• No results found

Early neurological delopment, growth and nutrition in very preterm infants - Chapter 1: General introduction

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Share "Early neurological delopment, growth and nutrition in very preterm infants - Chapter 1: General introduction"

Copied!
25
0
0

Bezig met laden.... (Bekijk nu de volledige tekst)

Hele tekst

(1)

UvA-DARE is a service provided by the library of the University of Amsterdam (https://dare.uva.nl)

Early neurological delopment, growth and nutrition in very preterm infants

Maas, Y.G.H.

Publication date

1999

Link to publication

Citation for published version (APA):

Maas, Y. G. H. (1999). Early neurological delopment, growth and nutrition in very preterm

infants.

General rights

It is not permitted to download or to forward/distribute the text or part of it without the consent of the author(s) and/or copyright holder(s), other than for strictly personal, individual use, unless the work is under an open content license (like Creative Commons).

Disclaimer/Complaints regulations

If you believe that digital publication of certain material infringes any of your rights or (privacy) interests, please let the Library know, stating your reasons. In case of a legitimate complaint, the Library will make the material inaccessible and/or remove it from the website. Please Ask the Library: https://uba.uva.nl/en/contact, or a letter to: Library of the University of Amsterdam, Secretariat, Singel 425, 1012 WP Amsterdam, The Netherlands. You will be contacted as soon as possible.

(2)

General introduction

1.1 Nutrition 1.1.1 Human milk 1.1.2 Formula feeding 1.1.3 Nutrition optimality 1.2 Growth 1.3 Neurological development 1.3.1 Behavioural states 1.3.2 Spontaneous motility 1.4 Neurological outcome

1.5 Scope and general outline of the thesis 1.5.1 Aims

1.5.2 General outline

(3)
(4)

of inadequate prenatal and postnatal nutrition on the long term neurological outcome of infants (47,143,144). Extensive research on infant nutrition has been done in

underdeveloped countries. All of these studies had problems determining the relative impact and/or the interaction of social, psychological, physical and environmental factors on the outcome (142). Usually, no instrument was available to detect neurological damage in these young infants irrespective of these factors. A recent (1998) doctoral thesis by A.F. Hassan

'Visual and Brain Impairment in Childhood Malnutrition' from our department used visual evoked potential (VEP) measurements in a study of protein energy malnutrition in infants from Egypt (72). In this study VEP was useful as a measure for neural deficiency and impared visual maturation. Improvements in the Reversal (RvN70) latencies were seen following treatment. The importance of nutrition for immature and critically ill infants and the impact on clinical outcome and neurological development has become a concern for neonatologists since there has been a dramatic improvement in survival with availability of modern intensive care in the 1970s and 80s. Circumstantially, nutrition for infants prematurely born is not comparable to the term infant who has received placental nutritional support to term. Preterm infants at term corrected age are usually smaller than fullterm infants and as a group preterm infants have delayed development. When an infant is born preterm many of the maturational processes are delayed and others essential for survival e.g., lung development are advanced post delivery. Maturation of the nervous system takes time and continues even in fullterm infants postnatally. Therefore, preterm birth is a severe event to the developing fetus. Intensive care helps these infants survive, but more research is needed to improve longterm outcome. The development of diagnostic tools to identify (early i.e., at term conceptional age) infants at risk for long-term

neurological abnormalities would be an important clinical advance which may allow prevention and/or interventions to improve outcome.

Although progress has been made in attaining more knowledge about and insight into nutrition, metabolism and growth of the fetus and preterm infant, more research is needed to determine what is optimal for infants born before term.

Maturation of the nervous system may be less affected by extrinsic factors compared to adverse effects of inadequate nutrition (113). The "brain sparing" concept hypothesizes that

(5)

when nutrients are moderately insufficient, the brain is less affected compared to other systems . The development of a tool(s) to access the neurological "growth curve" and potential for later function would provide new clinical information not evident in the standard physical growth curve. This new tool would allow the evaluation of nutritional or other intervention strategies on the long term neurologic outcome of the prematurely born infant.

1.1 Nutrition

In ancient Greece Hippocrates frequently advised his patients about what foods they should eat. This has become a common practice by physicians all over the world who have used dietetics as part of their treatment. However, it was not recognized as an independent science until the twentieth century. In the Netherlands the science of Human Nutrition was introduced in 1969 at the Agricultural University of Wageningen. Usually, it was not part of the medical curriculum. "Clinical nutrition" in the 1980s has become an important and integrated part in the management of patients. The subject of malnutrition in

underdeveloped countries is covered under general and clinical nutrition science. Nutrition for preterm infants can be categorized under clinical nutrition to address the unique needs of critically ill preterm infants who require intensive care and under general nutrition to address their requirments for later growth and development. What is "appropriate" nutrition for the prematurely born infant may be very different than for the infant born at term. Nutritional issues may have had a lower clinical priority when survival was the main issue. However, now that techniques to support the critically ill preterm infant are markedly improved further studies on acute and long term nutrition are needed. Any feeding regimen for the preterm infant must take into consideration the limitation in function of the immature gastrointestinal tract. More information about the exact nutrient supply for the human fetal maternal placental unit would give valuable insight into the nutritional needs of the fetus. Imitating the in utero nutrition would seem ideal. However, the preterm baby is confronted with completely different circumstances and demands. A fetus does not thermoregulate its body temperature. Thermoregulation for the preterm baby will require more energy expenditure and fuel. Providing optimal nutrition early in life will be important to improve longterm development (12,17,58,88,103).

(6)

1.1.1 Human milk

There is a concensus that breast milk is the optimal nutrition for infants. The nutritional needs of term infants was determined through studies of human milk samples. Analyses of human milk samples taken at different postnatal ages, at different times of day from the same mother support that the composition of human milk is dynamic. These data support that the nutrient composition of human milk is adapted for nutritional needs of the growing and developing human infant (69,86,131).

While there is general agreement among clinicians and other health workers and -organisations to advocate breastmilk for term infants this is less concensus for preterm infants. (11,14,31,50,51,93,111,127). Patricia B. Lawrence concludes in a review article on breastmilk published in 1994 that the growing positive attitude towards human milk of an infant's own mother, despite the fact that it is not considered to be ideal, "stems from a recognition that formula, although especially formulated for the preterm infant, is also less than ideal and may not be the optimal substitute for breast milk enriched with proteins and minerals". She also states that the breast milk of prematurely delivering mothers needs further study (93).

Some of the challenges in studying human lactation were noted by F.E. Hytten (77-81). The first and greatest difficulty is the collection of milk. Estimation of the quantity and composition of the milk received by an infant nursing at mother's breast is difficult. Milk expressed by the mother may not be the same as with her infant nursing. When a child is fed on demand the volume received may influence later milk production. Participation in human milk studies involves a commitment from the mother and inconvenience. Dedicated participants are imperative to get reliable results. This selection bias may affect the results. Other variables are: method of milk expression (137); changing composition during the course of the collection (48); diurnal variations in both volume and composition (52,92); changing composition with duration of lactation (2); varying composition between different individuals (2,6); effect of maternal diet on the composition (9,24,69).

These challenges surrounding human milk research are magnified for preterm milk. The mother may be ill at the time of the preterm birth and she may have received medications which may influence her milk production. The stress of the premature labor and birth affect her milk production and possibly her willingness to participate or to comply with the

(7)

protocol. These problems can be compounded when her infant is critically ill and the outcome is uncertian. Mothers of preterm infants may not produce sufficient volumes of milk and in these cases supplementation may be required (73,97). Moreover, the nutritional content of breast milk from mothers who deliver prematurely has greater variability compared to term milk. The breast milk composition also seems to depend on duration of gestation (95).

In the 1970s and 1980s extensive research was done which compared the composition of fullterm and preterm milk. The most striking difference was a higher protein content in preterm milk (7,9,10,26,35,36,57-59,86,89,90,92,101,123). In the 1990s further research work was done on fatty acid composition. Preterm milk has higher levels of easily absorbed medium-chain fatty acids and long-chain polyunsaturated fatty acids compared with term milk (17a,41,54,98).

These data have been interpreted to mean that preterm infants require more protein and higher levels of medium- and long-chain fatty acids. It is often concluded that preterm milk is especially adapted for the extro-uterine nutritional needs of prematurely born infants. Another interesting finding is that breast milk composition changes postnatally for term and preterm milk (7-10,26,57,77,78). Inspite of all research efforts to date the basis for these differences in nutrient composition are still unclear.

The duration of gestation is an obvious factor which could influence the composition of breastmilk (5,16,95). One hypothesis is that the postconceptional age changes in the breast milk of prematurly delivering mothers is an adaptation to the extro utero nutritional needs of the premature infant (49). To answer the question of specific suitability of preterm milk for preterm infants it is important to find out whether the differences found between milk produced by mothers of preterm and term infants are related to the different nutritional needs of these two groups of neonates.

1.1.2 Formula Feeding

Cow's milk was used as food for infants since the end of World War II. Its popularity decreased by the 1970s because of a resurgence of breastfeeding. With the evolution of modern neonatal intensive care at about the same time companies producing infant formula milk began focusing on the development and production of formulas for preterm infants.

(8)

Prior to the availablity of these special preterm formulas preterm infants were given banked human milk from mothers of term babies who were willing and able to donate breastmilk. Since the growth of these small infants on the banked milk was considered suboptimal by neonatologists, milk substitutes were sought. Although continuing research on the composition of term human milk provided relevant information for continuing adjustment of the formulas for term infants, these data did not advance the knowledge needed for the development of a preterm infant formula.

The first successful human milk substitutes were primarily based on cow's milk. The aim was to develop formulas that mimmiced the composition of term and preterm human milk. At this time the goal has changed to achieve the physiological growth effects seen in infants fed human milk (55,123). Current research has focused on the substances in human milk which facilitate the growth of the infant.

1.1.3 Nutrition optimality

Although the investigation of breast milk of the mother delivering before term is more challenging it can be argued that there is greater need for such information. What is the optimal nutrition for the prematurely born infant? In general, optimality of nutrition in infants is judged by following growth parameters (anthropometry). In the clinical practice adequacy of nutrition for preterm infants is asccessed from following the postnatal growth curves and by comparison with the in utero growth curves developed from fetal

measurements. Recommendations for daily intake are based on "expected growth" from the fetal growth curves (3). However, the question remains whether physical growth is the best criteriom to judge the quality of nutrition for preterm infants.

1.2 Growth

The intra-uterine growth curves are based on cross-sectional data from babies born at different gestational ages ranging from + 24 weeks G A to term age (85,87,96,136). For following the growth of a fetus in utero it is a reasonable reference curve for growth. For infants born premature such reference curves may be less appropriate since they are extra uterine and experiencing very different adaptations due to their premature birth. Is this growth trajectory optimal for the preterm infant?

(9)

Postnatal growth of premature babies is much more influenced by extrinsic factors such as the need for intensive care support and nutrition compared to term babies. This complicates generation of standards for "extra-uterine" preterm growth form longitudinal

anthropometric measurements of preterm infants of various gestational ages (37,43). A "healthy" preterm infant population measured under standard conditions would be required to establish such curves. These growth curves would be different from the postnatal growth curves of term infants at adjusted ages and may be different from the intra-uterine fetal growth curves (27).

1.3 Neurological development

1.3.1 Behavioural states

By the late 1950s the concept of behavioural states gradually emerged in infant research (115). Many review articles report on the research and significance of infant sleep (4,39,94,100,106,107,115,132). Through the development of polygraphic recording techniques it was possible to further the investigation of sleep research. Parmelee in 1970 (106) states that there was sufficient knowledge to allow the development of a predictive neurological assessment method for newborns. He emphasized the importance "of the study of sleep behavior in the newborn infant as a means of neurological assessment". The focus of infant sleep research was to use sleep characteristics as a measure of brain organization and development. By using these maturational changes a neurodiagnostic tool to assess the integrity of the central nervous system in newborn infants could be developed

(4,39,94,100,106,119,132).

Infant sleep has two distinctly different "behavioural" patterns. One which is characterized by eye movements, frequent small body movements and irregular respiration, rapid eye movement (REM), and paradoxical or active sleep. In the other state, called quiet sleep or non-rapid eye movements (NREM) sleep there are no eye movements, no body movements, except for occasional jerky body movements, and respiration is regular. Similar to adult sleep, infants show cyclic fluctuations between quiet and active sleep. The duration of sleep cycle was not found to be significantly increased with maturation from 35 weeks

(10)

but the most significant maturational change seen was a dramatic increase in the amount of quiet sleep relative to active sleep. This shift in proportion of quiet to active sleep with age was hypothesised to be an indicator of maturation of the nervous system

(30a, 105,119,132). While active sleep represents an anarchic primitive activity of the CNS, quiet sleep is a highly controlled state requiring complex feedback mechanisms. Sleep was less organized in preterm infants who had more undefinable state i.e., indeterminate sleep because of their more immature nervous systems. (39,107,117). Though Nijhuis in 1982 (102) states that organization of behavioural states, identical in fetus and premature infant of comparable age, is not developed before 36 weeks GA, in later years several

investigators found that state differentiation does exists long before term age intra- as well as extra-uterine (31,34,137). Therefore, changes in the development and amount of each behavioural state may be helpful in the assessment of neurological maturation, brain injury and to predict outcome in preterm infants (4,39,121,127).

Infant sleep research in infants has been hindered by a wide variety of criteria and state definitions between investigators (4,32,68,74,115,127,131). Harper et al. emphasizes that knowledge of normal development of sleep states is important for understanding early neurological maturation (68). Hoppenbrouwers in 1992 (74) gave the opinion that the use of sleep development was limited clinically due to differences in state definitions, data collection and analysis strategies. Curzi-Dascalova and Mirmiran recently reported a standardization of the method in their Manual of Methods for Recording and Analyzing Sleep-Wakefulness States in Preterm and Full-term Infants (33). However, the use of state analysis has been used primarily as a research method (81,99,122,130,138).

1.3.2 Spontaneous motility

The term spontaneous behaviour includes behavioural states and overt spontaneous motility occuring without manipulation in fetuses, preterm and fullterm newborns (38,112,117) In contrast to diminished interest in the last decade in behavioural states assessment as a method to evaluate brain function in newborns, assessment of general movements has been extensively explored for this application (1,18,28,47,52,65,82,83,88,139). In 1993 Julia J. Geerdink wrote her thesis on early motor development (51) and in 1997 Arie F. Bos noted

(11)

the scientific interest for the study of spontaneous activity in newborn infants in his thesis (19).

With the substantial improvements in perinatal care and increasing numbers of surviving preterm infants of early gestations, there is greater need for a reliable assessment technique to evaluate early neurological development and to predict outcome. Several studies in preterm infants concluded that qualitative and not quantative aspects of GMs have discriminative value for assessment of neurological development and outcome (20,21,47,120). Prechtl and co-workers have developed a method for the qualitative assessment of general movements as a diagnostic tool for early detection of brain dysfunction (47,64,113). By now general movements in preterms have been studied on a large scale, but never in large groups. Small groups of low-risk infants were studied and compared with small groups of infants potentially at risk for neurological dysfunction (22,23,40,42,43,47,53,118). By the nature of the assessment method GMs cannot easily be studied longitudinally at group level unless the Ferrari Optimality score is used (47). This semi-quantitative evaluation of GMs judges 8 distinguishable elements of movement. As a standard the method is now based on designing individual developmental trajectories using the global judgement of GM quality, i.e. normal or abnormal (47). Moreover, deviant movement quality can be scored using more descriptive categories (20,47,116). Most of the studies on spontaneous motilty in infants were done by or in collaboration with Prechtl and co-workers. Recently, another investigator made changes in the methods of assessment of GM quality which has lead to some disagreements about how the assessments should be used. Prechtl and his group have maintained the assessment technique as described in 1990 and reviewed in 1997 (44). Mijna Hadders-Algra endeavoured to improve this technique on the bases of a neurophysiologically accountable classification system (63,64). She introduced the category of mildly abnormal GMs in the global assessment and consequently developed different descriptive categories of deviant movement quality. Comparison of both scoring systems by independent researchers on the basis of long term data on neurological outcome should clarifiy whether the modification in methodology improves

(12)

1.4 Neurological outcome

Advances in technology and accessability of neonatal and perinatal care over the past decades have improved survival of preterm infants. Recently, it was reported in the United States that the incidence of prematurity is increasing due to delayed childbearing and the use of fertility drugs which has increased the incidence of multiple births (60). Preterm birth is accompanied by an increased risk of longterm (neurological) damage. This risk is inversely related to gestational age at birth. Though much progress has been made in treating (very) preterm infants the neurodevelopmental outcome for some of these infants is a concern (13,61,62,103,108,124,133). A clinical instrument to identify infants early (at term age) for later neurodevelopmental problems would be an important advance in neonatology. Such early identification may improve the chances of success of intervention and prevention.

1.5 Aims and general outline of the thesis

1.5.1 Aims

The aim of this thesis is twofold:

1. develop a clinical instrument to monitor neurological development and prognosticate outcome of very low birth weight infants ( < 30 weeks gestation)

2. investigate the applicability of this instrument to judge optimality of nutrition

1.5.2 General outline

In chapter 2 the infant population, protocol procedures and methods are described. The limitations for the present investigation as part of a parent study which examined thyroxine supplementation are briefly mentioned. Chapter 3 describes the macronutrient composition of very preterm human milk and analyzes whether there is a (functional) relation between the macronutrient composition of very preterm human milk and gestational age, postnatal or developmental (i.e. postmenstrual) age of the preterm infant. Chapter 4 describes relative growth of very preterm infants until term age using anthropometric measurements of 10 different parameters. The effects of different feeding regimes on this relative preterm growth within the randomized, placebo controlled (double-blind) trial on thyroxine

(13)

supplementation are reported. In chapter 5 development of behavioural states in very preterm infants was studied to determine whether longitudinal assessment of behavioural states is a good method for early detection of neurological deviations in very preterm infants. And chapter 6 documents the development of quality of spontaneous motility in very preterm infants in order to determine whether longitudinal assessment of quality of general movements provides a good method for early detection of neurological deviations in very preterm infants. Chapter 7 evaluates the predictive value of preterm cranial

ultrasound, the Prechtl neurological test at term and our newly introduced methods of studying the development of behavioural states and the quality of spontaneous motility at term age in preterm infants based on their neurodevelopmental outcomes at two years corrected age. The 5 sub-study sections are followed by a general discussion in chapter 8. This discussion addresses the question whether this research has developed an instrument to monitor neurological development in very preterm infants and predict the risk of

neurodevelopmental abnormalities. A comparison of the effects found of different feeding regimes on growth (physical development) and neurological development is made, and the applicability of the studied methods for investigation of optimality of nutrition in very young infants is discussed. The limitations of the study as described in this thesis are discussed. Directions for additional/ futher research are addressed. Chapters 9 and 10, finally, summarize the main results and conclusions of this thesis in English and Dutch respectively

1.6 References

1. Albers S, Jorch G. Prognostic significance of spontaneous motility in very immature preterm infants under intensive care treatment. Biol Neonate 1994;66:182-187. 2. Allen JC, Keller RP, Archer P, Neville MC. Studies in human lactation: milk

composition and daily secretion rates of macronutrients in the first year of lactation. Am J Clin Nutr 1991;54:69-80.

3. American Academy of Pediatrics, Committee on Nutrition. Nutritional needs of low-birth-weight infants. Pediatr 1985;75:976-985.

4. Anders TF. The infant sleep profile. Neuropädiatrie 1974;5:425-442.

(14)

(letter). Am J Clin Nutr 1984;39:168-170.

6. Anderson GH. The effect of prematurity on milk composition and its physiological basis. Fed Proc 1984;43:2438-2442.

7. Anderson GH, Atkinson SA, Bryan MH. Energy and macronutrient content of human milk during early lactation from mothers giving birth prematurely and at term. Am J Clin Nutr 1981;34:258-265.

8. Anderson DM, Williams FH, Merkatz RB, Schulman PK, Kerr DS, Pittard WB. Length of gestation and nutritional composition of human milk. Am J Clin Nutr

1983;37:810-814.

9. Atkinson SA, Anderson GH, Bryan MH. Human milk: comparison of the nitrogen composition in milk from mothers of premature and full-term infants. Am J Clin Nutr 1980;33:811-815.

10. Atkinson SA, Bryan MH, Anderson GH. Human milk: difference in nitrogen concen-tration in milk from mothers of term and premature infants. J Pediatr 1978;93:67-69. 11. Atkinson SA, Radde IC, Chance GW, Bryan MH, Anderson GH. Macro-mineral

content of milk obtained during early lactation from mothers of premature infants. Early Hum Dev 1980;4/1:5-14.

12. Battaglia FC, Thureen PJ. Nutrition of the fetus and premature infant. Nutrition 1997;13:903-906.

13. Battin M, Ling EW, Whitfield MF, Mackinnon M, Effer SB. Has the outcome for extremely low gestational age (ELGA) infants improved following recent advances in neonatal care? Am J Perinatol 1998;15:469-477.

14. Baum, JD. Preterm milk (editorial). Early Hum Dev 1980;4/1:1-3.

15. Beijers RJW, Graaf FVD, Schaafsma A, Siemensma AD. Composition of premature breast-milk during lactation: constant digestible protein content (as in full term milk). Early Hum Dev 1992;29:351-356.

16. Beijers RJ, Schaafsma A. Long-chain polyunsaturated fatty acid content in Dutch preterm breast milk; differences in the concentrations of docosahexaenoic acid and arachidonic acid due to length of gestation. Early Hum Dev 1996;44:215-223. 17. Bishop NJ. Feeding the preterm. Pediatr Nephrol 1994;8:494-498.

(15)

composition of breast milk from mothers of term and preterm infants. Am J Clin Nutr 1983;38:300-312.

19. Bos AF. Differential effects of brain lesions and systemic disease on the quality of general movements: a preliminary report. Early Human Development 1993;34:39-45. 20. Bos AF. Analysis of movement quality in preterm infants. Thesis, University of

Groningen, the Netherlands, 1997.

21. Bos AF, Van Loon AJ, Hadders-Algra M, Martijn A, Okken A, Prechtl HFR. Spontaneous motility in preterm, small-for-gestational age infants. II Qualitative aspects. Early Hum Dev 1997;50:131-147.

22. Bos AF, Van Loon AJ, Martijn A, Van Asperen RM, Okken A, Prechtl HFR. Spontaneous motility in preterm, small-for-gestational age infants. II Quantitative aspects. Early Hum Dev 1997;50:115-129.

23. Bos AF, Martijn A, Van Asperen RM, Hadders-Algra M, Okken A, Prechtl HFR. Qualitative assessment of general movements in high-risk preterm infants with chronic lung disease requiring dexamethasone therapy. J Pediatr 1998;132:300-306.

24. Bos AF, Martijn A, Okken A, Prechtl HFR. Quality of general movements in preterm infants with periventricular echodensities. Acta Paediatr 1998;87:328-335.

25. Brasil ALD, Vitolo MR, Lopez FA, De Nóbrega FJ. Fat and protein composition of mature milk in adolescents. J Adolescent Health 1991;12:365-371.

26. Britton JR. Milk protein quality in mothers delivering prematurely: implications for infants in the intensive care unit nursery setting. J Pediatr Gastroenterol Nutr 1986;5:116-121.

27. Butte NF, Garza C, Johnson CA, Smith EO, Nichols BL. Longitudinal changes in milk composition of mothers delivering preterm and term infants. Early Hum Dev 1984;9:153-162.

28. Casey PH, Kraemer HC, Bernbaum J, Yogman MW, Clifford Sells J. Growth status and growth rates of a varied sample of low birth weight, preterm infants: a

longitudinal cohort from birth to three years of age. J Pediatr 1991;119:599-605. 29. Cioni G, Prechtl HFR. Preterm and early postterm motor behaviour in low-risk

premature infants. Early Hum Dev 1990;23:159-191.

(16)

carbohydrate composition in human milk over 4 months of lactation. Pediatr 1993;91:637-641.

31. Cunningham AS, Jelliffe DB, Jelliffe EFP. Breast-feeding and health in the 1980s: A global epidemiological review. J Pediatr 1991;118:659-666.

32. Curzi-Dascalova L. Developpment du sommeil et des functions sous controle du système nerveux autonomie chez les nouveau-nes prematures et a terme. Arch Pediatr 1995;2:255-262.

33. Curzi-Dascalova L, Figueroa JM, Eiselt M, et al. Sleep state organization in

premature infants of less than 35 weeks gestational age. Pediatr Res 1993;34:624-628. 34. Curzi-Dascalova L, Lebrun F, Korn G. Respiratory frequency according to sleep

states and age in normal premature infants: a comparison with full term infants. Pediatr Res 1983;17:152-156.

35. Curzi-Dascalova L, Mirmiran M, (eds). Manual of Methods for Recording and Analyzing Sleep-Wake fulness States in Preterm and Full-term Infants. INSERM, Paris, 1996.

36. Curzi-Dascalova L, Peirano P, Morel-Kahn F. Development of sleep states in normal premature and full-term newborns. Dev Psychobiol 1988;21:431-444.

37. Darwish AEH, Dakroury AM, El-Feel MS, Nour NM. Comparative study on breast milk of mothers delivering preterm and term infants. Protein, fat and lactose. Nahrung 1989;33:249-251.

38. Dawodu AH, Osibanjo O, Damole 10. Nutrient composition of milk produced by mothers of preterm infants in Nigeria. East Afr Med J 1990;67:873-877.

39. Dewey KG, Heinig MJ, Nommsen LA, Peerson JM, Lönnerdal B. Growth of breast-fed and formula-breast-fed infants from 0 to 18 months: the Darling study. Pediatr

1992;89:1035-1041.

40. Dreyfus-Brisac C. Ontogenesis of sleep in human prematures after 32 weeks of conceptional age. Dev Psychobiol 1970;3:91-121.

41. Dreyfus-Brisac C, Monod N. Sleeping behaviour in abnormal newborn infants. Neuropädiatrie 1970;1:354-366.

42. Droit S, Boldrini A, Cioni G. Rhythmical leg movements in low-risk and brain-damaged preterm infants. Early Hum Dev 1996;44:201-213.

(17)

43. Ehrenkranz RA, Ackerman BA, Nelli CM. Total lipid content and fatty acid composition of preterm human milk. J Pediatr Gastroenterol Nutr 1984;3:755-758 44. Einspieler C. Abnormal spontaneous movements in infants with repeated sleep

apnoeas. Early Hum Dev 1994;36:31-48.

45. Einspieler C, Prechtl HFR, van Eykern L, de Roos B. Observation of movements during sleep in ALTE and apnoeic infants. Early Hum Dev 1994;40:39-50.

46. Einspieler C, Prechtl HFR, Ferrari F, Cioni G, Bos AF. The qualitative assessment of general movements in preterm, term and young infants - review of the methodology. Early Hum Dev 1997;50:47-60.

47. El-Tatawy S, Badrawi N, El Bishlawy A. Cerebral atrophy in infants with protein energy malnutrition. ANJR 1983;4:434-436.

48. Emery III WB, Canolty NL, Aitchison JM, Dunkley WL. Influence of sampling on fatty acid composition of human milk. Am J Clin Nutr 1978;31:1127-1130. 49. Ferrari F, Cioni G, Prechtl HFR. Qualitative changes of general movements in

preterm infants with brain lesions. Early Hum Dev 1990;23:193-231.

50. Fomon SJ, Ziegler EE, Vazquez HD. Human milk and the small premature infant. Am J Dis Child 1977;131;463-467.

51. Fomon JF, Ziegler EE. Milk of the premature infant's mother: Interpretation of data. J Pediatr 1978;93:164.

52. Garza C, Butte NF. Energy concentration of human milk estimated from 24-h pools and various abbreviated sampling schemes. J Pediatr Gastroenterol Nutr 1986;5:943-948.

53. Geerdink JJ. Early motor development in preterm infants. Thesis, Free University of Amsterdam, the Netherlands, 1993.

54. Geerdink JJ, Hopkins B. Qualitative changes in general movements and their prognostic values in preterm infants. Eur J Paediatr 1993;152:362-367. 55. Geerdink JJ, Hopkins B. Effects of birthweight status and gestational age on the

quality of general movements in preterm newborns. Biol Neonate 1993;63:215-224. 56. Gibson RA, Makrides M. The role of long chain polyunsaturated fatty acids

(LCPUFA) in neonatal nutrition. Acta Paediatr 1998;10:1017-1022.

(18)

of infant formulas: recent findings and possible applications. Nutr Res Rev 1994;7:1-23.

58. Gordon N. Nutrition and cognitive function. Brain Dev 1997;19:165-170. 59. Gross SJ, David RJ, Bauman L, Tomarelli RM. Nutritional composition of milk

produced by mothers delivering preterm. J Pediatr 1980;96:641-644.

60. Gross SJ, Geller J, Tomarelli RM. Composition of breast milk from mothers of preterm infants. Pediatr 1981;68:490-493.

61. Guerrini P, Bosi G, Chierici R, Fabbri A. Human milk: relationship of fat content with gestational age. Early Hum Dev 1981;5:187-194.

62. Guyer B, MacDorman MF, Martin JA, Peters KD and Strobius DM. Annual Summary of Vital Statics-1997. Pediatr 1998;102:1333-1349.

63. Hack M, Fanaroff AA. Outcomes of children of extremely low birthweight and gestational age in the 1990's. Early Humn Dev 1999;53:193-218.

64. Hack, M, Friedman, H, Avroy, A, Fanaroff, MB. Outcomes of extremely low birth weight infants. Pediatr 1996;98:931-937.

65. Hadders-Algra, M. The assessment of general movements is a valuable technique for the detection of brain dysfunction in young infants. A review. Acta Paediatr Suppl 1996;416:39-43.

66. Hadders-Algra, M, Klip-Van den Nieuwendijk AWJ, Martijn A, Van Eykern LA. Assessment of general movements: towards a better understanding of a sensitive method to evaluate brain function in young infants. Dev Med Child Neurol

1997;39:89-99.

67. Hadders-Algra M, Prechtl HFR. Developmental course of general movements in early infancy. I. Descriptive analysis of change in form. Early Hum Dev 1992;28:201-213. 68. Hall B. Uniformity of human milk. Am J Clin Nutr 1979;32:304-312.

69. Hambrsus L, Lönnerdal B, Forsum E, Gebre-Medhin M. Nitrogen and protein components of human milk. Acta Paediatr Scand 1978;67:561-565.

70. Harper RM, Leake B, Miyahara L, Mason J, Hoppenbrouwers T, Sterman MB, Hodgman J. Temporal sequencing in sleep and waking states during the first 6 months of life. Experimental neurol 1981;72:294-307.

(19)

Ann Nutr Metab 1984;28:231-239.

72. Hassan A-YF. Visual and brain impairment in childhood malnutrtion. Thesis, University of Amsterdam, the Netherlands, 1998.

73. Hibberd CM, Brooke OG, Carter ND, Haug M, Harzer G. Variation in the composi-tion of breast milk during the first 5 weeks of lactacomposi-tion: implicacomposi-tions for the feeding of preterm infants. Arch Dis Child 1982;57:658-662.

74. Hibberd CM, Brooke OG, Carter ND, Wood C. A comparison of protein

concentrations and energy in breast milk from preterm and term mothers. Journal of Human Nutrition 1981;35:189-198.

75. Hopkinson JM, Schanler RJ, Garza C. Milk production by mothers of premature infants. Pediatr 1988;81:815-820.

76. Hoppenbrouwers T. Polysomnography in newborns and young infants: sleep architecture. J Clin Neurophysiol 1992;9:32-47.

77. Hytten FE. Clinical and chemical studies in human lactation. I. Collection of milk samples. Br Med J 1954;1:175-176.

78. Hytten FE. Clinical and chemical studies in human lactation. II. Variation in major constituents during a feeding. Br Med J 1954;1:176-179.

79. Hytten FE. Clinical and chemical studies in human lactation. III. Diurnal variation in major constituents of milk. Br Med J 1954;1:179-182.

80. Hytten FE. Clinical and chemical studies in human lactation. IV. Trends in milk composition during course of lactation. Br Med J 1954;1:249-253.

81. Hytten FE. Clinical and chemical studies in human lactation. V. Individual differences in composition of milk. Br Med J 1954;1:253-255.

82. Jain S, Bijlani RL. The significance of some significant features of breast milk. Ind J Physiol Pharmac 1989;33:118-128.

83. Jorch G, Huster T, Rabe H. Dependency of Doppler parameters in the anterior cerebral artery on behavioural states in preterm and term neonates. Biol Neonate 1990;58:79-86.

84. Kakebeeke TH, Siebenthal KV, Largo RH. Differences in movement quality at term among preterm and term infants. Biol Neonate 1997;71:367-378.

(20)

to term. Biol Neonate 1998;73:145-154.

86. Karra MV, Udipi SA, Kirksey A, Roepke JLB. Changes in specific nutrients in breast milk during extended lactation. Am J Clin Nutr 1986;43:495-503.

87. Keen DV, Pearse RG. Birthweight between 14 and 42 weeks' gestation. Arch Dis Child 1985;60:440-446.

88. De Klerk A, Schulze KF, Kashyap S, Sahni R, Fifer W, Myers M. Diet and infant behavior. Acta Paediatr Suppl 1997;422:65-68.

89. Kloosterman GJ. On intrauterine growth. Int J Gynaecol Obstet 1970;8:895-912. 90. Konishi Y, Prechtl HFR. Finger movements and finger postures in pre-term infants

are not a good indicator of brain damage. Early Hum Dev 1994;36:89-100. 91. Kumbhat MM, Khanna SA, Bijur AM, Jadhav CS. Breast milk composition in

relation to gestation. Ind Pediatr 1985;22:229-233.

92. Lammi-Keefe CJ, Ferris AM, Jensen RG. Changes in human milk at 0600, 1000, 1400, 1800, 2200 h. J Pediatr Gastroenterol Nutr 1990;11:83-88.

93. Lawrence PB. Breast milk. Best source of nutrition for term and preterm infants. Pediatr Clin North Am 1994;41:925-941.

94. Lemons JA, Moye L, Hall D, Simmons M. Differences in the composition of preterm and term human milk during early lactation. Pediatr Res 1982;16:113-117.

95. Lemons JA, Reyman D and Moye L. Amino acid composition of preterm and term breast milk during early lactation. Early Hum Dev 1983;8:323-329.

96. Lenard HG. Sleep studies in infancy (review). Acta Paediatr Scand 1970;59:572-581. 97. Lepage G, Collet S, Bougie D, et al. The composition of preterm milk in relation to

the degree of prematurity. Am J Clin Nutr 1984;40:1042-1049.

98. Lubchenco LO, Hansman C, Dressier M, Boyd E. Intrauterine growth as estimated from liveborn birth-weight data at 24 to 42 weeks of gestation. Pediatr 1963;32:793-800.

99. Lucas A. Availability of preterm milk. Lancet 1983;1:1045-1046.

100. Luukkainen P, Salo MK, Janas M, Nikkari T. Changes in fatty acid composition of preterm and term human milk from 1 to 6 months of lactation. J Pediatr Gastroenterol Nutr 1994;18:355-360.

(21)

behavioural states on visual evoked responses in preterm and full-term newborns. Neuropediatr 1995;26:211-213.

102. Michaelis R, Parmelee AH, Stern E, Haber A. Activity states in premature and tern infants. Dev Psychobiol 1973;6:209-215.

103. Morley R, Lucas A. Influence of early diet on outcome in preterm infants. Acta Paediatr Suppl 1994;405:123-126.

104. Nijhuis JG, Prechtl HFR, Martin CB, Bots RSGM. Are there behavioral states in the human fetus? Early Human Dev 1982;6:177-195.

105. Oishi M, Nishida H, Sasaki T. Japanese experience with micropremies weighing less than 600 grams born between 1984 and 1993. Pediatr 1997;99:E7.

106. Pamblanco M, Ten A, Comin J. Proteins in preterm and term milk from mothers delivering appropriate or small-for-gestational age infants. Early Hum Dev 1986;14:267-272.

107. Parmelee AH. Sleep cycles in infants. Dev Med Child Neurol 1969;11:794-795. 108. Parmelee AH. Sleep studies for the neurological assessment of the newborn.

Neuropädiatrie 1970;1:351-353.

109. Parmelee AH, Waldemar HW, Akiyama Y, Schultz M, Stern E. Sleep states in premature infants. Dev Med Child Neurol 1967;9:70-77.

110. Philip AG. Neonatal mortality rate: is further improvement possible? J Pediatr 1995;126:427-433.

111. Pierse P, Van Aerde J, Clandinin MT. Nutritional value of human milk. Prog Food NutrSci 1988;12:421-447.

112. Prechtl HFR. The behavioral states of the newborn infant (a review). Brain Res 1974;76:185-212.

113. Prechtl HFR (ed). Continuity of neural functions from prenatal to postnatal life. Clinics in Developmental Medicine no.94. SIMP/Blackwell. Oxford, 1984. 114. Prechtl HFR. Fetal behavior. In: Hill A, Volpe JJ, editors. Fetal neurology. Raven

Press. New York, USA, 1989:1-16.

115. Prechtl, HFR. Qualitative changes of spontaneous movements in fetus and preterm infant are a marker of neurological dysfunction (Editorial). Early Hum Dev 1990;23:151-158.

(22)

116. Prechtl HFR. The organization of behavioral states and their dysfunction. Seminars in Perinatology 1992;16:258-263.

117. Prechtl HFR, O'Brien MJ. Behavioural states of the full-term newborn, the emergence of a concept. In P. Stratton (ed.). Psychobiology of the human newborn. John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Chichester, 1982, p53-73.

118. Prechtl HFR, Einspieler C, Cioni G, Bos AF, Ferrari F, Sontheimer D. An early marker for neurological deficits after perinatal brain lesions. Lancet 1997;349:1361-1363.

119. Prechtl HFR, Fargel JW, Weinmann HM, Bakker HH. Postures, motility and respiration of low-risk preterm infants. Dev Med Child Neurol 1979;21:3-27. 120. Prechtl HFR, Ferrari F, Cioni G. Predictive value of general movements in

asphyxiated fullterm infants. Early Hum Dev 1993;35:91-120.

121. Prechtl HFR, Lenard HG. A study om eye movements in sleeping newborn infants. Brain Res 1967;5:477-493.

122. Prechtl HFR, Nolte R. Motor behaviour of preterm infants. In: Prechtl HFR, editor. Continuity of neural functions from prenatal to postnatal life, Clinics in

Developmental Medicine No 94. Blackwell Scientific Publications Ltd. Oxford, UK, 1984:79-92.

123. Prechtl HFR, Theorell K, Blair AW. Behavioural state cycles in abnormal infants. Dev Med Child Neurol 1973;15:606-615.

124. Raemakers VT, Casaer P, Daniels H, Smet M, Marchai G. The influence of behavioural states on cerebral blood flow velocity patterns in stable preterm infants. Early Hum Dev 1989;20:229-246.

125. Redel CA, Shulman RJ. Controversies in the composition of infant formulas. Ped Clin North Am 1994;41:909-923.

126. Roth SC, Baudin J, McCormick DC, Edwards AD, Townsend J, Stewart AL, Reynolds EOR. Relationship between ultrasound appearance of the brain of very preterm infants and neurodevelopmental impairment at 8 years. Dev Med Child Neurol 1993;35:755-768.

127. Sann L, Bienvenu F, Lahet C, Bienvenu J, Bethenod M. Comparison of the composition of breast milk from mothers of term and preterm infants. Acta Paediatr

(23)

Scand 1981;70:115-116.

128. Schanler RJ, Oh W. Composition of breast milk obtained from mothers of premature infants as compared to breast milk obtained from donors. J Pediatr 1980;96:679-681. 129. Shirataki S, Prechtl HFR. Sleep state transitions in newborn infants: preliminary

study. Dev Med Child Neurol 1977;19:316-325.

130. Silber GH, Hachey DL, Schanler RJ, Garza C. Manipulation of maternal diet to alter fatty acid composition of human milk intended for premature infants. Am J Clin Nutr

1988;47:810-814.

131. Simonin C, Rüegg M, Sidiropoulos D. Comparison of the fat content and fat globule size distribution of breast milk from mothers delivering term and preterm . Am J clin Nutr 1984;40:820-826.

132. Smit BJ, Kok HJ, de Vries LS, van Wassenaer AG, Dekker FW, Ongerboer de Visser BW. Somatosensory evoked potentials in very preterm infants in relation to L-thyroxine supplementation. Pediatr 1998;101:865-869.

133. Stefanski M, Schulze K, Bateman D, Kairam R, Pedley TA, Masterson J, James LS. A scoring system for states of sleep and wakefulness in term and preterm infants. Ped Res 1984;18:58-62.

134. Stern E, Parmelee AH, Akiyama Y, Schultz MA, Wenner WH. Sleep cycle characteristics in infants. Pediatr 1969;43:65-70.

135. Taylor HG, Hack M, Klein N, Schatschneider C. Achievement in children with birth weights less than 750 grams with normal cognitive abilities: evidence for specific learning abilities. J Pediatr Psychol 1995;20:703-719.

136. Thoman EB. Sleeping and waking states in infants: a functional perspective. Neuroscience & Biobehav Rev 1990;14:93-107.

137. Thomas MR, Chan GM, Book LS. Comparison of macronutrient concentration of preterm human milk between two milk expression techniques and two techniques for quantitation of energy. J Pediatr Gastroenterol Nutr 1986;5:597-601.

138. Usher R, McLean F. Intrauterine growth of live-born Caucasian infants at sea level: standards obtained from measurements in 7 dimensions of infants born between 25 and 44 weeks of gestation. J Pediatr 1969;74:901-910.

(24)

to 32 weeks of gestation. Ped Res 1987;22:655-658.

140. Vies JS, van Oostenbrugge RJ, Hasaart TH, Caberg H, Kingma H, Casaer PJ, Blanco CE. State profile in low-risk pre-term infants: a longitudinal syudy of 7 infants from 32-36 weeks of postmenstrual age. Brain Dev 1992;14:12-17.

141. Vies J, Kingma H, Caberg H, Duniels H, Casaer P. Posture and motility of lowrisk preterm infants: a study from 32-36 weeks of postconceptional age. Dev Med Child Neurol 1989;31:191-195.

142. Winick M, ed. Malnutrition and brain Development. Oxford University Press. New York, London, Toronto, 1976.

143. Winick M, Rosso P. Head circumference and cellular growth of the brain in normal and marasmic children. J Pediatr 1969;74:774-778.

144. Winick M, Rosso P, Waterlow J. Cellular growth of cerebrum, cerebellum, and brain stem in normal and marasmic children. Experimental neurol 1970;26:393-400. 145. Wright K, Dawson JP, Fallis D, Vogt E, Lorch V. New postnatal growth grids for

(25)

Referenties

GERELATEERDE DOCUMENTEN

Natuurlijk maak je een proefschrift niet alleen, daar zijn altijd meerdere mensen bij betrokken. Daarom schrijven de meeste promovendi een dankwoord achterin hun

Gedurende haar studietijd loopt Marleen stage bij Vincent Christoffels en Antoon Moorman op de afdeling anatomie en embryologie aan het Academisch Medisch Centrum (AMC) te

Epithelial barrier and dendritic cell function in the intestinal mucosa..

AA classical broadcast channel allows one party to send a classical bit to all the otherr players. In the quantum setting this would mean that a qubit would be sentt to all the

It is not permitted to download or to forward/distribute the text or part of it without the consent of the author(s) and/or copyright holder(s), other than for strictly

If you believe that digital publication of certain material infringes any of your rights or (privacy) interests, please let the Library know, stating your reasons. In case of

It is not permitted to download or to forward/distribute the text or part of it without the consent of the author(s) and/or copyright holder(s), other than for strictly

UvA-DARE is a service provided by the library of the University of Amsterdam (http s ://dare.uva.nl) UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository).. Quantum query complexity and