• No results found

Minority influence in climate change mitigation

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Share "Minority influence in climate change mitigation"

Copied!
7
0
0

Bezig met laden.... (Bekijk nu de volledige tekst)

Hele tekst

(1)

University of Groningen

Minority influence in climate change mitigation

Bolderdijk, Jan Willem; Jans, Lise

Published in:

Current Opinion in Psychology DOI:

10.1016/j.copsyc.2021.02.005

IMPORTANT NOTE: You are advised to consult the publisher's version (publisher's PDF) if you wish to cite from it. Please check the document version below.

Document Version

Publisher's PDF, also known as Version of record

Publication date: 2021

Link to publication in University of Groningen/UMCG research database

Citation for published version (APA):

Bolderdijk, J. W., & Jans, L. (2021). Minority influence in climate change mitigation. Current Opinion in Psychology, 42, 25-30. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2021.02.005

Copyright

Other than for strictly personal use, it is not permitted to download or to forward/distribute the text or part of it without the consent of the author(s) and/or copyright holder(s), unless the work is under an open content license (like Creative Commons).

Take-down policy

If you believe that this document breaches copyright please contact us providing details, and we will remove access to the work immediately and investigate your claim.

Downloaded from the University of Groningen/UMCG research database (Pure): http://www.rug.nl/research/portal. For technical reasons the number of authors shown on this cover page is limited to 10 maximum.

(2)

Review

Minority influence in climate change mitigation

Jan Willem Bolderdijk

1

and Lise Jans

2

Abstract

While the majority of people care about environmental quality, they keep engaging in carbon-intensive practices that exac-erbate climate change. Can we expect humans to collectively change by themselves, from the bottom up? Social change is often initiated by minorities – individuals who challenge the status quo. The dominant literature paints a rather pessimistic picture about the ability of minorities to instigate change in the environmental domain: environmental activists, vegans, and other minority members often elicit social sanctions, thereby ironically reinforcing the majority’s commitment to current, environmentally harmful norms. Recent findings, however, point towards more optimism: pro-environmental minorities can pave the way towards‘tipping points’ and spontaneous social change. Policymakers can speed up this process by offering top-down support for minorities – by giving them ‘voice’. Addresses

1

Department of Marketing, Faculty of Business and Economics, University of Groningen, Nettelbosje 2, 9747 AE Groningen, The Netherlands

2Department of Psychology, Faculty of Behavioural and Social

Sci-ences, University of Groningen, Grote Kruisstraat 2/1, 9712 TS, The Netherlands

Corresponding author: Bolderdijk, Jan Willem (j.w.bolderdijk@rug.nl)

Current Opinion in Psychology 2021, 42:25–30

This review comes from a themed issue on Psychology of Climate Change

Edited by Mark F. Ferguson and Michael T. Schmitt For a complete overview see theIssueand theEditorial

Available online 17 February 2021

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2021.02.005

2352-250X/© 2021 The Author(s). Published by Elsevier Ltd. This is an open access article under the CC BY license (http://creativecommons. org/licenses/by/4.0/).

Keywords

Minorities, Social influence, Pro-environmental behaviour, Social change, Tipping points, Moral do-gooders.

Human behaviour plays a key role in the rise and severity of environmental problems, including climate change. Dropping common but carbon-intensive prac-tices such as eating red meat on a daily basis and flying across the ocean for holidays is, therefore, part and parcel of the transition to a more sustainable society [1]. The majority of people see this need for social change.

They are aware of the environmental costs of their current actions [2], value environmental quality [3,4], and perceive protecting the environment as their moral duty [5]. Yet, they keep engaging in practices that exacerbate climate change. Acting sustainably is often-times still the (descriptive) social norm [6e8]. Can we expect more sustainable social norms to evolve from the bottom up - emerging organically as a result of social interactions between individuals?

Social change is often instigated by minorities e those taking an anti-normative position, holding no position of status [9]. As discussed below, recent literature paints a rather pessimistic picture about minorities’ ability to overthrow current, unsustainable norms: individuals who publicly challenge normative but unsustainable practices often run the risk of being ridiculed by ma-jority members. Other findings, however, offer room for optimism: minorities who go against current, unsus-tainable norms may be able to pave the way for social change in society at large. However, their influence is often subtle and indirect, and thus easily overlooked, both by policymakers and minorities themselves.

Why social change from the bottom up

seems unlikely

By challenging the status quo, minorities give exposure to alternatives [10], thereby gradually ‘infecting’ others in their social network [11]. Ultimately, such alterna-tives may become adopted by the majority and thus become the new social norm. Social change unfolds more quickly when people anticipate they can reap reputational benefits by being among the first to adopt [12,13]. This dynamic helps to explain the rapid diffu-sion of new technologies (e.g. smartphones) across human networkse frontrunners are typically considered ‘savvy’.

However, the same reputational benefits may not befall frontrunners whose practices challenge an unsustainable status quo, such as vegans, environmental activists, and ethically conscious consumers. These individuals are typically motivated by non-selfish, moral concernse e.g. the conviction that meat consumption is exacerbating climate change and thus harming future generations. Their lack of self-interest could make them especially effective change advocates [14,15], but recent research found that such ‘do-gooders’ can also elicit hostile re-sponses in others. When acting out of moral concern,

(3)

frontrunners may implicitly question the moral integrity [16,17] and altruistic reputation [18] of those who stick with the current, unsustainable status quo. As a result, majority members can respond defensively when inter-acting with environmentally conscious frontrunners; they may derogate, ridicule, or even exclude individuals who stand up to environmental disregard [19], refuse to eat meat [20] or promote no-packaging stores [21*]. This sanctioning of do-gooders is not unique to the environmental domain and appears to happen across different cultures [22], but especially in ‘tight’ societies [23*]. Importantly, by inviting social sanctions from majority members [17], triggering negative stereotypes [24,25], and by shaping exclusive social identities [26**], environmentally conscious frontrunners have even been argued to reinforce the majority’s commit-ment to the current, environcommit-mentally damaging norms, thus potentially slowing down social change.

In sum, recent literature seems to paint a rather pessi-mistic picture of the ability of environmentally conscious frontrunners (e.g. activists, vegans) to over-throw current, unsustainable social norms and that of social change to emerge in a bottom-up fashion. How-ever, this interpretation overlooks another stream of literature, one that documents the positive impact of minorities on social change.

A more optimistic view on the impact of

minorities

Within a group, minorities are a source of intragroup conflict, as they threaten the validity of the majority opinion and thus prevent group consensus [9,27]. The most obvious way to resolve this intragroup threat is for majority members to pressure minority members to change their public behaviour via social sanctions (i.e. conformity) or to exclude or derogate those unwilling to budge [28,29]. As reviewed above, this response has been frequently documented in recent psychological literature on responses to environmentally conscious minorities. Conformity pressure and the social exclusion of minorities are, however, not the only ways for groups to deal with deviance [9,27e29]. Minorities, although not always in obvious ways, can influence majorities.

Minorities can change private opinions

By introducing deviant views, minorities (especially when consistently sticking to the same deviant position on multiple occasions) can trigger majority members to consciously reconsider their private opinion about the status quo, a process called ‘conversion’ [9,27,28]. While private conversion can occur directly as a result of expo-sure to dissent, minority influence on publicly visible actions tends to be indirect, as people want to avoid po-tential social sanctions from the majority [9,27]. On a societal level, this means that minorities initially only affect others’ opinions and only later affect their public

behaviours [30]. Indeed, within human networks, atti-tudes and opinions often spread before actions do [31]. The same process may hold in the context of pro-environmental minorities. While many people, thanks to the groundwork by minorities in the past, now pri-vately have adopted the notion that climate change is an urgent problem and that drastic change is needed, their public actions often lag behind. This lag, at least partly, may be driven by fear of social sanctions: people strongly fear social exclusion when their deviant moral principles are exposed to majority members [32], and fear of stigma has been found to prevent aspiring vegans from adopting a plant-based diet [33]. Indeed, particularly in ‘tight’ societies, in which conformity pressure is strong, public behaviours often do not reflect private pro-environmental opinions [34]. Thus, while pro-environmental minorities may affect others’ private opinions, their effect on others’ public actions is often less visible. As a result, both minority and majority members may underestimate the influence that minor-ities can have.

Minorities can change norm perceptions

Norms tend to be self-fulfilling [35*,36]. An oft-cited example is that of meat consumption: if most people believe that most people eat meat, most people will keep eating meat [37] without questioning the validity of that practice. At the collective level, the pressure to conform to current majorities can create a self-perpetuating feedback loop, whereby a network of in-dividuals can end up getting ‘locked in’ [38**] by a social norm that is societally disadvantageous [39]. One crucial effect of minorities is that they challenge the self-fulfilling nature of current norms. By deviating from the status quo, they show that an alternative path is possible: an increase in the number of vegetarians, for instance, signals to majority members that high levels of meat consumption are not a ‘natural’ or lasting feature of prosperous societies per se. Thus, the awareness that an increasing minority is deviating from the norm (i.e. dy-namic norms) can give majority members the feeling that the current norm is ‘vulnerable’ [40], less ‘tight’ than previously thought [41,42], and will likely change in the future [43**], thus motivating majority members to deviate as well.

Recent studies have demonstrated this outcome in the context of meat consumption [44]. Mentioning the fact that a minority of Americans already limits their meat consumption had little impact on non-vegan partici-pants. They, however became more interested in eating less meat themselves after reading a message indicating that, although still a minority, more and more Americans ‘make an effort to reduce/limit their meat consumption’. Communicating that an increasing number of people are 26 Psychology of Climate Change

(4)

choosing meatless dishes may even increase the number of vegetarian orders, provided that recipients perceive that trend to apply to their own group [45].

Thus, pro-environmental minorities can influence ma-jority members when they are perceived as frontrunners of an emerging social norm. While summary statistics [46*] and written messages (‘more and more people are switching from to-go-cups to sustainable alternatives’) can convey that change is imminent [47], physical dis-plays of deviance are typically more powerful in inspiring others [48,49]. Indeed, by organising pro-environmental initiatives in public settings (e.g. jointly picking up litter, community energy projects), minority members can make majority members of the same overarching group re-appraise their shared identity, strengthening the perception of pro-environmental group norms and identification with this group [50**]. Minority mem-bers’ public pro-environmental initiatives can thereby motivate other members of the overarching group to also display pro-environmental actions [50,51].

Minorities can offer social support for others

Asch [52] and others after him [53] demonstrated that dissenters can facilitate others to also withstand major-ity pressure, and liberate them to deviate as well. For majority members who are privately uncomfortable with existing environmentally harmful norms but whose be-haviours lag behind due to fear of social sanctions, the physical presence of ‘allies’ seems pivotal. During social encounters with a unanimous majority of meat-eating peers, many vegetarian participants chose to keep their meat-free principles to themselves, thus silently reinforcing the meat-eating norm [54**]. This pattern disappeared, however, when a physically present moderator from the same overarching group showed visible support for the minority position: vegetarian participants now consistently displayed their meat-free principles, despite the physical presence of a majority of meat-eating peers.

In sum, by breaking the consensus, visible minority members can give witnesses the feeling they would not be alone when publicly challenging an environmentally damaging practice as well, thus facilitating others to follow suit.

Change may come

Without realising it, minority members can influence the opinions and actions of majority members. Minor-ities seem most influential when they are perceived as representative of the larger group they share with the majority [45,50**, cf.55], and when they ‘walk the talk’ [56]e when their words are supported by their actions (e.g. joining a solar panel program themselves while promoting it among majority members [57]). Impor-tantly, those ‘infected’ majority members may, in turn,

start influencing other majority members as well [58]. By showing their deviant principles, minorities can therefore help to create a vicious cycle [59,60] whereby the magnetic force of the new norm increases over time, up to the point where ‘critical mass’ is reached; when a pro-environmental practice reaches a certain threshold, the likelihood of it spreading throughout the entire social network (i.e. the majority) increases dramatically. However, when a minority is significant but has not reached critical mass (recently pinpointed to hover around 25% of the population [61]) or fails to elicit sufficient ‘momentum’ [62], there is a chance that, despite seeing the benefits of behavioural change, the majority will not change their behaviour. The lack of visible change in behaviours, however, does not mean that the majority has a different opinion than the mi-nority or that the mimi-nority is not influential. Rather, it may mean that the pro-environmental minority is not influencing behaviours, i.e. descriptive norms, yet.

Concluding remarks

The fact that currently, carbon-intensive practices are still commonplace (e.g. high levels of meat consump-tion) and that minorities challenging those norms un-dergo social sanctions does not mean that social change is not happening, nor that minorities are ineffective in instigating change from the bottom up. Rather, minority influence is often more indirect: they initially change majority opinions (e.g. more and more people consider themselves ‘flexitarian’) and often only later affect ma-jority behaviours. Although subtle, their influence is crucial: once majority members notice that norms are shifting, sudden tipping points can ensue. Thus, we see room for optimism.

To kickstart this process, however, existing minorities need to share their desire for change with majority members during social interactions. There is reason to assume this will not happen readily: existing minorities may erroneously believe they are not ‘infecting’ others and merely risk social sanctions by deviating from an unsustainable norm. We, therefore, believe that policymakers may be able to speed up social change in the environmental domain by encouraging (preferably consistent and representative) minority members to publicly challenge unsustainable norms during social interactions. We see this as an important avenue for future research.

Specifically, policymakers can introduce interventions targeted at making it more socially comfortable for vegans, environmental activists, and other minority members to challenge the environmentally damaging norms in their social network during interactions with majority members [see e.g. 63]. The introduction of vegetarian defaults, for instance, could make it more comfortable for aspiring vegetarians to introduce

(5)

plant-based meals in more conservative social environments. Further, policymakers can (financially and logistically) support bottom-up pro-environmental initiatives, thereby facilitating minority members to influence ma-jority members through their visible, pro-environmental actions [cf.50**].

In sum, while social change can and does occur spon-taneously in a bottom-up fashion (e.g. #MeToo and #BlackLivesMatter), policymakers may be able to speed up this process in the environmental domain by giving existing minorities ‘voice’.

Conflict of interest statement

Nothing declared.

Acknowledgements

The authors thank Hans Risselada and Anna Tirion for providing comments on an earlier version of this manuscript. This research was supported by grant 421-14-020 awarded to Jan Willem Bolderdijk by the Dutch Research Council (NWO).

References

Papers of particular interest, published within the period of review, have been highlighted as:

* of special interest * * of outstanding interest

1. Intergovernmental Panel on Climate Change (IPCC). In Global warming of 1.5C. An IPCC Special Report on the impacts of global warming of 1.5C above pre-industrial levels and related global greenhouse gas emission pathways, in the context of strengthening the global response to the threat of climate change, sustainable development, and efforts to eradicate poverty. Edited by Masson-Delmotte V, Zhai P, Pörtner H-O, Roberts D, Skea J, Shukla PR, Pirani A, Moufouma-Okia W, Péan C, Pidcock R, Connors S, Matthews JBR, Chen Y, Zhou X, Gomis MI, Lonnoy E, Maycock T, Tignor M, Waterfield T; 2018.https://www.ipcc.ch/ sr15/; 2018.

2. Hornsey MJ, Fielding KS: Understanding (and reducing) inac-tion on climate change. Soc Iss Policy Rev 2020, 14(1):3–35,

https://doi.org/10.1111/sipr.12058.

3. Bouman T, Steg L: Motivating society-wide pro-environmental change. One Earth 2019, 1:27–30,https://doi.org/10.1016/ j.oneear.2019.08.002.

4. Eurobarometer: Attitudes of European citizens towards the envi-ronment. Special Eurobarometer 468, European Union. 2017.

https://mehi.hu/sites/default/files/ebs_468_en_1.pdf.

5. Jia F, Soucie K, Alisat S, Curtin D, Pratt M: Are environmental issues moral issues? moral identity in relation to protecting the natural world. J Environ Psychol 2017, 52:104–113,https:// doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2017.06.004.

6. Amel E, Manning C, Scott B, Koger S: Beyond the roots of human inaction: fostering collective effort toward ecosystem conservation. Science 2017, 356(6335):275–279,https:// doi.org/10.1126/science.aal1931.

7. Gifford R, Lacroix K, Chen A: Understanding responses to climate change: psychological barriers to mitigation and a new theory of behavioural choice. In Psychology and climate change: human perceptions, impacts, and responses. Edited by Clayton S, Manning C, New York: Academic; 2018:161–184. 8. Sparkman G, Howe L, Walton G: How social norms are often a

barrier to addressing climate change but can be part of the solution. Behav Public Policy 2020:1–28,https://doi.org/10.1017/ bpp.2020.42.

9. Moscovici S: Toward a theory of conversion behaviour. Adv Exp Soc Psychol 1980, 13:209–239.

10. Bollinger B, Gillingham K: Peer effects in the diffusion of solar photovoltaic panels. Market Sci 2012, 31(6):900–912,https:// doi.org/10.1287/mksc.1120.0727.

11. Nardini G, Rank-Christman T, Bublitz MG, Cross SN,

Peracchio LA: Together we rise: how social movements suc-ceed. J Cons Psychol 2020,https://doi.org/10.1002/jcpy.1201. 20201020.

12. Catalini C, Tucker C: When early adopters don't adopt. Science 2017, 357(6347):135–136,https://doi.org/10.1126/

science.aal4476.

13. Noppers E, Keizer K, Milovanovic M, Steg L: The role of adop-tion norms and perceived product attributes in the adopadop-tion of Dutch electric vehicles and smart energy systems. Energy Res Soc Science 2019, 57:101237,https://doi.org/10.1016/ j.erss.2019.101237.

14. Maass A, Clark RD: Hidden impact of minorities: fifteen years of minority influence research. Psychol Bull 1984, 95(3):428,

https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.95.3.428.

15. Pohling R, Diessner R: Moral elevation and moral beauty: a review of the empirical literature. Rev Gen Psychol 2016, 20(4): 412–425,https://doi.org/10.1037/gpr0000089.

16. Minson JA, Monin B: Do-gooder derogation: disparaging morally motivated minorities to defuse anticipated reproach. Soc Psychol Pers Sci 2012, 3(2):200–207,https://doi.org/ 10.1177/1948550611415695.

17. Zane DM, Irwin JR, Reczek RW: Do less ethical consumers denigrate more ethical consumers? the effect of willful ignorance on judgments of others. J Consum Psychol 2016, 26(3):337–349,https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcps.2015.10.002. 18. Pleasant A, Barclay P: Why hate the good guy? antisocial

punishment of high cooperators is greater when people compete to be chosen. Psychol Sci 2018, 29(6):868–876,

https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797617752642.

19. Steentjes K, Kurz T, Barreto M, Morton TA: The norms associ-ated with climate change: understanding social norms through acts of interpersonal activism. Global Environ Change 2017, 43:116–125,https://doi.org/10.1016/

j.gloenvcha.2017.01.008.

20. MacInnis CC, Hodson G: It ain't easy eating greens: evidence of bias toward vegetarians and vegans from both source and target. Group Process Intergr Relat 2017, 20(6):721–744,https:// doi.org/10.1177/1368430215618253.

21

* . Bolderdijk JW, Brouwer C, Cornelissen G: When do morallymotivated innovators elicit inspiration instead of irritation? Front Psychol 2017, 8,https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.02362. 2362–2362.

The authors show that environmentally conscious frontrunners can make witnesses feel morally inadequate, and thereby elicit defensive responses.

22. Herrmann B, Thöni C, Gächter S: Antisocial punishment across societies. Science 2008, 319(5868):1362–1367.

23 *

. Kawamura Y, Kusumi T: Altruism does not always lead to a good reputation: a normative explanation. J Exp Soc Psychol 2020, 90:104021.

Across a systematic set of studies, the authors demonstrate that do-gooders may be disliked merely because of the fact that their altru-istic actions can be considered deviant.

24. Bashir NY, Lockwood P, Chasteen AL, Nadolny D, Noyes I: The ironic impact of activists: negative stereotypes reduce social change influence. Eur J Soc Psychol 2013, 43(7):614–626,

https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.1983.

25. Brough AR, Wilkie JEB, Ma J, Isaac MS, Gal D: Is eco-friendly unmanly? the green-feminine stereotype and its effect on sustainable consumption. J Consum Res 2016, 43(4):567–582,

https://doi.org/10.1093/jcr/ucw044. [26]

* *. Kurz T, Prosser AMB, Rabinovich A, O'Neill S: Could vegansand lycra cyclists be bad for the planet? theorizing the role of moralized minority practice identities in processes of 28 Psychology of Climate Change

(6)

societal-level change. J Soc Issues 2020, 76(1):86–100,

https://doi.org/10.1111/josi.12366.

Building on social identity theory, the authors explain how minorities that strongly identify with green practices can ironically demotivate others to join their ranks, thus slowing down social change.

27. Wood W, Lundgren S, Ouellette JA, Busceme S, Blackstone T: Minority influence: a meta-analytic review of social influence processes. Psychol Bull 1994, 115(3):323–345.

28. Butera F, Falomir-Pichastor JM, Mugny G, Quiamzade A: Mi-nority Influence. In The Oxford Handbook of Social Influence. Edited by Harkins SG, Williams KD, Burger J, Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press; 2017:317–337.

29. Jetten J, Hornsey MJ: Deviance and dissent in groups. Annu Rev Psychol 2014, 65(1):461–485,https://doi.org/10.1146/ annurev-psych-010213-115151.

30. Jung J, Crano WD, Bramson A: An agent-based model of in-direct minority influence on social change and diversity. Soc Influ 2018, 13(1):18–38,https://doi.org/10.1080/

15534510.2017.1415961.

31. Zino L, Ye M, Cao M: A two-layer model for coevolving opinion dynamics and collective decision-making in complex social systems. Chaos 2020, 30(8),https://doi.org/10.1063/5.0004787. 32. Jans L, Koudenburg N, Dillmann J, Wichgers L, Postmes T, den Hartigh RJR: Dynamic reactions to opinion deviance: the role of social identity formation. J Exp Soc Psychol 2019, 84: 103803,https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2019.04.001.

33. Markowski KL, Roxburgh S:“If I became a vegan, my family and friends would hate me:” Anticipating vegan stigma as a barrier to plant-based diets. Appetite 2019, 135:1–9,https:// doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2018.12.040.

34. Tam K-P, Chan H-W: Environmental concern has a weaker association with pro-environmental behavior in some soci-eties than others: a cross-cultural psychology perspective. J Environ Psychol 2017, 53:213–223,https://doi.org/10.1016/ j.jenvp.2017.09.001.

35 *

. Hawkins RXD, Goodman ND, Goldstone RL: The emergence of social norms and conventions. Trends Cognit Sci 2019, 23(2): 158–169,https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2018.11.003.

A comprehensive overview outlines how norms differ from related concepts such as conventions, and how norms affect human behaviour at the interpersonal and collective level.

36. Lindström B, Jangard S, Selbing I, Olsson A: The role of a "common is moral" heuristic in the stability and change of moral norms. J Exp Psychol Gen 2018, 147(2):228–242,https:// doi.org/10.1037/xge0000365.

37. Leenaert T: How to create a vegan world: a pragmatic approach. Lantern Books 2017.

[38]

* *. Muthukrishna M, Schaller M: Are collectivistic cultures moreprone to rapid transformation? computational models of cross-cultural differences, social network structure, dy-namic social influence, and cultural change. Pers Soc Psychol Rev 2020, 24(2):103–120,https://doi.org/10.1177/ 1088868319855783.

Using computer simulations, the authors demonstrate that the micro-level tendency to conform to local norms leads to a more pro-nounced s-shape in diffusion; it takes collective societies longer to escape current norms, but once critical mass is reached, change un-folds faster. The paper also convincingly explains how social simula-tions can be used by psychologists to generate novel hypotheses. 39. Smerdon D, Offerman T, Gneezy U:‘Everybody's doing it’: on

the persistence of bad social norms. Expl Econs 2020, 23: 392–420,https://doi.org/10.1007/s10683-019-09616-z. 40. Sunstein CR: How Change Happens. MIT Press; 2019. 41. Gelfand MJ, Harrington JR, Jackson JC: The strength of social

norms across human groups. Perspect Psychol Sci 2017, 12(5):800–809,https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691617708631. 42. Gelfand MJ, Raver JL, Nishii L, Leslie LM, Lun J, Lim BC, Duan L,

Almaliach A, Ang S, Arnadottir J, et al.: Differences between tight and loose cultures: a 33-nation study. Science 2011, 332(6033):1100–1104,https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1197754.

43 * *

. Sparkman G, Walton GM: Witnessing change: dynamic norms help resolve diverse barriers to personal change. J Exp Soc Psychol 2019, 82:238–252,https://doi.org/10.1016/

j.jesp.2019.01.007.

Building on their earlier work, the authors explore the various reasons why exposure to dynamic norms – information signaling that an increasing minority is adopting a behaviour – influences majorities. The authors conclude that dynamic norms give witnesses the feeling they can personally change is possible, is compatible with desired identities, and is valued by others.

44. Sparkman G, Walton GM: Dynamic norms promote sustain-able behavior, even if it is counternormative. Psychol Sci 2017, 28(11):1663–1674,https://doi.org/10.1177/

0956797617719950.

45. Sparkman G, Weitz E, Robinson TN, Malhotra N, Walton GM: Developing a scalable dynamic norm menu-based interven-tion to reduce meat consumpinterven-tion. Sustainability 2020, 12(6),

https://doi.org/10.3390/su12062453. 2453–2453. 46

*

. Mortensen CR, Neel R, Cialdini RB, Jaeger CM, Jacobson RP, Ringel MM: Trending norms: a lever for encouraging behav-iors performed by the minority. Soc Psychol Pers Sci 2019, 10(2):201–210,https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550617734615. This work adds empirical support to the notion that minorities, at least when perceived as increasing in size, affect not only opinions, but also non-publicly visible actions; reduced water consumption.

47. Loschelder DD, Siepelmeyer H, Fischer D, Rubel JA: Dynamic norms drive sustainable consumption: norm-based nudging helps café customers to avoid disposable to-go-cups. J Econ Psychol 2019, 75(Part A):102146,https://doi.org/10.1016/ j.joep.2019.02.002.

48. Tankard ME, Paluck EL: Norm perception as a vehicle for social change. Soc Iss Policy Rev 2016, 10(1):181–211,https:// doi.org/10.1111/sipr.12022.

49. Prentice D, Paluck EL: Engineering social change using social norms: lessons from the study of collective action. Curr Opin Psychol 2020, 35:138–142,https://doi.org/10.1016/

j.copsyc.2020.06.012. 50

* *

. Jans L: Changing environmental behaviour from the bottom up: the formation of pro-environmental social identities. J Environ Psychol 2021, 73:101531,https://doi.org/10.1016/ j.jenvp.2020.101531.

Three studies support the propostion that bottom-up pro-environmental initiatives (i.e. formed by some regular members of a group them-selves) can strenghten pro-environmental social identity (pro-environ-mental norms and group identification) in members of the overarching group, and thereby motivate pro-environmental actions.

51. Sloot D, Jans L, Steg L: Can community energy initiatives motivate sustainable energy behaviours? the role of initiative involvement and personal pro-environmental motivation. J Environ Psychol 2018, 57:99–106,https://doi.org/10.1016/ j.jenvp.2018.06.007.

52. Asch SE: Social Psychology. Prentice-Hall; 1952.

53. Morris WN, Miller RS: The effects of consensus-breaking and consensus-preempting partners on reduction of conformity. J Exp Soc Psychol 1975, 11(3):215–223,https://doi.org/10.1016/ S0022-1031(75)80023-0.

54 * *

. Bolderdijk JW, Cornelissen, G. How do you know someone's vegan? They won't always tell you. Working paper.Accepted pending minor revisions.Using Asch’s classic conformity para-digm, the authors found that many veg*n participants refused to sign a meat-free petition after a majority of confederates refused to sign.

55. Abrahamse W, Steg L: Social influence approaches to encourage resource conservation: a meta-analysis. Global Environ Change 2013, 23(6):1773–1785,https://doi.org/10.1016/ j.gloenvcha.2013.07.029.

56. Sparkman G, Attari SZ: Credibility, communication, and climate change: how lifestyle inconsistency and do-gooder derogation impact decarbonization advocacy. Energy Res Soc Sci 2020, 59:101290,https://doi.org/10.1016/

(7)

57. Kraft-Todd GT, Bollinger B, Gillingham K, Lamp S, Rand DG: Credibility-enhancing displays promote the provision of non-normative public goods. Nature 2018, 563(7730):245–248,

https://doi.org/10.1038/s41586-018-0647-4.

58. Fowler JH, Christakis NA: Cooperative behavior cascades in human social networks. Proc Natl Acad Sci Unit States Am 2010, 107(12):5334–5338,https://doi.org/10.1073/

pnas.0913149107.

59. Hindriks F: The duty to join forces: when individuals lack control. Monist 2019, 102(2):204–220,https://doi.org/10.1093/ monist/onz006.

60. Nyborg K, Anderies JM, Dannenberg A, Lindahl T, Schill C, Schluter M, Adger WN, Arrow KJ, Barrett S, Scheffer M, et al.:

Social norms as solutions. Science 2016, 354(6308):42–43,

https://doi.org/10.1126/science.aaf8317.

61. Centola D, Becker J, Brackbill D, Baronchelli A: Experimental evidence for tipping points in social convention. Science 2018, 360(6393):1116–1119,https://doi.org/10.1126/ science.aas8827.

62. Chenoweth E, Belgioioso M: The physics of dissent and the effects of movement momentum. Nature Hum Behav 2019, 3(10):1088–1095.

63. Habyarimana J, Jack W: Results of a large-scale randomized behavior change intervention on road safety in Kenya. Proc Natl Acad Sci Unit States Am 2015, 112(34):E4661–E4670,

https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1422009112. 30 Psychology of Climate Change

Referenties

GERELATEERDE DOCUMENTEN

- Ontwikkeling van de eOverheid - Gebruik van elektronische diensten - Factoren van invloed op gebruik - Voorkeur voor kanalen.. - Een benadering vanuit bronnen en situaties

RQ2: In hoeverre verschillen nieuwsmedia van elkaar in de mate van overeenkomst bij overname via directe en indirecte route?... 10 Mogelijke verschillen tussen webmedia

The main findings of this study can be summarized as follows: (i) banks take on more exposure to liquidity risk against the background of a higher degree of central

tion targets using Tabriz, Iran, as a case study. The objectives are to; 1) quantify carbon sequestration and storage in the current urban forest, 2) elaborate scenar- ios on

Ik verwacht zeg maar dat wat je nu ziet: dat groene daken vanuit het waterbeleid ontstaaneen heel ander stuk, namelijk de leefbaarheid van de stad: wat met hitteoverlast en fijnstof

Due to the different local conditions and development approaches was the local vulnerability to climate change impacts in Rotterdam and sustainability as a societal issue in

(2018) are the first and thus far the only researches on dynamic norms (to my knowledge). The research seems promising as either the backfire effect is countered or the dynamic norm

Niet alleen thuis maar ook in de buurt is er geen beschikbaar stukje wilde na­ tuur waar zij de oerelementen van de natuur leren ervaren.. Veel onderwijsmensen