are focused on people, as Nguyen et al., (2013) point out,

likeability traits of individuals can be applied to firms, and more broadly, source of communication.

Human traits are often used in branding and persuasive communication, as firms want to appeal to

values, feeling and personal experiences in order to create an emotional link with the potential

customer (Poels & Dewitte, 2006). On the other side, the same traits can hardly be applied to public

(12)

11

for three reasons, as theorized by Wæraas & Byrkjeflot (2012): first, public organizations must

serve anyone, without possibility of segmenting specific buyer groups or ideal personas; second, as

they are generally based on a traditional bureaucratic management model, they suffer from a

widespread discreditation among the public, which makes it even harder to build charisma

(Goodsell, 2004); and third, public organizations struggle to have a positive reputation because the

issues they handle are very often insoluble. Problem such as unemployment, crime, poverty or

ecological crises are unlikely to be solved for good (Wæraas & Byrkjeflot, 2012). Furthermore,

citizens refer to such organizations to obtain essential and accessible services because they need

them, not for personal satisfaction, like in the case of buying goods for example, where an

emotional appeal is possible for companies with strong reputations (Fombrun & van Riel, 2004).

Based on the general poor image associated by citizens to public organizations (Wæraas &

Byrkjeflot, 2012; Goodsell, 2004) and, on the opposite, the common charisma and emotional appeal

linked to commercial brands (Poels & Dewitte, 2006; Fombrun & van Riel, 2004), the second

hypothesis is proposed.

H2 A message promoting an environmentally sustainable behaviour posted by a brand will result in higher perceptions of likeability than a message posted by a public sector organization.

There is consistent literature that demonstrates how source likeability is a relevant indicator of

effectiveness in advertising (Yilmaz et al., 2001; Chao et al., 2005). A stream of literature uses

social adaptation theory to justify source likeability (Kahle & Homer, 1985): source likeability is a

peripheral cue influencing the audience’s attitude when processing the advertisement information, if this information is useful in individuals’ adaptation to the environment (Yilmaz et al., 2011).

Most effects of source likeability are towards attitude change. Himmelfarb & Arazi (1974), Callcott

& Phillips (1996), Wilson & Sherrell (1993) are some of the researchers who demonstrated how a

(13)

12

likeability of a spokesperson by Phillips and Lee have shown how the dimension correlates

positively with both attitude toward the ad and attitude toward the brand (as cited in Yilmaz et al.,

2011). Based on the mediation relationship that AAD has on behavioural measures of advertising

effectiveness (Lutz et al., 1983; MacKenzie et al., 1986; MacKenzie & Lutz, 1989), and on the

previously mentioned studies, we formulate the following fourth hypothesis.

H4 There is a positive relationship between likability and (a) attitudes towards the message and (b) behavioural intention.

Cynicism

Several studies have showed that there is a large slice of consumers who are increasingly sceptical

about the positive statements shared on media or across commercial sources of information, by

companies (Mohr et al., 2001).

A sceptic is defined as a person who usually doubts that claims or statements are true, especially

those that other people believe in (Oxford Learner’s Dictionaries, 2020). According to Mohr et al.

(1998), scepticism together with cynicism explain the reactions that individuals have to others’ communications. Although often used interchangeably, the author specifies that these two terms

have a pretty clear separation: cynicism is an enduring disbelief of others, while scepticism may be

present only in specific circumstances. Cynicism should be defined as a personality trait (stable

over time), while scepticism represents a varying cognitive response, depending on the content and

the context in which a message is communicated. Despite this distinction, the two concepts are

highly correlated, as the cynicism trait impacts the level of scepticism of an individual (Mohr et

al.,1998). In other words, a cynic person is more likely to also being sceptical, compared to an

individual with low levels of cynicism. Kanter and Mirvis (1989, p. 301) further express the concept

as: “sceptics doubt the substance of communications; cynics not only doubt what is said but the

(14)

13

be influenced. As cynicism represents an enduring trait it would be very difficult, or nearly

impossible, to influence it; while on the other side scepticism can be leveraged, mainly increasing the audience’s knowledge.

As seen previously, consumers have in general a certain amount of distrust and cynicism towards

advertising, and this becomes remarkably more emphasized when the persuasive communication

concerns CSR-related initiatives (Becker-Olsen et al., 2006). Such negative attitude has been

observed to relate to individuals’ cynicism about the motives behind such “socially driven”

initiatives from organisations. One of the main “triggers” of this scepticism is the perception of the firm’s motives for such initiatives as “self-serving” (Brønn & Vrioni, 2001). Forehand and Grier (2003) specify, on the same line of thought, that the main drive of audience scepticism is in the

discrepancy between declared objectives and actual firm actions: when the former is of complete

social nature but the latter appear indeed as self-serving.

Considering that the concept of cynicism has been rarely investigated in experimental studies and

the fact that a large part of consumers is specifically cautious towards CSR-related messages, an

exploratory research question has been proposed.

Based on the theory and hypothesis discussed above, the following model (Figure 1) was

developed.

SOURCE

(brand vs public org.)

BEHAVIOURAL INTENTION AD-ATTITUDE LIKEABILITY CREDIBILITY H1 H2 H3a H3b H4a H4b CYNICISM CYNICISM

(15)

14

Figure 1: Conceptual model of the study

Method

Research design and sample

The study used a between-subjects experimental design where one independent variable with two

levels, (1) brand source and (2) public organization source, was manipulated. The dependent

variables were attitude toward the message and recycling intention. Furthermore, credibility and

likeability were measured as mediators while cynicism was explored as a continuous moderator. A

convenience sample of adults participated in the online experiment: subjects were recruited mainly

through social media (Facebook groups, personal Instagram profile) and electronic word of mouth

(WhatsApp). Every individual above the age of eighteen could take part in the study. In total, 208

individuals started the study. Ten of them did not properly complete it, one was below the minimum

required age, therefore they were excluded. Based on the manipulation check, further thirty-four

participants were excluded. After cleaning all the data, a total of 163 subjects constituted the final

sample. 65.0% of participants were female (106), 33.1% male (54) and 1.8% prefer not to specify,

with age ranging from 18 to 60 (M = 25.60, SD = 6.46). Majority of participants hold a bachelor

(45.4%) or master’s degree (31.3%).

Procedure

The study was built and distributed on the Qualtrics platform. Participants could access the study on

any device through internet connection. Once reached the first page, they were informed about the

general purpose of the study and requested to give their informed consent, which was required in

order to begin the experiment. The software then randomly presented participants with one of the

two stimuli; pictures graphically manipulated for the purposes of the study. Subsequently, subjects

(16)

15

variables (credibility and likeability) first, then the dependent variables (attitude toward the message

and recycling intention), and the moderator (cynicism). A manipulation check question was then

presented to the subjects. Finally, participants were asked some demographic information such as

age, gender and level of education. In the end, a text debriefed the participants regarding the

fictious nature of the stimuli and thanked for the participation.

Independent variable: message source

The two message sources consisted of images of a real print advertisement created to raise

awareness on the waste problem and promote recycling among citizens. The image presents a big

icon of the earth in the centre, composed paper and cardboard waste in blue and green, representing

the South American continent (green) and oceans (blue). On the left side of the picture the text: “the

most valuable assets need the strongest protection” and on the right side the call to action:

“separate and recycle – every act counts”. The two images exhibit these identical characteristics, except for the logo on the right bottom side, which identify the source of the message and represent

the experimental manipulation of the source. One picture presents the logo of a brand (HP,

Hewlett-Packard), and another one the logo of an intergovernmental organization (UN, The United Nations).

Participants were randomly exposed to the picture, presenting either the brand or the public

organization logo, all the other characteristics including visuals, colours, text and size were kept

constant. The stimulus material is showed in Appendix A.

Pre-test

In order to reduce potential errors that would damage the statistical estimation of effect on the

population, it was necessary to select two sources that generate a neutral perception among the

participants.

This also ensures the reliability of the results and prevent possible participants’ bias based on prior

(17)

16

In the pre-test participants were exposed to ten different brands and ten different public

organizations and were asked to rate on a 5-point bipolar semantic scale, each brand and

organization on three different dimensions: friendliness (very unfriendly – very

eco-friendly); attractiveness (very unattractive – very attractive); and familiarity (very unfamiliar – very

familiar) of the brands and organizations. These variables were measured because perceived

eco-friendliness and attractiveness of the source can potentially distort participants’ evaluation of the

message, either negatively or positively. Familiarity was measured to ensure an underlying recognition of the sources, which would ensure the participant’s comprehensive evaluation of the message.

The ten brands were selected from the median range of the Best Global Brand rankings, created by

Interbrand (2019), while the ten organizations were chosen from a list of international or

supranational organizations and agencies with the characteristic of operating worldwide and not

directly work on environmental-related issues.

Thirty respondents participated in the pre-test, which was distributed in paper forms, using a

convenience sampling at the university library. This to prevent that the same participants will then

take part in the study, and to partially replicate the characteristics of the study sample group

(university students). Hewlett-Packard (HP) resulted the best fit for brand source, based on its score

on eco-friendliness (M = 2.63, SD = 1.03), attractiveness (M = 3.23, SD = 0.72) and familiarity (M

= 4.06, SD = 1.22). The United Nations (UN) resulted the best option for public organization

source, based on the same measures of eco-friendliness (M = 3.26, SD = 1.08), attractiveness (M =

3.93, SD = 0.83) and familiarity (M = 4.10, SD = 1.02). These two sources were chosen because

scored close to the cumulative means of eco-friendliness (M = 3.07), attractiveness (M = 3.45) and

familiarity (M = 3.79) and had a low amount of dispersion. A one-way analysis of variances

(ANOVA) was used to test that all the selected brands did not differ significantly from each other

(18)

17

All the analyses resulted significant, meaning that we found variation on the organization’s score in

each group: familiarity, F (9, 290) = 10.13, p < .001, eta2 = 0.23; attractiveness, F (9, 290) = 6.37, p

< .001, eta2 = 0.16; and eco-friendliness, F (9, 290) = 6.40, p < .001, eta2 = 0.16. A more

interesting result is the post-test: Bonferroni post-hoc tests indicated no significant difference

between UN and HP on familiarity (Mdifference = -.30, p = 1.000), attractiveness (Mdifference = -.70, p =

.217) and eco-friendliness (Mdifference = -.63, p = .631). Therefore, UN and HP did not differ

significantly from each other on the three measures.

Mediators: Credibility and Likeability

Participants were asked to evaluate the message they saw. In order to measure perceived credibility

and likeability of the message, two sets of four seven-point bipolar items were used, selected from

scales used by Yilmaz et al. (2011). The two scales were first developed by respectively Zhang and

Buda (1999) and Whittler and DiMeo (1991), as cited by Yilmaz et al. (2011). The complete scales

are shown in Appendix 1. Three items of the credibility scale and two on the likeability scale were

not taken into consideration because deleted in relevant previous studies due to failed confirmatory

factor analyses (Yilmaz et al., 2011).

Regarding credibility, the four items measure how reliable, knowledgeable, credible and

trustworthy the message is perceived to be by the participants (Cronbach’s coefficient alpha, α =

.93). The four items were then averaged into one credibility dimension (M = 5.54, SD = 1.34).

For likeability, the items evaluate to what extent participants grasped the messages as friendly,

likeable, warm and appealing (α = .867). Likeability was averaged into a unique dimension (M =

5.08, SD = 1.37).

(19)

18

Participants reaction to the message was measured with the popular attitude toward the ad (AAD)

measure used by MacKenzie et al. (1986). Two seven points bipolar scales were introduced by “I consider the message as…”, measuring the extent to which participants perceived the message as favourable/unfavourable and interesting/boring. The reliability of AAD, comprising the two items,

was good: α = .800 (M = 5.31, SD = 1.33).

Dependent variable: Recycling intention

Participants future intention in recycling their waste was measured with three 7-point Likert scales, anchored by ‘strongly disagree – strongly agree’, based on Wan et al. (2017). Introduced by ‘Please indicate to what extent do you agree or disagree with the following statements…’, participants were

asked to score the following statements : ‘I intend to recycle my recyclables in the next four weeks’; ‘I will recycle my recyclables every time I have it for disposal’; ‘I am willing to participate in the recycling scheme in the future’. The dimension ‘recycling intention’ had a good reliability, α = .883

(M = 6.03, SD = 1.27)

Moderator: Cynicism

Participants level of cynicism was measured using six 7-point Likert scales by Moher et al., (1998).

The scales were introduced by ‘Please indicate to what extent you agree or disagree with the

following statements…’ and composed of statements indicating individual’s social behaviour (i.e. ‘most people are just out for themselves’, ‘most people will tell a lie if they can gain by it’). The six averaged items formed the ‘cynicism’ dimension (M = 4.67, SD = 1.25) with a good reliability, α = .850.

Manipulation check

A manipulation check was conducted for message source. Participants were asked which was the

source of the message they were exposed to at the beginning of the online experiment, choosing

(20)

19

Results

Manipulation check

A total of 34 participants (17% of the initial sample) were excluded because of failing the

manipulation check.

Randomization check

To establish that both the conditions did not differ in terms of participants demographics,

randomization checks were performed. In order to check if participants’ age was comparable over

the two source conditions, a one-way ANOVA was conducted. This one-way analysis of variance

had message source (brand vs. public organization) as independent variable and age as dependent.

The ANOVA showed that participants’ mean age in the brand source condition (M = 25.81, SD = 6.87) was not significantly different from participants’ mean age in the public organization source

condition (M = 25.40, SD = 6.09), F (1, 162) = 0.16, p = .690.

Additionally, cross-tabulation showed that the sample was equally distributed over the two

conditions for gender (χ2 (2) = 3.07; p = .215) and education (χ2 (5) = 10.57; p = .060). Hence, the

randomization check was successful.

RQ1 Hypothesis testing

To answer the first research question, two separate regression analyses were conducted using the

PROCESS (Hayes, 2019) modelling tool for IBM SPSS (version 25), with Model 4 for mediation.

One analysis included AAD as dependent variable, the second had recycling intention as dependent

variable.

It was first hypothesized that a message with a public organization source results in higher levels of

(21)

20

variable, regression analysis showed a significant effect of message source on credibility, b = .61,

SE = .19, t (161) = 3.16, p = .002, 95%BCBCI [.23, .99]. A message from a public organization

source (M = 5.84, SD = 1.26) leads to a 0.61 higher score on credibility compared to the same

message from a brand source (M = 5.23, SD = 1.20). H1 is therefore confirmed.

Later, it was tested the second hypothesis that message from brand source leads to a higher score on

likeability compared to public organization source. The analysis showed a non-significant

relationship between the source of the message and the mediator likeability, b = .01, SE = .20, t

(161) = 0.07, p = .945, 95%BCBCI [-.39, .42]. A successive analysis of variance showed how brand

source (M = 5.06, SD = 1.33) scored lower than public organization source (M = 5.08, SD = 1.28)

on likeability. H2 must be rejected.

A significant relationship (H3a) between credibility and AAD was found, b = .15, SE = .07, t (159) =

2.15, p = .033, 95%BCBCI [.01, .28]. For every one-unit increase in credibility, attitude toward the

message increases by 0.15. [indirect effect = 0.07, boot SE = 0.09, 95% BCCI (-.18; .18)].

Finally, the effect of likeability on AAD was tested (H4a). A significant effect was found, b = .54, SE

= .06, t (159) = 8.44, p < .001, 95%BCBCI [.42, .67]. For every one-unit increase in likeability

there is a 0.54 increase in AAD. [indirect effect = 0.00, boot SE = 0.09, 95% BCCI (-.18; .18)]. The

first part of our fourth hypothesis is therefore confirmed.

A second analysis using the same model but with recycling intentions as dependent variable was

conducted to test H3b and H4b. It was found a statistically significant effect of credibility on

recycling intention (H3b), b = .21, SE = .09, t (159) = 2.26, p = .025, 95%BCBCI [.02, .40]. For

every one-unit increase in credibility, recycling intention increases by 0.21. [indirect effect = 0.10,

boot SE = 0.05, 95% BCCI (.02; .22)]. H3 is therefore completely confirmed. Furthermore, when

credibility is excluded, the effect of source on the two variables disappears, displaying a full

(22)

21

For H4b instead it was found a non-significant effect of likeability on recycling intention, b = -.01,

SE = .09, t (159) = -.08, p = .934, 95%BCBCI [-.19, .17]. [indirect effect = 0.00, boot SE = 0.01,

95% BCCI (-.01; .03)]. Thus, H4b is rejected and H4 can only be partially accepted.

RQ2 Testing

The second research question explores the possibility that the effect of message source on

credibility and likeability may be moderated by participant’s level of cynicism. Model 7 from

PROCESS was used to investigate the possibility that the relationship between source and

credibility changes depending on the level of cynicism of participants. We found a not significant1

moderation effect of cynicism on the two variables, b = -.20, SE = .15, t (159) = -1.28, p = .201,

95%BCBCI [-.50, .11]. Also, the moderation on H2 (source - likeability relationship) resulted not

significant, b = -.20, SE = .16, t (159) = -1.24, p = .218, 95%BCBCI [-.52, .12]. The effect of

message source on likeability was not mediated by cynicism. Figure 2 shows a visual representation

of the tested analyses.

1

For completeness, moderation was also tested with the initial sample (N = 197), including participants who failed the manipulation check. This showed a significant moderation effect of cynicism on the effect of source on credibility, b = -.36, t (193) = -2.448, p = .015, 95%BCBCI [-.38, -.05]. However, moderation resulted significant only at low level of cynicism (c = 0.95, p = .001) were subject would score 0.95 points higher on perceived credibility. The moderation effect on likeability instead was not significant, b = -.29, SE = .16, t (193) = -1.873, p = .063.

(23)

22

Figure 2: Conceptual model with analyses results

Additional analysis: Direct effect

Analysis in PROCESS (Hayes, 2019; Model 4) showed also the direct effect of source on the two

dependent variables. Message source had a non-significant direct effect on AAD, b = .15, SE = .15, t

(159) = 1.009, p = .315, 95%BCBCI [-.14, .44]. An independent-samples t-test showed that public

organization source scored higher (M = 5.45, SD = 1.21) than brand source (M = 5.24, SD = 1.24)

on attitude towards the ad.

Message source had a non-significant direct effect on recycling intention, b = -.39, SE = .21, t (159)

= -1.86, p = .064, 95%BCBCI [-.80, .02]. An independent-samples t-test showed that brand source

scored higher (M = 6.14, SD = 1.28) than public organisation (M = 5.88, SD = 1.32) on recycling

intention. As the effects of the independent variable on the dependent ones are not significant, the

study model presents a full mediation. A visual representation of the variation in means can be

found in Figure 3.

SOURCE

RECYCLING INTENTION AD-ATTITUDE LIKEABILITY CREDIBILITY b = 0.54, p < .001 b = 0.21, p = .025 b = 0.01, p = .945 CYNICISM CYNICISM

brand vs. public org.

b = - 0.20 p = .201

b = - 0.20 p = .218

(24)

23 5,23 5,06 5,24 6,14 5,84 5,08 5,45 5,88 5 5,2 5,4 5,6 5,8 6 6,2

Credibility** Likeability* Attitude toward the

message*

Recycling intention*

Message source means

Brand source Public organization source

Note: *p > .05, ** p < .05

Figure 3: Message source means

Conclusions & discussion

This study examined the different effects of message source in sustainability-related

communication, on consumers’ attitude toward the ad and recycling intention, and the potential

differences in these effects for cynical subjects.

The first objective of the study was to indicate whether the two message sources scored differently

on perceived credibility and likeability. Successively, the study aimed at investigating whether

credibility and likeability positively mediated the effect of message source on AAD and recycling

intentions. In the second part of the study, the aim was to gain insight into how the effect of

message source on credibility and likeability could change, depending on participants’ level of cynicism.

(25)

24

Regarding the first objective, it was found that message with a public organization source score

significantly higher in credibility than the same message from a brand source. These findings

indicate that brands communicating sustainability efforts are perceived by the public as less credible

and trustworthy in their intentions than public organizations. This is pertinent with literature

indicating how consumers tend to be dubious regarding CSR and corporates’ real motives for it

(Becker-Olsen, 2006; Brønn & Vrioni, 2001; Forehand and Grier, 2003). In accordance with

Adhearne et al. (2005), this result could depend on the fact that participants did not evaluate the promoted environmental cause as pertinent with the brand’s attributes.

On the other hand, no statistically significant effect of message source on likeability was found.

Therefore, a message from a brand source has no different effect on likeability compared to a public

organization source. Wæraas & Byrkjeflot (2012) pointed out that public organizations generally

suffer from a bad image and poor reputation, while Nguyen et al., (2013) indicated that brands are

likeable because consumers tend to personify them on an affect-based perception. Results of this

study counter such assumptions. Current findings imply that in the context of a

sustainability-related message, the type of message source does not significantly change consumer’s perception of source likeability. Surprisingly, it was also found a slightly higher likeability score of public

organization source (M = 5.08, SD = 1.28) compared to brand source (M = 5.06, SD = 1.33). The

fact that such difference is relatively small could be related with the specific context of a CSR-type

of communication, where consumer’s distrust toward brands is particularly enhanced.

The second objective of the study investigated whether credibility positively mediated the

effect of message source on AAD (H3a) and recycling intention (H3b). A positive effect of credibility

on two dependent variables was found. This is in line with the suggested literature that refers to

source credibility as one of the main factors in persuading individuals to accept the content of a

(26)

25

effect of message source on credibility, the study has showed a full mediation of credibility, as it

was hypothesized.

On the other hand, current results demonstrated that likeability has an effect on AAD but not on

recycling intention. This is in contrast with already mentioned studies that regard likeability as

positively related to attitude change (Himmelfarb & Arazi, 1974) and correlated with credibility in

affecting positively AAD, attitude toward the brand and purchase behaviour (Yilmaz et al., 2011).

Furthermore, results showed that message source has no significant effect on likeability. Therefore,

perceived likeability of the message does not mediate this relationship.

These results are fascinating, because they show that likeability is not a crucial element in driving individual’s intention of adopting a more sustainable behaviour. Despite likeability’s importance in traditional advertising has been largely accepted (Kelman, 1961; Nguyen et al., 2013; Yilmaz et al.,

2011), results of this study demonstrated that credibility (over likeability) of a message source represent a key explanation of individuals’ attitude towards a sustainability-related message and the behavioural intention promoted through it.

Finally, the effect of message source on credibility and likeability was not moderated by participant’s level of cynicism. This finding, although unexpected, must be considered in the explorative perspective of the research question. The small amount of experimental research

available on cynicism within the field of communication does not encompass CSR-related

scenarios. Despite it was expected that participants’ cynicism would accurately repeat results from

more popular experiments on consumer scepticism (Becker-Olsen et al., 2006; Brønn & Vrioni,

2001; Forehand and Grier, 2003) presenting a significant moderation of the effect of message

source, results showed that this was not the case. Therefore, it is interesting to see that the level of

cynicism of an individual does not change how a message by a brand or by a public organization

(27)

26

Limitations and implications for future research

Some limitations must be taken into account for the current study. First, some dimension

investigated in this paper present little prior research: cynicism has been very rarely investigated in

experimental studies and often disregarded in favour of the closely-related scepticism. Future

research could address this area, measuring also the effect of behavioural scepticism on the

analysed variables. This would allow to confirm the current suppositions regarding a considerable

difference between the effect of cynicism and scepticism in sustainability-related communication.

As in every experimental study, sample represents another limitation. The use of convenience

sampling, mainly through social media and electronic word of mouth, resulted in majority of young

participants, with a higher educational background. This reduced considerably the number of older

adults with a lower education present in the sample and facilitated an overrepresentation of the

former socio-demographic group. Taken this under consideration, it is hard to generalize the

findings of this study to a wide population, as the external validity might be impacted by this.

The third limitation concerns the stimuli used for the experimental condition. First, only two

conditions (brand vs. public organization) were tested in this study. Including other manipulations

(non-governmental organizations, for example) could have yielded to different results and produced

interesting outcomes for policy makers and advertising practitioners. As individuals attribute higher

credibility to NGOs compared to brands when communicating sustainability-related practices, these

types of organizations might result overall highly likeable to the general public, and potentially

more credible to them, than governments-related organizations.

Furthermore, in the pre-test conducted for the selection of the two manipulations, elements such as

nationality of the participants, their level of knowledge about supranational public organizations

and regarding sustainability practices adopted by such brands and organizations could have

(28)

27

composed the pre-test questionnaire and their source might have shrunk the extent to which the

current results are generalizable to. In particular, brands for the pre-test were selected from the

Interbrand (2019) ranking which, according to its methodology, excludes companies who do not

have revenue from foreign countries, are not present in both Asia, Europe and North America and

have a negative economic profit on the longer term. Given the fact that the enormous amount of

companies available worldwide and their very different industries are hard to dilute in a very

precise and accurate manner, further studies can address this aspect. Future research might want to

investigate to what extent source effects of sustainability-related messages variate based on

different levels of popularity or wealth of the brand source, for example by adding several types of

brands in the manipulation phase. This could lead to deeper insight into whether the type of brand

source has an effect on the outcome of a CSR message and, eventually, to what extent.

In addition, the experimental conditions of this study presuppose a certain level of knowledge about

the HP brand and the UN organization. Such knowledge however may vary noticeably depending

on the participant’s country of residence. This means that replicating the same study in different

countries could potentially unveil different results based on cultural and social factors, consumers’

awareness or market penetration of the brand, and so on. These aspects suggest that it could be

interesting for future studies to replicate the present research only at national scope. The use of a

local brand and a national public organization, with a filtered sample of solely residents in the

country, may provide more significant results and insightful scenarios. For example, a replication

study conducted in the UK, using a domestic brand (i.e. Marmite) and public organization (i.e. the

Home Office) would provide results with higher scientific validity.

This study addressed the relatively unexplored differences between the effects of a

sustainability-related message communicated by a brand and by a public organization. The findings of the study

show valuable insight for future efforts in the promotion of sustainable and eco-friendly behaviours

(29)

28

sustainability should be communicating taking care the importance of the credibility of the message

source. Public organizations should focus on their trustworthy status in their communication, while

brands that want to communicate CSR should ensure that they are perceived as enough credible in

doing so. Otherwise they should first focus on changing that perception in the public. Finally, this

study opens possibilities towards future potential partnerships between the two types of sources, to

help each other reaching the effectiveness in sustainability-related communication.

References

Ahearne, M., Bhattacharya, C. B., & Gruen, T. (2005). Antecedents and consequences of

customer-company identification: Expanding the role of relationship marketing. Journal of applied

psychology, 90(3), 574.

Amos, C., Holmes, G. & Strutton, D. (2008) Exploring the relationship between celebrity endorser

effects and advertising effectiveness: a quantitative synthesis of effect size. International Journal of

Advertising, 27(2), pp. 209–234.

Barr, S. (2016). Environment and society: Sustainability, policy and the citizen. Routledge.

Basu, K., & Palazzo, G. (2008). Corporate social responsibility: A process model of sensemaking.

Academy of Management Review, 33, 122–136.10.5465

BBC (2020) Types of business organisations - National 5 Business management - BBC Bitesize.

Retrieved 31 January 2020, from https://www.bbc.co.uk/bitesize/guides/zpx7gdm/revision/1

Becker-Olsen, K., Cudmore, B. & Hill, R. (2006) The impact of perceived corporate social

(30)

29

Belch, G., & Belch, M. (2015). Advertising and promotion: an integrated marketing

communications perspective (10th ed., global ed.). Singapore: McGraw-Hill Education.

Bergkvist, L., & Rossiter, J. R. (2008). The role of ad likability in predicting an ad's campaign

performance. Journal of advertising, 37(2), 85-98.

Berlo, D. K., Lemert, J. B., & Mertz, R. J. (1969). Dimensions for evaluating the acceptability of

message sources. Public opinion quarterly, 33(4), 563-576.

Brønn, P. S., & Vrioni, A. B. (2001). Corporate social responsibility and cause-related marketing:

an overview. International journal of Advertising, 20(2), 207-222.

Callcott, M. F., & Phillips, B. J. (1996). Observations: Elves make good cookies: Creating likable

spokes-character advertising. Journal of Advertising Research, 36(5), 73-73.

Chaiken, S. (1979). Communicator physical attractiveness and persuasion. Journal of Personality

and social Psychology, 37(8), 1387.

Chao, P., Wuhrer, G. & Werani, T. (2005) Celebrity and foreign brand name as moderators of

country-of-origin effects. International Journal of Advertising, 24(2), pp. 173–192.

Dahlén, M., Rosengren, S., & Törn, F. (2008). Advertising creativity matters. Journal of

Advertising Research, 48(3), 392-403.

Dahlsrud, A. (2008). How corporate social responsibility is defined: an analysis of 37 definitions.

Corporate Social Responsibility and Environmental Management, 15(1), 1–13.

Directive 2003/4/EC of the European Parliament and of the Council on public access to

environmental information and repealing (2003), Official Journal L41, p.27.

Du, S., Bhattacharya, C. B., & Sen, S. (2010). Maximizing business returns to corporate social

responsibility (CSR): The role of CSR communication. International Journal of Management

(31)

30

Eagly, A. H., & Chaiken, S. (1975). An attribution analysis of the effect of communicator

characteristics on opinion change: The case of communicator attractiveness. Journal of personality

and social psychology, 32(1), 136.

Eisend, M. (2003), “Is it Still Worth to be Credible? A Meta-Analysis of Temporal Patterns of

Source Credibility Effects in Marketing”, Association of Consumer Research Proceedings,October,

Toronto, Canada, forthcoming.

Ellen, P. S., Mohr, L. A., & Webb, D. J. (2000). Charitable programs and the retailer: do they mix?.

Journal of retailing, 76(3), 393-406.

Enderle, G. & Tavis, A.L. (1998) 'A balanced concept of the firm and the measurement of its

long-term planning and performance', Journal o/ Business Ethics, 17(11), 1129-1143

European Commission. (2002). COMMUNICATION FROM THE COMMISSION concerning

Corporate Social Responsibility: A business contribution to Sustainable Development [Ebook] (p.

5). Brussels. Retrieved from

https://ec.europa.eu/europeaid/communication-commission-concerning-corporate-social-responsibility-business-contribution_en

European Environmental Agency. (2016). Communication, environment and behaviour [Ebook]

(13th ed., pp. 4-15). Luxembourg. Retrieved from

https://www.eea.europa.eu/publications/communication-environment-and-behaviour

Fombrun, C. (1996). Reputation: Realizing value from the corporate image. Boston: Harvard

Business School Press.

Fombrun, C. (2012). The Building Blocks of Corporate Reputation: Definitions, Antecedents,

Consequences. In The Oxford Handbook of Corporate Reputation.

Fombrun, C. J. and C. van Riel, B. M. 2004. Fame and Fortune: How Successful Companies

(32)

31

Forehand, M. R., & Grier, S. (2003). When is honesty the best policy? The effect of stated company

intent on consumer skepticism. Journal of consumer psychology, 13(3), 349-356.

Giffin, K. (1967). The contribution of studies of source credibility to a theory of interpersonal trust

in the communication process. Psychological bulletin, 68(2), 104.

Goldsmith, R. E., Lafferty, B. A., & Newell, S. J. (2000). The impact of corporate credibility and

celebrity credibility on consumer reaction to advertisements and brands. Journal of advertising,

29(3), 43-54.

Goodland, R. (1995). The concept of environmental sustainability. Annual review of ecology and

systematics, 26(1), 1-24.

Goodsell, C. T. 2004. The Case for Bureaucracy. Washington, DC: CQ Press.

Guzmán, F., & Sierra, V. (2012). Public-private collaborations: branded public services? European

Journal of Marketing, 46(7/8), 994–1012.

Hartmann, P., Apaolaza Ibáñez, V., & Forcada Sainz, F. J. (2005). Green branding effects on

attitude: functional versus emotional positioning strategies. Marketing Intelligence & Planning,

23(1), 9-29.

Himmelfarb, S., & Arazi, D. (1974). Choice and source attractiveness in exposure to discrepant

messages. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 10(6), 516-527.

Hovland, C. I., and W. Weiss (1951), “The Influence of SourceCredibility on Communication Effectiveness”, Public OpinionQuarterly, 15, 635-650.

Irwin A, 1995 Citizen Science: A Study of People, Expertise and Sustainable Development

(Routledge, London)

Interbrand. (2019). Best Brands. Retrieved 8 January 2020, from

(33)

32

Joseph, W. B. (1982). The credibility of physically attractive communicators: A review. Journal of

advertising, 11(3), 15-24.

Kahle, L.R. & Homer, P.M. (1985) Physical attractiveness of the celebrity endorser: a

social adaptation perspective. Journal of Consumer Research, 11(4), pp. 954–961.

Kanter, Donald L. and Philip H. Mirvis (1989), Cynical Americans: Living and Working in an

Age of Discontent and Disillusionment, San Francisco CA: Jossey-Bass Publishers

Keh, H. T., & Xie, Y. (2009). Corporate reputation and customer behavioral intentions: The role of

trust, identification and commitment. Industrial Marketing Management, 38, 732–742.

Kelman, H. (1961). Processes of Opinion Change. The Public Opinion Quarterly, 25(1), 57–78.

Kelman, H. C., & Hovland, C. I. (1953). " Reinstatement" of the communicator in delayed

measurement of opinion change. The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 48(3), 327.

Kyoto Protocol to the United Nations Framework Convention on Climate Change. Kyoto,

Dec. 11, 1997, U.N.T.S. 30822.

Landrum, N. E. (2018). Stages of Corporate Sustainability: Integrating the Strong Sustainability

Worldview. Organization & Environment, 31(4), 287–313.

https://doi.org/10.1177/1086026617717456

Lii, Y. S., & Lee, M. (2012). Doing right leads to doing well: When the type of CSR and reputation

interact to affect consumer evaluations of the firm. Journal of business ethics, 105(1), 69-81.

Lutz, R. J., MacKenzie, S. B., & Belch, G. E. (1983). Attitude toward the ad as a mediator of

advertising effectiveness: Determinants and consequences. ACR North American Advances.

MacKenzie, S. B., & Lutz, R. J. (1989). An empirical examination of the structural antecedents of

(34)

33

MacKenzie, S. B., Lutz, R. J., & Belch, G. E. (1986). The role of attitude toward the ad as a

mediator of advertising effectiveness: A test of competing explanations. Journal of marketing

research, 23(2), 130-143.

Marin, L., Ruiz, S., & Rubio, A. (2009). The role of identity salience in the effects of corporate

social responsibility on consumer behavior. Journal of Business Ethics, 84, 65–78.

Mohr, L. A., Eroǧlu, D., & Ellen, P. S. (1998). The development and testing of a measure of skepticism toward environmental claims in marketers' communications. Journal of consumer

affairs, 32(1), 30-55.

Mohr, L. A., Webb, D. J. and Harris, K. E. (2001). Do Consumers Expect Companies to be Socially

Responsible? The Impact of Corporate Social Responsibility on Buying Behavior. The Journal of

Consumer Affairs, Summer, 45-72.

Nan, X., & Heo, K. (2007). Consumer response to corporate social responsibility (CSR) initiatives.

Journal of Advertising, 36, 63–74.

Nguyen, B., Melewar, T. C., & Chen, J. (2013). A framework of brand likeability: an exploratory

study of likeability in firm-level brands. Journal of Strategic Marketing, 21(4), 368-390.

Öberseder, M., Schlegelmilch, B., & Murphy, P. (2013). CSR practices and consumer perceptions.

Journal of Business Research, 66(10), 1839–1851.

Osterhus, T. L. (1997). Pro-social consumer influence strategies: when and how do they work?.

Journal of marketing, 61(4), 16-29.

Oxford Learner’s Dictionaries (2020) Sceptic noun - Definition, pictures, pronunciation and usage notes, retrieved 31 January 2020, from

https://www.oxfordlearnersdictionaries.com/definition/english/sceptic?q=sceptic

(35)

34

Pérez, R. C. (2009). Effects of perceived identity based on corporate social responsibility: The role

of consumer identification with the company. Corporate Reputation Review, 12, 177–191.

Poels, K., & Dewitte, S. (2006). How to capture the heart? Reviewing 20 years of emotion

measurement in advertising. Journal of Advertising Research, 46(1), 18-37.

Pornpitakpan, C. (2004) The persuasiveness of source credibility: a critical review of five decades’ evidence. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 34(2), pp. 243–281.

Qader, I.K.A., and Y.B. Zainuddin (2011), “The Impact of Media Exposure on Intention to Purchase Green Electronic Products Amongst Lecturers,” International Journal of Business and Management, 6 (3), 240–248.

Sen, S., & Bhattacharya, C. B. (2001). Does doing good always lead to doing better? Consumer

reactions to corporate social responsibility. Journal of marketing Research, 38(2), 225-243.

Shaw, D., & Shiu, E. (2003). Ethics in consumer choice: a multivariate modelling approach.

European Journal of Marketing, 37(10), 1485–1498.

Simonin, B., & Ruth, J. (1998). Is a Company Known by the Company It Keeps? Assessing the

Spillover Effects of Brand Alliances on Consumer Brand Attitudes. Journal of Marketing Research,

35(1), 30–42.

Skard, S., & Thorbjørnsen, H. (2014). Is publicity always better than advertising? The role of brand

reputation in communicating corporate social responsibility. Journal of Business Ethics, 124(1),

149-160.

Smith, R. E., MacKenzie, S. B., Yang, X., Buchholz, L. M., & Darley, W. K. (2007). Modeling the

(36)

35

Strahilevitz, M. (2003). The effects of prior impressions of a firm's ethics on the success of a

cause-related marketing campaign: Do the good look better while the bad look worse?. Journal of

Nonprofit & Public Sector Marketing, 11(1), 77-92.

Swaen, V., & Vanhamme, J. (2005). The use of corporate social responsibility arguments in

communication campaigns: does source credibility matter?. ACR North American Advances.

Upadhye, B., Das, G., & Varshneya, G. (2019). Corporate social responsibility: a boon or bane for

innovative firms? Journal of Strategic Marketing, 27(1), 50–66.

Visser, W. (2011). The age of responsibility: CSR 2.0 and the new DNA of business. John Wiley &

Sons.

Wæraas, A., & Byrkjeflot, H. (2012). Public sector organizations and reputation management: Five

problems. International Public Management Journal, 15(2), 186-206.

Wan, C., Shen, G. Q., & Choi, S. (2017). Experiential and instrumental attitudes: Interaction effect

of attitude and subjective norm on recycling intention. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 50,

69-79.

Wilson, D. C., Rodic, L., Scheinberg, A., Velis, C. A., & Alabaster, G. (2012). Comparative

analysis of solid waste management in 20 cities. Waste Management & Research, 30(3), 237-254.

Wilson, E. J., & Sherrell, D. L. (1993). Source effects in communication and persuasion research: A

meta-analysis of effect size. Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, 21(2), 101.

Whittler, T.E. & Dimeo, J. (1991) Viewer’s reaction to racial cues in advertising

stimuli. Journal of Advertising Research, 31(6), pp. 37–46.

Wooden, R. (2008). The advertising discipline and social change. Journal of Advertising Research,

(37)

36

Yilmaz, C., Eser Telci, E., Bodur, M., & Eker Iscioglu, T. (2011). Source characteristics and

advertising effectiveness: The roles of message processing motivation and product category

knowledge. International Journal of Advertising, 30(5), 889-914.

Yoon, Y., Gürhan‐Canli, Z., & Schwarz, N. (2006). The effect of corporate social responsibility (CSR) activities on companies with bad reputations. Journal of consumer psychology, 16(4),

377-390.

Zhang, Y. & Buda, R. (1999) Moderating effects of need for cognition on responses to positively

versus negatively framed advertising messages. Journal of Advertising,

28(2), pp. 1–15.

Appendix A

Stimulus 1:

Brand source

Stimulus 2:

(38)

37

(39)

38

Appendix B

Pre-test questionnaire: Brands

Please rate the following brands on each of the three distinctions:

AXA

Very eco-unfriendly [] [] [] [] [] Very eco-friendly Very unattractive [] [] [] [] [] Very attractive

Very unfamiliar [] [] [] [] [] Very familiar

HP

Very eco-unfriendly [] [] [] [] [] Very eco-friendly Very unattractive [] [] [] [] [] Very attractive

Very unfamiliar [] [] [] [] [] Very familiar

PHILIPS

Very eco-unfriendly [] [] [] [] [] Very eco-friendly Very unattractive [] [] [] [] [] Very attractive

Very unfamiliar [] [] [] [] [] Very familiar

STARBUCKS

Very eco-unfriendly [] [] [] [] [] Very eco-friendly Very unattractive [] [] [] [] [] Very attractive

Very unfamiliar [] [] [] [] [] Very familiar

VISA

Very eco-unfriendly [] [] [] [] [] Very eco-friendly Very unattractive [] [] [] [] [] Very attractive

Referenties

GERELATEERDE DOCUMENTEN

Para ver de qué manera ‘Los niños de la furia’ se contrapone al discurso de seguridad de Felipe Calderón, se emplea un acercamiento discursivo de los estereotipos:

My main goal of this research was to provide the building blocks for a tool, in order to analyze the justification given in ethical discussions and reconstruct the role of

In the concern of friction reduction, the pillar (Z003) texture has the advantage over Hilbert curve and grooved channel textures in decreasing the friction force under the

factors used to construct the rheological and scratch healing master curves follow the same temperature dependence. The viscoelastic contributions of the hard and soft blocks as well

Whether it’s the ascetic movements of fifteenth-century Flanders, whether it’s all the types of religious communities that developed in Germany, at the same time or right after

medical first responder who has received a call that a bomb has gone off on a subway platform. The player is  then  told  to  find  the  subway  platform 

The purpose of this study was to investigate the moderating effect of industry regulations on the relationship between corporate social performance (CSP) and corporate

In order to answer the research question ‘What is the effect of visual persuasion on image-centric social media on consumer attitude towards the brand when mediated by