• No results found

Researching trust in Wikipedia

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Share "Researching trust in Wikipedia"

Copied!
2
0
0

Bezig met laden.... (Bekijk nu de volledige tekst)

Hele tekst

(1)

Chi Sparks  Conference proceedings  23 June 2011 

Researching Trust in Wikipedia

Teun Lucassen

University of Twente

P.O. Box 215, 7500 AE

Enschede, The Netherlands

+31 53 489 3604

t.lucassen@gw.utwente.nl

Jan Maarten Schraagen

University of Twente

P.O. Box 215, 7500 AE

Enschede, The Netherlands

+31 53 489 3604

j.m.c.schraagen@gw.utwente.nl

ABSTRACT

As the use of collaborative online encyclopedias such as Wikipedia grows, so does the need for research on how users evaluate its credibility. In this paper we compare three experimental approaches to study trust in Wikipedia, namely think aloud, eye-tracking, and online questionnaires. The advantages and disadvantages of each method are discussed. We conclude that it is best to use multiple methods when researching information trust, as each single one of the discussed methods alone does not give all possible information.

Keywords

Trust, credibility, Wikipedia, think aloud, eye-tracking, online questionnaires.

INTRODUCTION

Over the last decade, Wikipedia has evolved from a small spin-off project to a massive online encyclopedia, covering almost every imaginable topic. Wikipedia owes its rapid growth to its collaborative nature, with thousands of volunteers contributing to the articles. Information quality has been shown quite high [3]; however, the open-source character also brings a large disadvantage as the authors of the information are mostly unknown. This means that the user can never be sure of the trustworthiness and expertise of the author and thus has to be aware of the possibility of less credible information.

In this paper, we describe three experiments in which the way users cope with the uncertainty of the credibility of information is investigated. First, the Wikipedia Screening Task is introduced. After this, three experiments in which different methodologies were applied are shortly discussed, each of them featuring this task in a different setting. WIKIPEDIA SCREENING TASK

The Wikipedia Screening Task was first introduced in [4]. In this task, a Wikipedia article is presented, in which obvious cues of credibility, such [citation needed] remarks, are removed. The participant is asked to evaluate the credibility of the article. It is not specified how to perform this task, so the participant is free to incorporate features from the article which he or she deems relevant to credibility.

Various manipulations can be made in experiments featuring the Wikipedia Screening Task. An example is

varying the quality of the articles, based on the ratings given by the Wikipedia Editorial Team1. A second useful manipulation is the familiarity of the participant with the topic at hand, as different behavior can be expected when a participant has some knowledge on the content.

THINK ALOUD

In this experiment, the Wikipedia Screening Task was performed while thinking aloud [4]. Using this method, the participant is asked to verbalize everything that comes to mind while performing a task [2]. Audio is recorded and typed out afterwards. Based on this, the utterances of each participant can be categorized using a coding scheme. Using only a few participants (N=12), a comprehensive list of features relevant to trust could be established. It was found that the most important features were references (quantity and quality), several textual features (e.g., length, comprehensiveness), and pictures (e.g., quality, relevance). Article quality and familiarity with the topic were manipulated. The provided credibility ratings indicated that the participants (college students) were able to distinguish good and poor information quality. However, no differences in the features used could be found between good and poor information quality as well as familiar and unfamiliar topics.

Think aloud is a great method to gather a lot of rich information on the behavior of participants. Direct insights into cognitive processes of the participants are gained, as data is gathered during task execution, instead of afterwards. Only a few participants were needed to form a comprehensive list of features relevant to trust.

On the downside, the data analysis of think-aloud is very labor-intensive. All utterances of each participant have to be fully typed out before they can be categorized. Furthermore, two coders are needed in order to calculate inter-rate reliability. A second drawback is that thinking-aloud does not come naturally to everybody. Some participants will be more capable of expressing their thoughts than others.

1http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Wikipedia:Version_1.0_Edito

(2)

Chi Sparks  Conference proceedings  23 June 2011  EYE-TRACKING

In a second experiment, the Wikipedia Screening Task was performed while the gaze of the participants was constantly monitored using an eye-tracker [6]. Gaze is a very good indicator of visual attention [1]. In this experiment, we were particularly interested in the use of references in credibility evaluation. Four strategies of reference use in credibility evaluation were proposed: (1) references are not considered, (2) the presence of references is checked, (3) the number of references is checked, and (4) the quality of references is checked.

Each of these strategies can easily be distinguished by visual inspection of the eye-tracking data. It was found that each of the strategies was applied, with the dominant strategy being the first, namely not to consider the references at all.

This observation seems to contradict to the findings in the think aloud experiment, in which references were found to be very important to the participants. This may indicate socially desirable behavior in the first experiment. However, the lack of visual attention does not necessarily mean that references were not part of the mental model of the participants

Eye-tracking has the advantage to be less obtrusive than think aloud. Using a table-mounted device, participants are somewhat restricted in their movements, but they are not required to perform a secondary task. Moreover, gaze is a very good indicator of visual attention, so the elements of an article which are attended when evaluating credibility can be assumed to be of relevance to the participants. A drawback of eye-tracking is that while we know what objects are attended, we don’t know why. One could suggest a combination of eye-tracking and think aloud to obtain this information. However, this is not advisable since think aloud tends to slow down task performance, which may lead to different attention. A possible solution is retrospective think aloud. In this method, the participant first performs the task without thinking aloud (but possibly with an eye-tracker). Video is recorded and played back directly afterwards. During the play-back the participants is asked to verbalize what they were thinking when they were performing the task.

ONLINE QUESTIONNAIRES

Whereas the think aloud and eye-tracking studies both took place in a controlled lab-environment, a different approach was taken in a third experiment, which was performed online [5]. Over 650 Internet users took part in a short experiment, in which the influence of factual accuracy on trust of experts and novices was examined. This was done by showing them articles on car engines, featuring varying levels of deliberate errors.

It was shown that the experts were influenced by the errors, whereas the novices were not. Moreover, proof was found for the proposed 3S-model, in which three strategies were

proposed in which domain expertise, information skills, and source experience lead to different features in the information being noticed.

Obviously the biggest advantage of online questionnaires is that a high number of participants can easily be recruited, for instance by posting requests for participation on online forums. This also means that participants with particular characteristics can be addressed, for instance automotive experts.

However, these advantages come with an immediate drawback: the behavior of the participants can hardly be accounted for. It can never be assured that each participant takes the experiment seriously. Therefore, the data should always be inspected manually for bogus answers. Furthermore, it is very hard to repeat an experiment or perform a similar experiment, as the same participants (with unwanted prior knowledge) may participate again. CONCLUSION

In this paper we showed three methods to combine with the Wikipedia Screening Task. Each method has its own advantages and disadvantages, but the most important lesson learned is that different approaches yield different results. When researching online trust, multiple methods should be apprehended to avoid that conclusions on credibility evaluation behavior are biased by the methodology applied.

REFERENCES

1. A. T. Duchowski. A breadth-first survey of eye-tracking applications. Behav Res Methods Instrum Comput, vol. 34, no. 4, pp. 455-470, Nov. 2002.

2. K. A. Ericsson and H. A. Simon. Protocol Analysis:

Verbal Reports as Data. The MIT Press, 1984.

3. J. Giles. Internet encyclopaedias go head to head. Nature, vol. 438, no. 7070, pp. 900-901, Dec. 2005.

4. T. Lucassen and J. M. Schraagen. Trust in Wikipedia: how users trust information from an unknown source. In Proceedings of the 4th workshop on Information

credibility (WICOW '10). ACM, New York, NY, USA,

pp. 19-26, 2010.

5. T. Lucassen and J. M. Schraagen. Factual Accuracy and Trust in Information: The Role of Expertise. Journal of the American Society for Information Science and Technology, in press.

6. T. Lucassen, M. Risto, M. L. Noordzij, and J. M. Schraagen. Strategies of Reference Use in Credibility Evaluations of Wikipedia Articles. In preparation.

Referenties

GERELATEERDE DOCUMENTEN

- het behoort niet tot de alkenen, want er zijn chlooratomen aanwezig in het molecuul 13 Wanneer CH 3 Br ontleedt, kunnen de ontledingsproducten alleen C- , H- en

Bepaalde informatie over het aantal binnenkomende links kan gemakkelijk uit de boxplot in combinatie met tabel 2 afgelezen worden. Voor andere informatie is het

− Uit de boxplot (in combinatie met tabel 2) kun je gemakkelijker afleiden dat (minstens) de helft van de artikelen 9 of meer binnenkomende links heeft. Juiste voorbeelden bij

KNOL and uTest provide similar incentives to improve contributions, like showing the pageviews of your article (KNOL), or placing a ranking of most bugs approved

ABSTRACT In de afgelopen jaren is Wikipedia uitgegroeid tot één van de meest dominante partijen in de informatievoorziening op het internet, maar ze wordt

Wikipedia is een internetenencyclopedie die gemaakt wordt door talloze vrijwilligers, en die door iedereen kan worden aangepast. Hoe blijft iets in stand dat iedereen kan

In this study we investigate whether surface features of articles, such as 'references', 'internal links', 'pictures', and 'length', positively influence trust judgments.. We will

working with the condition cued CTAP take significantly more time with finishing a task than in comparison to the conditions classic CTAP and classic RTAP.. This result is not in