• No results found

Exploring the effectiveness of message framing and time context on alcohol warning labels

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Share "Exploring the effectiveness of message framing and time context on alcohol warning labels"

Copied!
74
0
0

Bezig met laden.... (Bekijk nu de volledige tekst)

Hele tekst

(1)

Exploring the effectiveness of message framing and time context on

alcohol warning labels

Author: Xueyao Yin

Student number: 10842152 Thesis type: Master’s Thesis

School: Graduate School of Communication Science Program: Research Master’s

Supervisor: Prof. Dr. S.J.H.M. (Bas) van den Putte Completion date: 27th June, 2017

(2)

Abstract

There was limited research on alcohol warning messages regarding message phrasing,

showing inconsistent results. In order to explore message effectiveness in the context of

alcohol container warning labels, the current study compared the effects of messages that

adopt different framing styles and different time contexts. A 2 (message framing: gain vs.

loss) × 2 (time context: short vs. long term) between-subjects experiment was conducted

among 166 young adults worldwide. The main dependent variables in this study were

attitude towards reducing alcohol drinking and intention of reducing alcohol drinking.

The result showed different findings compared to previous research. On contrary to what

has been expected, loss-framed messages worked more effectively to increase the

intention of reducing alcohol drinking. Time context had no significant effect on attitude

or intention. The interaction between time context and message framing and the

interaction between time context and future orientation did not show any significant

effect either. In addition, attitude did not mediate the effect of alcohol warning messages

on intention. Additional analysis with group comparison suggested that the path

(3)

Exploring the effectiveness of message framing and time context on

alcohol warning labels

Heavy alcohol consumption is positively correlated with many health problems

among young adults, such as physical injury, risky sexual behaviors, mental health issues,

long-term brain functioning damage, and more. (Hermens et al., 2013; World Health

Organization [WHO], 2014). These problems can have life-changing effects to young

adults, which can further lead to detrimental social and cultural standing and position

(e.g., reduced economic productivity; Courtney & Polich, 2009). Despite the negative

consequences, excessive drinking is most popular among young adults, compared to

other age groups (Johnston, O'malley, Bachman, Schulenberg, & Miech, 2015). Thus it is

urgent to reduce alcohol consumption within this group.

Countries worldwide have implemented various strategies to reduce alcohol

consumption. In previous years, research findings revealed that taxation, restriction of

availability and advertisements are the most cost-effective options to reduce

alcohol-caused death and disabilities on population level (WHO, 2014). When targeting college

students specifically, brief motivational interventions have been widely adopted to reduce

heavy drinking, however, studies showed that this strategy only had small effects on

alcohol consumption related outcomes (Huh et al., 2015). Therefore, it is necessary to

explore new approaches to target this problem. Similar to alcohol consumption, tobacco

usage is also a prevalent health threatening behavior. Regarding smoking, warning labels

(e.g., text message stating “Smoking Kills”) on tobacco packages have been used as an

effective health promotion message in many regions (Noar et al., 2016). Alcohol warning

(4)

warning messages can reduce alcohol consumption as what tobacco warning labels did to

cigarette usage. The current study aims to examine whether using warning labels on

alcohol containers can be an effective approach to reduce alcohol consumption among

young adults.

In 1989, the United States became the first country that requires mandatory

warning labels on alcohol containers (Greenfield, Graves, & Kaskutas, 1999). By 2012,

at least 31 countries required health and safety warning labels on alcohol containers,

including France, Belarus, South Africa, and Brazil (WHO, 2014) Even though

Europeans on average consume more alcohol than any other region (WHO, 2017), most

of European countries (e.g., The Netherlands and Germany) do not require warning labels

on alcohol containers. Previous research found mixed results of the effects of alcohol

warning labels on drinking behaviors. Many of the studies (Wilkinson & Room, 2009)

indeed found increased awareness of the negative effect of alcohol, however, alcohol

warning labels has little effect on behavior or even resulted in boomerang effects (Bell,

Zizzo, & Racine, 2015). So far, alcohol warning label did not raise enough attention in

the academic world and this has not been much improved since first implementation.

Thus, the current study compared different message types to test how to increase the

effectiveness of alcohol warning labels, which might further serve as evidence to

persuade more countries to require mandatory alcohol warning labels.

In order to search for more effective alcohol warning labels, two comparisons

were made in the current study. The first comparison was between different message

frames. According to framing theory (Rothman & Salovey, 1997), message receivers

(5)

Gain-framed messages emphasize the benefits of behaving in the recommended manner; while

loss-framed messages emphasize the disadvantages of not behaving in the recommended

manner. Meta-analysis on previous studies (Gallagher & Updegraff, 2012; O'Keefe &

Jensen, 2009) found that gain-framed messages, compared to loss-framed messages, had

bigger effects on encouraging prevention behaviors, but the effect size depends on the

behavior type. Until now, the effectiveness of gain- and loss- framed messages have not

been tested on alcohol warning labels. The current study made up this gap.

Apart from message framing, the current study also explored the effect of time

context. Previous studies showed that people discount future outcomes, meaning that

individuals have the tendency to consider a reward as more attractive if it comes sooner

and the tendency to consider punishment is more attractive if it comes later (Green &

Myerson, 2004). When applied to alcohol intervention, this process of temporal

discounting can influence the effect of a health message, depending on whether the

message states long-term consequences (discounted future outcomes) or short-term

consequences. Previous research did not show consistent findings on the effect of time

context on alcohol drinking behavior or related problems (Bernstein, Wood, & Erickson,

2016; Churchill, Pavey, Jessop, & Sparks, 2016; Gerend & Cullen, 2008). For example,

Gerend and Cullen (2008) found a main effect of time context, such that short-term

messages worked more effectively compared to long-term messages, while another study

(Bernstein, Wood, & Erickson, 2016) found a marginally significant effect of time

context, in which long-term messages leaded to more desired outcomes.

As mentioned above, the effect of message framing depends on specific behavior,

(6)

message has more effectiveness than loss-framed message in changing recipients’ alcohol

related outcome variables. The current study provided empirical evidence in testing if

framing theory can be applied to alcohol warning labels by comparing labels that contain

gain- and loss-framed messages. Regarding time context, there was limited research on

alcohol consumption and the results were mixed. Thus by comparing alcohol warning

labels that contain short- or long-term messages, the current study added new insights

into the discussion and helped testing the application of time context variation in alcohol

warning label settings. The two comparisons made in this study provided great value for

future alcohol warning message design because it made up the research gap of message

framing in alcohol warning labels, and provided new information to the discussion of the

effect of time context.

Theoretical Framework Message framing

Persuasive messages often present the consequences of a certain behavior in

order to motivate individuals to adopt desired behaviors or abandon undesired ones. The

same consequence can be framed in different ways by focusing on the advantages of

compliance (i.e., gain-framed messages), or by focusing on the disadvantages of

non-compliance (i.e., loss-framed messages). For example, the same underlying content can be stated as “Pursue education for a better job opportunity” or as “Dropping-out of school

makes you perform worse in the job market”. There were discussions about which type of

message is more persuasive. In health message domain, framing theory (Rothman &

Salovey, 1997) proposed that the effectiveness of message framing depends on the type

of intended behavior: for health-affirming behaviors (e.g., quit smoking to avoid lung

(7)

for illness-detecting behaviors (e.g. going to the hospital for breast cancer check),

loss-framed messages are more persuasive. When connected to the current research topic,

alcohol consumption falls into the category of health-affirming behavior because

individuals can increase their chances of staying healthy by reducing the consumption of

alcohol, Meta-analyses on framing (Gallagher & Updegraff, 2012; O'Keefe & Jensen,

2009) found that overall message framing showed significant effects on behavior change,

but they also suggested that the effectiveness of message framing depends on the specific

behavior.

O'Keefe and Jensen (2009) investigated the effect of message framing on disease

prevention behavior (e.g., alcohol related behaviors) by analyzing the general effect on

attitude, post-communication agreement, behavioral intention, and behavior, all together.

They found that gain-framed messages in general had a small advantage (r = .03) over

loss-framed messages. Focusing on specific behavioral categories, the only large and

significant effect of message framing was on dental hygiene behaviors (r = .15). Thus,

the result was misleading in a way that the advantage of gain-framed messages was

mainly in one specific behavior. Another meta-analysis (Gallagher & Updegraff, 2012) of

message framing found that message framing did not have significant effects on attitude

or intention. When examining specific behaviors, the gain-framed message advantage

was found in encouraging three prevention behaviors (i.e., physical activity, skin cancer,

and smoking; r = .01). This study did not include alcohol behavior into its analysis

neither.

Even though in general gain-framed messages showed higher effectiveness

(8)

framing depends on the specific behavior type. Thus it is necessary to conduct a study

that focuses on alcohol drinking behavior specifically, which is not included in previous

meta-analyses. The lack of alcohol related studies in previous meta-analysis is probably

because there was only limited research investigating message framing under the scope of

alcohol consumption. Kingsbury, Gibbons and Gerrard (2015) investigated the

effectiveness of gain- versus loss- framed message of health consequences on alcohol

consumption (e.g., maintaining a healthy weight) and found that gain-framed health

consequences were associated with lower heavy drinking intentions versus loss-framed

messages. Contrarily to this study and the previous meta-analyses, Yu, Ahern,

Connolly-Ahern, and Shen (2010) found that loss-framed messages were more persuasive

compared to gain-framed messages in changing college students’ intention to know more,

perception of severity, as well as their perceived fear about fetal alcohol spectrum

disorder. However, the dependent variables in this study were not necessarily linked to

behavior change. From this perspective, it is improper to fully apply the result of this

study into the current research.

There is limited evidence regarding the effect of message framing on alcohol

consumption. Previous meta-analysis showed that in general gain-framed messages work

better than loss-framed messages, even though the effect size vary per behavior type.

There was one study (Kingsbury, Gibbons, & Gerrard, 2015) about alcohol consumption

and it was in line with framing theory. Along with the previous findings, the following

(9)

Hypothesis 1: Gain-framed alcohol warning labels will be more effective in changing alcohol related outcomes such as attitude (H1.1) and intention (H1.2), compared to loss-framed alcohol warning labels.

Time context

Time context represents the occurrence time of the consequences that imbedded

in the alcohol warning labels. Different time context might influence the effectiveness of

alcohol warning labels because of temporal discounting, which is a type of psychological

distance proposed in construal-level theory (CLT; Trope & Liberman, 2010). Temporal

discounting formed the tendency that make individuals overvalue a sooner outcome

compared to a later outcome (Green, Myerson, Lichtman, Rosen, & Fry, 1996;Trope &

Liberman, 2010). CLT proposed that people build up an abstract mental construal of

distal objects to plan for the distant future. Future events (e.g., behavior outcome) with a

longer abstract distance from individuals cause less concrete thoughts and are less valued.

For example, if a reward of the same economic value is provided immediately or delayed,

the delayed reward has less present subjective value compared to the immediate reward.

In a classic example, participants have preference for an immediate gain of $100,

compared to a gain of $120 one month later (Green & Myerson, 2004). When connected

to health intervention messages, temporal discounting may influence the effectiveness of

the messages, depending on the content presenting long-term outcomes, or short-term

outcomes.

In practice, a commonly used warning label on alcohol containers includes a

general consequence, regardless of time context: “Excessive consumption of alcohol is harmful to your health” (WHO, 2014). Even though it is not often explicitly stated,

(10)

alcohol consumption among young adults can cause both short-term and long-term

consequences (Hermens et al., 2013; WHO, 2014). When applying the idea of temporal

discounting on alcohol interventions, messages stating long-term consequences will have

less effects on recipients compared to short-term consequences because the long-term

future outcome is discounted and less valued. Thus, changing the time context of the

message will influence related outcome variables. For example, “Drinking increases the possibility of getting cancer” might be treated less valued than “Drinking increases the

possibility of a car accident” and has less effect on recipients. Up to this time, no research

has investigated the effect of temporal discounting on alcohol warning labels. Thus the

second hypothesis has been made based on construal-level theory:

Hypothesis 2.1: Alcohol warning labels that include short-term consequences will be more effective in changing alcohol related outcomes, such as attitude (H2.1.1), and intention (H2.1.2), compared to alcohol warning labels that includes long-term

consequences.

Even though there was no research conducted in the context of alcohol warning

labels, the previous research of temporal discounting on other health messages might

enlighten the current research. One study showed that the effect of temporal discounting

might be influenced by the personalities of individuals, such as future orientation (Kees,

2010). Future orientation concretes the inclination of individuals to place more emphasis

on future events versus immediate events. Kees (2010) found an interaction effect of subjects’ future orientation and temporal discounting: for participants who are low in

regards to future orientation (i.e. placing more emphasis on immediate events), a

(11)

in changing participants’ concern and risk perception about healthy food choice. The

difference between long- or short-term consequence messages was not observed among

high future orientation (i.e., placing more emphasis on future events) participants. Low

future-oriented individuals may have a steeper discounting rate for a long-term

consequence because they do not take future event into account as much. For high

future-oriented individuals, the discounted rate might be much milder, which makes a short-term

and a long-term consequence remain at a similar level of importance.

Hypothesis 2.2: The effects of time context on alcohol related outcome variables are moderated by the future orientation of participants, such that for high future

orientated individuals, the effect of long-term consequence messages on outcome variables, such as attitude (H2.2.1) and intention (H2.2.2), will be stronger. Message framing and time context

Previous research on time context showed that the rate of the discounting of

choices depends on whether the outcome is positive or negative: the value of gains

discounts faster compared to the value of losses. The asymmetry of discounting rate has

been observed in hypothetical monetary and health choices (Gerend & Cullen, 2008). For

example, Hardisty and Weber (2009) found that when the participants were asked to

evaluate monetary options, the absolute value of equivalent future outcomes differ

between an immediate gain of $250 and an immediate loss of $250. An immediate gain

of $250 was as attractive as a gain of $337.50 after one year; while an immediate loss of

$250 was as much as a loss of $265 after one year. Further analysis showed that the

discounting rate of gain or loss was significantly different in monetary choices. The same

(12)

temporal discounting rate differs for gain and for loss within different subjects. They also

found that the discounting rate of gain in one domain correlated with discounting rates of

gain in other domains (i.e., monetary choices and environmental choices), indicating a

universal discounting rate across subjects. Thus it is possible to apply the previous

research of temporal discounting in other subjects to the current research interests.

There is limited research that was conducted to explore the interaction effect of

message framing and time context on alcohol related outcomes and they yielded

inconsistent results. Gerend and Cullen (2008) tested the effect of message framing (gain

versus loss) and time context on alcohol behavior among college students. The result

showed that gain-framed messages were more effective than loss-framed messages, but

only when participants received short-term consequence message. Inconsistent findings

were found later by Bernstein, Wood, and Erickson (2016). In a field experiment among

college students, no interaction effect between message framing and time context was

found. Churchill, Pavey, Jessop, and Sparks (2016) also tested the interaction effect on

drinking behaviors and did not find a significant two-way interaction between gain- and

loss-framed messages with time context. The inconsistent findings call for more

exploration in this subject. Meanwhile, all of these studies suffer from one problem that

they used the same consequences in two time context conditions but explicitly indicated

if the consequence is in long- or in short-term. For example, driving accidents were used

as a consequence in both short- and long-term conditions. Even though the manipulation

check showed that participants can correctly indicate the messages focused on the long-

or short-term consequences, this might occur because the time context was explicitly

(13)

which leaves a big question about whether they indeed manipulated participants to

believe the commonly encountered short-term consequence (e.g., driving accident) can be

a long-term drinking consequence. This defect makes the results less trustworthy.

Depending on the effectiveness of warning message and believability of the message,

participants will react differently to the experimental exposure. When believability is not

taken into consideration, the result is rather random instead of valid. For example, in a

long-term condition, driving accident showed great effectiveness but this advantage

actually comes from driving accident itself (as a short-term consequence), instead of the

manipulation message that defined driving accident as a long-term consequence. This

kind of scenario can explain the insignificant effect of time context found by Bernstein,

Wood, and Erickson (2016). Apart from these three studies, there was no research testing

the interaction effect on alcohol warning labels, which was investigated in the current

study.

Due to the lack of valid research into message framing and temporal discounting

on alcohol related outcomes, the following hypothesis has been formulated based on

research that showed asymmetric discounting rate regarding monetary and health

consequences:

Hypothesis 3: Message framing (gain- vs loss-framed message) interacts with time context (long-term or short-term consequences) in influencing alcohol warning label effectiveness on attitude (H3.1) and intention (H3.2). Gain-framed messages that include short-term consequences will lead to more desired changes.

(14)

The effect pathways: attitude as a mediator

When designing health promotion messages, it is necessary to explore the effect

mechanism to identify the key factors in influencing individual’s health behavior. Based

on the theory of planned behavior (TPB; Ajzen, 1991), an individual’s attitude can lead to

intention change, which further leads to behavior change. This model was widely tested

in health behavior settings (Albarracin, Johnson, Fishbein, & Muellerleile, 2001;

Armitage & Conner, 2001). For example, Godin and Kok (1996) conducted a

meta-analysis to test the prediction power of the theory of planned behavior on health related

behaviors, including smoking, screening behavior, eating, etc. The results showed that the

model explained an average of 41 percent of variance of the health behaviors (R2 = .41)

and attitude indeed predicted behavior through intention change.

However, the findings of Gallagher and Updegraff (2012) challenged the

commonly accepted mediators in TPB model. Inconsistent to TPB, Gallagher and

Updegraff (2012) found that message framing do not influence attitude and intention,

instead it has a direct effect on behavior. They proposed that gain-framed messages may

convey information that changes outcome expectations, which might further change

behavior more directly than attitude and intention. However according to TPB, if

message framing indeed influences the outcome expectations, it will eventually lead to

attitude change, intention change and then behavior change. Apart from the previous

insignificant findings, another study (Kingsbury, Gibbons, & Gerrar, 2015) found

message framing indeed has effect on drinking intention, indicating a direct effect of

message framing on intention. The inconsistency of theory and research findings leaves

(15)

important to test possible models that include message effect on attitude, as well as on

intention to test how does message influence attitude and intention, and to test the

mediation role of attitude.

Both intention and attitude are two elements in the TPB model, in which attitude

predicts intention together with perceived behavioral control and injunctive norms. In

order to test the causality between attitude and intention, all the other predictors of

intention were included in the conceptual model (Figure 1) to control their effects on

intention. Apart from perceived behavioral control and injunctive norm, descriptive norm

was taken into account because it was shown to be a valid predictor of intention (Rivis &

Sheeran, 2003). In a meta-analysis (Rivis & Sheeran, 2003), the researchers found that

descriptive norm increases five percent more of the explained variance of intention, after

including attitude, perceived behavioral control and injunctive norm. Descriptive norm

refers to the attitudes and behaviors of significant others, which provides information that

individuals might use in making decisions (Rivis & Sheeran, 2003). It is different from

injunctive norm (Hagger & Chatzisarantis, 2005), which refers to the approval of

important others about the conduction of a certain behavior. Thus after including

descriptive norm in the analysis, perceived behavioral control, descriptive norm and

injunctive norm were all taken as control variables.

Because the previous findings were mixed, the following research question was

made to be answered in the current study.

Research question: Does attitude mediate the effect of message framing, time context, future orientation and their interaction terms on intention?

(16)

The conceptual model and hypothesis are presented in Figure 1: main effects of

message framing are indicated by H1.1 and H1.2; main effects of time context are

indicated by H2.1.1 and H2.1.2; interaction effects of message framing and time context

are indicated by H3.1 and H3.2; while interaction effects of time context and future

orientation are indicated by H2.2.1 and H2.2.2. Attitude is located in the model as a

mediator between message type and intention.

Figure 1. Conceptual model and hypothesis

Methods Participants and Design

A total of 194 participants were recruited from personal network, in which 7 were

excluded from the data analysis due to age restriction (i.e., only young adults between the

age of eighteen to forty were kept in the data analysis); and further 21 participants were

excluded because they indicated zero alcohol consumption. In total, the data of 166

participants (M age = 25.70 years, SD = 3.22; 60.24% female) were included in the

analysis. Most of the participants are highly educated (e.g., 91.2% of participants are Message Framing gain/loss Time Context short/long-term Attitude Intention Future Orientation H2.2.1 H1.1 H3.1 H2.1.1 H1.2 H2.1.2 H2.2.2 H3.2

(17)

currently following or have completed a Bachelor’s Degree or a higher degree).

Regarding nationality, there were 47 different nationalities held by the participants,

among which the major nationalities were Dutch (19.3%) and Chinese (8.4%).

A 2 (time context: long-term versus short-term messages) x 2 (message framing:

gain- or loss- framed messages) between subjects design was adopted in the current

study. Participants were randomly assigned into four conditions and were exposed to different warning labels. At the end, participants’ attitude, intention, perceived behavioral

control, subjective norm, descriptive norm, and future orientation were measured.

Procedure

Participants received a link to an online survey that was built in Qualtrics. They

were firstly presented with an informed consent page. In the first section of the survey,

participants answered demographic questions, including age, gender, education level,

nationality, and alcohol consumption amount. They were also asked if they prefer beer or

wine, or if they do not drink alcohol. If one type of alcohol is chosen, the following

alcohol bottles will match the preferred drinking type. If they identified as non-alcohol

drinkers, they were thanked and dismissed from the questionnaire. The participants were

then randomly assigned to four experimental conditions and were exposed to

corresponding messages on alcohol warning labels either on beer bottles or on wine

bottles (for examples of experimental material, see Figure 2 and Figure 3).

In each condition, four alcohol bottles with warning messages were presented.

After each bottle, personal relevance was measured. At the end of the exposure,

participants were asked to rank the four messages about how proper they are to be shown

(18)

four messages. In the next section, the dependent variables were measured, including

attitude and intention. Other variables, such as perceived behavioral control, injunctive

norm, descriptive norm, and future orientation were measured for testing the conceptual

model. At the end of the questionnaire, four alcohol warning messages were presented

again. Two manipulation questions were asked after each of the alcohol messages, to test

whether the participant identified the message as short/long framed, or as gain/loss

focused.

Figure 2. Example beer bottle. Figure 3. Example wine bottle.

Experimental Materials

Elicitation. Sixteen students (M age = 24.50, SD = 1.59; 68.8% women) were asked to fill in a self-report form (see Appendix A) about the alcohol drinking related

short-term or long-term consequences. Consequences mentioned in these self-reports, as

(19)

(Churchill, Pavey, Jessop, & Sparks, 2016; Gerend & Cullen, 2008) and a study that

developed cancer warning statements for alcoholic beverages (Pettigrew et al., 2014),

were selected and used in 40 messages for the pre-test (see Appendix B).

Pretest. A total of 36 students (M age = 24.50, SD = 1.67; 66.7% women) were recruited for pretesting experimental materials. Participants in the pretest were asked to

fill in the characteristics of the 20 messages (either 20 gain-framed messages; either 20

loss-framed messages) regarding their valence, gain- or loss-frame, long-term or

short-term consequence focus, and believability (see Appendix C). The order of pretested

messages were randomized. At the end, four pairs of health consequences were chosen

based on a match of their absolute mean grade. Each pair contained one short-term

consequence and one long-term consequence. These four pairs fulfilled the requirement

of manipulation, and they had a non-significant difference on valence (both p > .287) and

believability (both p > .160) (see Appendix D). Based on these eight consequences,

sixteen messages (see Table 1) were framed as either gain or loss.

Table 1 Alcohol warning labels in the questionnaire.

Gain-frame Loss- frame

Short-term - Drinking less alcohol decreases your risk of getting into driving accidents

- Drinking less alcohol decreases your risk of getting alcohol poisoning

- Drinking less alcohol decreases your risk of getting blackouts

- Drinking less alcohol decreases your risk of getting impaired judgment

- Drinking more alcohol increases your risk of getting into driving accidents - Drinking more alcohol increases your risk of getting alcohol poisoning

- Drinking more alcohol increases your risk of getting blackouts

- Drinking more alcohol increases your risk of getting impaired judgment Long-term - Drinking less alcohol decreases your risk

of getting organ problems

- Drinking less alcohol decreases your risk of getting brain damage

- Drinking less alcohol decreases your risk of heart diseases

- Drinking more alcohol increases your risk of getting organ problems

- Drinking more alcohol increases your risk of getting brain damage

- Drinking more alcohol increases your risk of heart disease

(20)

- Drinking less alcohol decreases your risk of high blood pressure

- Drinking more alcohol increases your risk of high blood pressure

Measurements

Attitude. Attitude was measured by six questions (adjusted from Keer, van den Putte & Neijens, 2012): “Do you think reducing alcohol drinking is”: 1 = unpleasant to 7

= pleasant, 1 = valuable to 7 = worthless (reverse coded); 1 = nasty to 7 = nice; 1 =

useless to 7 = useful; 1 = beneficial to 7 = harmful (reverse coded); 1 = enjoyable to 7 = not enjoyable (reverse coded). A factor analysis showed that they load on one factor, and the reliability test yielded satisfactory result (M = 3.37, SD = .81, α = .92).

Intention. Intention was measured by an agreement level on three statements (based on Mollen, Engelen, Kessels, & van den Putte, 2016): “I am willing to reduce alcohol consumption”, “I intend to reduce alcohol consumption”, and “I will try to reduce

alcohol consumption”. Answers were made on a scale from 1 = strongly disagree to 7 =

strongly agree. Factor analysis showed that they load on one factor, and reliability test yielded satisfactory result (M = 3.57, SD = 1.58, α = .82).

Perceived behavioral control. Perceived behavioral control was measured by four questions: “I am confident that I can reduce my alcohol consumption”; “Reducing

my alcohol consumption is completely up to me”; “For me, reducing alcohol

consumption is easy” and “It is impossible for me to reduce my alcohol consumption”

(reverse coded). Answers were made on a scale from 1 = strongly disagree to 7 =

strongly agree. A factor analysis showed that two factors have an eigenvalue bigger than 1 and the Cronbach’s Alpha is low (α = .55). As suggested by the result, the reversed

(21)

from the scale and it yielded one factor and a good reliability (M = 5.72, SD = 1.05, α

= .75).

Social norms. Social norms were measured in two aspects: descriptive norm and injunctive norm (Hendriks, de Bruijn, Meehan, & van den Putte, 2016). The injunctive

norm was measured by three questions (e.g., “Most of the people who are important to me would accept it, if I drink less alcohol”) with answers ranging from 1 = strongly

disagree to 7 = strongly agree. Factor analysis showed that they load on one factor, and reliability test yielded satisfactory result (M = 4.28, SD = 1.25, α = .71). Descriptive norm was measured by one question, “Among all the people who are important to you, how

many of them drink regularly?”, with answers ranging from 1 = about 0%/none of them

to 7 = about 100%/all of them.

Future orientation. Future orientation was measured by five five-point Likert Scale questions (Strathman, Gleicher, Boninger, & Edwards, 1994). Example statements are: “I spend very little time thinking about how things might be in the future” and “I

would rather save my money for a rainy day than spend it right away on something fun”.

Answers are ranged from 1 = definitely false to 7 = definitely true. Factor analysis

showed that two factors have an eigenvalue bigger than 1 and the Cronbach’s Alpha is low (α = .60). As suggested by the result, one item (i.e., “I would rather save my money

for a rainy day than spend it right away on something fun”) was deleted from the scale

and it yielded one factor and good reliability result (M = 3.43, SD = .84, α = .71).

(22)

Analytical plan

Structural equation modelling (SEM) was used to test the hypothesis and to

answer the research question. Specifically, a path model was tested in IBM- SPSS Amos

21. The path model was chosen because it provides an effective way to model various

multiple regressions that contain direct effects, indirect effects and moderation effects

(Lei & Wu, 2007). As illustrated in Figure 1, the exogenous variables included the

message framing, time context, their interaction; as well as future orientation, and the

interaction of future orientation and time context. These variables were expected to have

an influence on attitude, which further influences intention. Meanwhile, intention was

influenced by perceived behavioral control, injunctive norm and descriptive norm. Before

testing the SEM model, all the variables were standardized in order to prevent

multicollinearity. After standardization, interaction term (i.e., time context × message

framing; time context × future orientation) were made and standardized for SEM

analysis.

Several control variables (i.e., personal relevance, and alcohol consumption) were

tested to check if they have association with attitude and intention. For example, a t-test

was conducted to test if participants in the beer or wine condition held a different level of

attitude towards reducing alcohol consumption. If this is the case, then alcohol type

would be included in the analysis by adding a path from alcohol type to attitude.

After constructing the SEM model, the first step was to achieve an acceptable

model fit. AMOS software reports several types of goodness-of-fit indices. Normally

higher values of CFI and lower values of RMSEA represent good model fit (Lei & Wu,

(23)

smaller than .95 and RMSEA should not be bigger than .06. After achieving a good

model fit, specific parameters were examined to test the hypothesis. If the conceptual

model and hypothesis (Figure 1) were true, the corresponding parameters in the SEM

model should all be significant. Besides, bootstrapping with a 95% confidence interval

was adopted to estimate the direct effect, indirect effect and total effect of message

characteristics (e.g., message framing and time context) on intention. If attitude is a

mediator as in Hypothesis 4, the indirect effect will be significant.

Results Randomization

A one-way ANOVA was conducted to test whether participants were randomly

assigned over experimental conditions. The result showed that there was no difference

between the four conditions on age (p = .458). Chi-square tests were conducted to check

the distribution of gender and education level. The result showed that gender (p = .313)

and education (p = .196) were not significantly different across conditions.

Manipulation check

A t-test was conducted with the short- versus long-term condition as the

independent variable. The dependent variable was the manipulation question “Do you

think the alcohol warning message on this bottle focuses on a short-term consequence or

a long-term consequence of alcohol drinking?”. The answer ranged from 1 = short-term

to 7 = long-term. Because each participant was exposed to four consequences and was

therefore asked four times this question, the average was taken as an overall

representative. The four experimental conditions were recoded into two conditions

(24)

expected, there was a significant different in the scores in the short-term conditions (M =

2.83, SD = 1.27) and long-term conditions (M = 5.50, SD = 1.42); t (164) = 12.80, p

< .001.

Two t-tests were conducted to check if the message framing was successfully

manipulated by taking message frame conditions as the independent variable. The four

experimental conditions were recoded into two conditions (gain-framed vs. loss-framed)

by combining short-term conditions and long-term conditions. The dependent variable

was the manipulation question “This alcohol warning label focuses on 1 = the advantage

of drinking less alcohol or 0 = the disadvantage of drinking more alcohol.” Because each participant was exposed to four consequences and was therefore asked four times this

question, the average was taken as an overall representative. As expected, there was a

significant difference in the scores in the gain condition (M = .62, SD = .40) and loss

condition (M =.10, SD = .24); t (136.31) = -10.05, p < .001.

Control Variables

Personal Relevance. Pearson’s correlation analysis showed that personal relevance has a significant correlation with attitude (r = .16, p <. 05) and intention (r

= .31, p < .001), thus it was included as a control variable in the analysis.

Alcohol Consumption. Pearson’s correlation analysis showed that alcohol

consumption was not significantly correlated with attitude (r = -.01, p = .919) or intention

(r = .05, p =. 500), thus it was not included as a control variable.

Alcohol Type. Participants in the same condition were either exposed to beer bottles or wine bottles based on their preference. In order to dispel this concern, two

(25)

intentions regarding reducing alcohol consumption. According to the results, the mean

score of attitude (p = .726) and intention (p = .818) did not significantly differ between

alcohol container types. Thus the alcohol type was not taken into consideration in the

further analysis.

Main Analyses

The descriptive statistics (Table 2) showed an overview of the measured

variables. In general, the mean scores of attitude (M = 3.37, SD = .81) and intention (M =

3.57, SD = 1.58) were on a median level compared to the measurement range. The

average perceived behavioral control (M = 5.72, SD = 1.05) was high compared to the

maximum range of 7, indicating participants believe they have good control of decreasing

alcohol consumption if they intend to. Personal relevance scored in the lower half of the

range (M = 2.94, SD = 1.47), which means that participants perceive the consequences

mentioned on the alcohol warning labels as not very relevant to themselves. Injunctive

norm (M = 4.28, SD = 1.25), descriptive (M = 4.36, SD = 1.54) norm and future

orientation (M = 3.43, SD = .84) all have mean scores that were on a medium level.

Table 2 Descriptive statistics.

Mean SD Minimum Maximum

attitude 3.37 .81 1 5

intention 3.57 1.58 1 7

perceived behavioral control 5.72 1.05 1 7 injunctive norm 4.28 1.25 1 7 descriptive norm 4.36 1.54 1 7 personal relevance 2.94 1.47 1 7 future orientation 3.43 .84 1.25 5

While evaluating the SEM model, the Variance Inflation Factor (VIF) statistic

(26)

to 1, meaning there was no multicollinearity. Besides, because all the questions were

forced-answer questions, there was no missing value in the dataset, which means the

standardization was not influenced by the listwise deletion in AMOS. The zero-order

correlations between variables were presented in Table 3.

The AMOS output indicated that the data violated the criteria of multivariate

normality, the multivariate kurtosis is 8.146, which is over the critical value of 6. AMOS

spotted several outliers in the data, which have Mahalanobis distance with p values

greater than .05. After examining the data, there was no legitimate reason to drop these

cases, thus they were kept in the data analysis.

Table 3 Zero-order correlations between variables.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 1.attitude — 2.intention .47** — 3.perceived behavioral control .36** 0.07 — 4.injunctive norm .27** .43** .14 — 5.descriptive norm 0.01 -0.12 -.07 -.10 — 6.personal relevance .16* .31** -.07 .25** .23** — 7.future orientation 0.04 -.16* .15 -.05 -.08 -.18* — 8.time context -.02 .09 .01 .07 -.12 -.06 .04 — 9.message framing -0.06 -.13 .02 -.01 .02 .07 -.03 .02 — 10. interaction of 8 and 9 -0.05 -.08 .13 .02 -.03 -.01 .04 -.00 -.00 — 11.interaction of 7 and 8 -0.05 -.01 -.08 -.08 -.09 .02 .08 -.01 .04 -.03 — Note 1. * p < .05; ** p < .01.

Note 2. Message framing: gain frame was coded as 1, loss frame was coded as 0; time context: short-term was coded as 1, long-term was coded as 0.

The SEM model was constructed as showed in Figure 4. All the exogenous

variables were allowed to correlate freely. The model did not yield a satisfactory model fit (χ²(24) = 38.51, p < .05, χ2/df =.62, CFI = .88, RMSEA = .06, 90% CI [.02, .10]). The

(27)

control, and between attitude and injunctive norm. This suggestion makes sense because

these three variables are all predictors of intention according to TPB model. Thus the

covariance was added between the error terms of attitude and the other two variables.

Besides, because descriptive norm was also suggested as an additional predictor of

intention, the error term of descriptive norm was also allowed to covariate with the error

term of attitude. After this step, the model yielded a satisfactory model fit: χ²(21) = 7.93,

p = .995, χ2/df = .38, CFI = 1.00, RMSEA = .00, 90% CI [.00, .00].

The estimates of the SEM model showed that personal relevance was the only

significant indicator of attitude (b* = .18, p < .05). Thus Hypothesis 1.1, Hypothesis

2.1.1, Hypothesis 2.2.1 and Hypothesis 3.1 were all rejected. The results of structural

equation modelling was presented in Figure 4.

Regarding intention, gain- and loss-framed condition variable showed a

marginally significant negative effect on intention (b* = -.11, p = .057), indicating

gain-framed message (coded as 1) decreased intention of reducing alcohol consumption

compared to loss-framed message, thus Hypothesis 1.2 is rejected. All the other

experimental conditions or interaction terms did not yield significant result, thus

Hypothesis 2.1.2, Hypothesis 2.2.2 and Hypothesis 3.2 were all rejected.

Bootstrap with Bias-corrected percentile method showed that there was no

significant indirect effect that is mediated by attitude (all p > .18), thus the research

question was answered: attitude does not mediate the effect of message type on drinking

intention.

Injunctive norm (b* = .26, p < .001), attitude (b* = .39, p < .001), descriptive

(28)

(b* = -.13, p < .05) all significantly predicted intention. The direction of effect is along

with what has been expected for injunctive norm (i.e., the more important others support

reducing alcohol consumption, the higher the intention of reducing alcohol consumption),

for descriptive norm (i.e., the more people drinking in the surroundings, the less intention

to reduce drinking) and for attitude (i.e., higher attitude towards reducing alcohol

consumption predicts higher intention towards reducing alcohol consumption).

Figure 4. Estimations in structural equation model.

*The numbers on arrow represents standardized regression estimate. * p < .05; ** p < .01; ***p < .001; † 0.1 < p < .05. .39*** Message framing gain/loss short/long-term consequences Attitude Intention Interaction of long/short term consequence and future orientation Perceived Behavioral Control Injunctive Norm Interaction of gain/loss framing and

long/short term consequence Descriptive norm Personal Relevance Future Orientation -.11† -.08 -.07 -.05 -.03 .08 -.06 .05 . 03 -.13* -.15** .26*** -.07 .20** .18**

(29)

Additional Analyses

There are some other variables which might influence the conceptual model. For

example, Kingsbury, Gibbons and Gerrard (2015) suggested that a previous level of

drinking can moderate the effect of message type on drinking related outcome variables.

An additional analysis was conducted by using current drinking behavior as grouping

variable in AMOS. With different current consumption levels, participants might react

differently to the alcohol warning label. For example, light drinkers might believe there is

no reason to drink less because they are already in a low risk group. Meanwhile, people

who drink more alcohol might be influenced by the message in a greater extent because

the message make them aware of the health problems. After a median split (median of

behavior was 5 glasses of alcohol per typical week), the current drinking amount of

participants was used to group participants.

The model yielded a satisfactory model fit: χ²(42) = 43.79, p = .395, χ2/df = 1.04,

CFI = .99, RMSEA = .02, 90% CI [.00, .06], which means the model could be applied for

both groups. The result (Table 4) showed that the lower half (group 1) and higher half

participants (group 2) indeed react differently to the alcohol warning label messages. For

the group 1, gain-framed messages leads to less intention of reducing alcohol (b*= -.15, p

< .05), which has the same direction as showed in the main analysis without grouping

participants. However, this effect is not significant for the group 2. For group 2, regarding

time context, messages with short-term consequences showed advantages in changing participant’s intention of reducing alcohol (b*= .21, p < .05): the short-term condition has

higher intention to reduce alcohol consumption, which support hypothesis 2.1.2. The

(30)

(b*= -.22, p < .01), indicating gain-framed (coded as 1) and short-term (coded as 1)

message combination or loss-framed and long-term message combination leads to less

intention of reducing alcohol consumption. Considering the advantage of short-term

messages, short-term and loss-frame combination should be preferred because they lead

to more intention of reducing alcohol drinking. Besides, for both groups, attitude did not

mediate any indirect effect between message type and intention, which was along with

the previous main analysis.

Table 4. Estimations in SEM model, grouping by current alcohol consumption.

group 1 (lower half) N = 93 group 2 (higher half) N = 73

Path Estimate S.E. Estimate S.E.

attitude <--- message framing (gain) -.00 .10 -.10 .10 attitude <--- time context (short) -.00 .10 .02 .10 attitude <--- interaction of message framing and time context -.10 .10 -.03 .10 attitude <--- interaction of future orientation and time context -.08 .11 -.07 .11 attitude <--- future orientation .19† .11 -.05† .11 attitude <--- personal relevance .19† .11 .27* .12 intention <--- message framing (gain) -.15* .08 -.07 .09 intention <--- time context (short) -.02 .08 .21* .09 intention <--- interaction of message framing and time context .05 .08 -.22** .09 intention <--- interaction of future orientation and time context .13 .08 .10 .09 intention <--- future orientation -.09 .08 -.21* .09

intention <--- attitude .43*** .08 .34** .11

intention <--- injunctive norm .31*** .08 .23* .10 intention <--- descriptive norm -.22** .06 -.06 .10 intention <--- perceived behavioral control -.21* .10 .11 .10 intention <--- personal relevance .22* .07 .21* .10

Note. † 0.1 < p < .05; * p < .05; ** p < .01; ***p < .001.

In the next step, all the path parameters in the model were set to be the same

across two groups to test the equality of parameters. After the constraining, the model yielded good model fit: χ²(58) = 62.44, p = .322, χ2/df = 1.08, CFI = .97, RMSEA = .02,

(31)

90% CI [.00, .05]. The chi-square difference test did not yield a significant effect, (△χ²=

18.65, △df = 16, p = .287), thus the simpler model (model with equal path parameters

constrain) should be preferred. This result indicates that the model with free estimation of

parameters is not significantly better than the model with equal path parameter

constrains.

Conclusion and Discussion Message framing and time context

The first aim of this study was to investigate the effect of different alcohol

warning label types on alcohol related outcomes. It was expected that alcohol warning

labels with gain-framed health consequences that state the advantage of decreasing

alcohol consumption would lead to more desired changes, compared to labels with

loss-framed health consequences that state the disadvantage of increasing alcohol

consumption. It was also expected that messages with short-term health consequences

that state what might happen after alcohol consumption in a short time period would be

more effective, compared to messages with long-term health consequences that state what

might happen in a longer period. This difference was expected to be stronger when the

message is framed as a gain instead of a loss. Meanwhile, future orientation was expected

to moderate the effect of time context (i.e., short-term versus long-term context), the

advantage of messages with short-term consequences would be bigger if the individual

has a low future orientation.

The result showed that framing the message as gain or loss did not have a

significant effect on attitude. Contrary to what has been expected, gain-framed messages,

(32)

intention. Thus Hypothesis 1 was rejected. This finding is against framing theory

(Rothman & Salovey, 1997), which states that for health-affirming behaviors,

gain-framed messages worked more effectively than loss-gain-framed messages. It challenged the

framing theory by showing that the application of framing theory into the context of

alcohol warning labels should be carefully examined. Previous research (O'Keefe &

Jensen, 2009) showed that the effect of message framing depends on different behaviors,

and the current research showed that a loss-framed message has advantage in the alcohol

warning label context. Besides, the current analysis challenged the findings in a previous

meta-analysis (Gallagher and Updegraff, 2012), in which the researchers found that

message framing in general did not have any effect on attitude or intention. The

difference between the current research and this meta-analysis also reflects the fact that

the application of message framing should vary per health behavior. Practitioners should

keep this in mind when designing health messages that targeting different behaviors.

The additional analysis of the conceptual model showed that there’s no significant

difference between the model with group comparison and without group comparison,

indicating that current drinking behavior does not moderate the model paths. However,

when applying the same model to two groups of participants (i.e., higher half or lower

half regarding their drinking amount), the effect sizes and significant levels differ

between two groups. The significant effect of message framing was only found among

participants whose alcohol consumption was in the lower half, not in the higher half. The

result indicates that current drinking behavior might moderate the effect of message

framing on drinking intention. Most of participants in the current sample do not have

(33)

made in the analysis might not capture the difference between participants with two level

of drinking. This might be the reason of non-significant model comparison. Further

analysis could take this into account and recruit more heavy drinkers to make a better

comparison between heavy drinkers and light drinkers. Practitioners should consider their

target audience when designing health promotion messages. The differences between

heavy users and light users can also be applied to other health behavior, such as smoking,

drug usage, etc. Further research about communicating health promoting message can

take current usage level as a moderator in the model.

Apart from message framing, this study also manipulated time context of the

alcohol warning label messages. Contrary to what has been expected, short-term or

long-term consequences of alcohol consumption did not show any significant effect on attitude

and intention, neither did the interaction between message framing and time context, and

the interaction between time context and future orientation. Thus Hypothesis 2.1, 2.2 and

Hypothesis 3 were all rejected. This finding was consistent with what had been found by

Churchill, Pavey, Jessop, and Sparks (2016) and Bernstein, Wood and Erickson (2016).

Thus the current research showed that manipulating time context may not be an effective

way in changing alcohol related outcome variables.

Even though the multi-group comparison in additional analysis showed that

current drinking behavior does not moderate the model paths, the path coefficients in the

model and their significant levels are different between the two groups when the model

was tested separately. As mentioned before, the non-significant model improvement

might be induced by the low percentage of heavy drinker in the sample. The findings

(34)

the higher half group. This research finding raises attention of the role of current drinking

behavior. Because the alcohol warning messages mainly intend to encourage people to

drink in moderately, the heavy drinkers might be taken as the main focus of this strategy.

Attitude as a mediator

The second aim of this study was to test the mediation role of attitude. After

controlling for other TPB variables, attitude indeed positively predicted intention as

expected, namely a positive attitude towards reducing drinking will lead to a higher

intention of reducing alcohol consumption. However, due to the fact that message type

did not have any effect on attitude, the effect of message type on intention was not

mediated by attitude.

As presented in the result section, attitude towards reducing alcohol consumption

significantly predicted intention of reducing alcohol consumption and there was a strong

correlation between these two variables. In both of the main and additional analysis,

message types showed significant effects on intention but not on attitude. This indicates

that health messages do not necessarily need to change attitude in order to change

intention of a certain behavior. Intention of decreasing alcohol consumption could be an

outcome of cognitive thinking produced directly by being exposed to persuasive

messages. Attitude of decreasing alcohol consumption might be an outcome of all

previous alcohol related experience and it is hard to be changed by a manipulation in a

one-time experiment. Even though attitude is an important predictor of intention

according to TPB model, promoting attitude change does not need to be the priority in

health communication. If the health message has a desired effect on intention, the effect

(35)

By adding personal relevance as a control variable, this study also found that

personal relevance has a positive effect on intention and attitude, which means if the

participant perceived the negative health consequence as relevant to himself or herself,

meanwhile s/he will have a more positive attitude and a stronger intention towards

reducing alcohol consumption. Because this variable was not manipulated in the current

study, it is difficult to make a causal conclusion based on cross-sectional data. Thus

future research could investigate the effect of personal relevance on attitude and intention

by manipulating this variable. As suggested by this study, the alcohol warning messages

should include consequences that are more relevant to the young adults group. In this

way, the message can further lead to less positive attitude towards drinking and weaker

drinking intention.

Overall evaluations

The participants of this study were reached through a personal network, which

brought several advantages as well as limitations to the study. The research budget and

time restriction made it beyond the researcher’s ability to conduct a random sampling

process, thus the data was not collected with random sampling methods and it might not

be representative of the whole population. However, due to the fact that the participants

were from around 50 nationalities, the current study is more universally applicable

compared to previous studies of alcohol consumption. After all, most of previous studies

were conducted among western participants in a certain university or in a certain area.

Participants in the current study had more mixed backgrounds, which could be the reason

(36)

research could further explore this possibility by conducting cross-culture studies to test

the differences between people from various backgrounds.

Compared to previous research on the effectiveness of alcohol warning messages,

the current study produced less biased research findings because a pre-test was conducted

to select proper experimental material. Messages on alcohol warning labels in this study

were selected based on their believability and valence. In this way, messages in different

experimental conditions on average have similar believability and valance scores, which

ruled out the possibility that these two issues caused the change in intention or attitude.

Apart from benefitting the current study results, the alcohol warning messages can also

benefit future studies by providing several validated study material.

There are also some limitations in the current study. Firstly, the present findings

were based on message manipulation in some two-dimensional alcohol containers. The

effect of alcohol warning message might be different in a realistic situation when the

participants have a real alcohol bottle in hand. The intention of drinking might be more

intensely triggered when participants are facing actual alcohol bottles. Future research

could further explore the effect of message framing by conducting the experiment in a

more realistic situation. Secondly, other potential moderators of message framing were

not tested in the current research. For example, self-esteem (e.g., Covey, 2014); and

perceived risk (e.g., Quick & Bates, 2010). Future research can extend the current study

by testing the effect of moderators of the alcohol warning messages, which could help

with designing more effective and tailored alcohol warning messages. The third

limitation of this study is related to the features of the alcohol warning label. Unlike

(37)

to mass audience once they are released to the market. The current study only

manipulated message framing and time context of the message. A significant effect was

found of future orientation on intention, indicating that personalities play an important

role in formulating intention towards drinking less alcohol. Tailored health promotion

messages can put more emphasis on low future orientated individuals because they tend

to have less intention towards stop drinking. However in practice, this variable is less

relevant for alcohol warning label design because the warning labels are designed for the

whole population. Even though high future oriented people showed less intention of

drinking alcohol, it is impossible to specifically target these group when launching

alcohol warning label messages. Future research can explore more variables that can be

manipulated in warning messages, thus the effective ones could be adopted in alcohol

warning label design. The fourth limitation is that this study is a cross-sectional study

without measuring behavior change of the participants. Previous research showed that

there is a gap between intention and behavior (Sheeran, 2002), thus further research

should take this into account and measure behavior change that could be induced by

alcohol warning labels.

Overall, the current study explored the effect of message framing and time

context on the attitude and intention of reducing alcohol consumption in the context of

alcohol warning label, and it also tested the mediation role of attitude. The results showed

that loss-framed alcohol warning labels have marginally significant advantages compared

to gain-framed messages in changing participant’s intention of drinking. However, the

advantage was only showed among people who drink less alcohol. The additional

(38)

changing the intention among people who drink more alcohol. This study provided

evidence that alcohol warning label could lead to desired effects on participants’ intention

(39)

References

Ajzen, I. (1991). The theory of planned behavior. Organizational Behavior and Human

Decision Processes, 50, 179-211.

Albarracin, D., Johnson, B. T., Fishbein, M., & Muellerleile, P. A. (2001). Theories of

reasoned action and planned behavior as models of condom use: A meta

analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 127, 142-146.

Armitage, C. J., & Conner, M. (2001). Efficacy of the theory of planned behaviour: A

meta‐analytic review. British Journal of Social Psychology, 40, 471-499.

Bell, E., Zizzo, N., & Racine, E. (2015). Caution! Warning labels about alcohol and

pregnancy: Unintended consequences and questionable effectiveness. The

American Journal of Bioethics, 15(3), 18-20.

Bernstein, M. H., Wood, M. D., & Erickson, L. R. (2016). The effectiveness of message

framing and temporal context on college student alcohol use and problems: A

selective e-mail intervention. Alcohol and Alcoholism, 51, 106-116.

Churchill, S., Pavey, L., Jessop, D., & Sparks, P. (2016). Persuading people to drink

less alcohol: The role of message framing, temporal focus and

autonomy. Alcohol and Alcoholism, 51, 727-733.

Courtney, K. E., & Polich, J. (2009). Binge drinking in young adults: Data, definitions,

and determinants. Psychological Bulletin, 135, 142-156.

Covey, J. (2014). The role of dispositional factors in moderating message framing

effects. Health Psychology, 33, 52-65.

Gallagher, K. M., & Updegraff, J. A. (2012). Health message framing effects on attitudes,

(40)

Medicine, 43, 101-116.

Gerend, M. A., & Cullen, M. (2008). Effects of message framing and temporal context on

college student drinking behavior. Journal of Experimental Social

Psychology, 44, 1167-1173.

Godin, G., & Kok, G. (1996). The theory of planned behavior: A review of its

applications to health-related behaviors. American Journal of Health

Promotion, 11, 87-98.

Green, L., & Myerson, J. (2004). A discounting framework for choice with delayed and

probabilistic rewards. Psychological Bulletin, 130, 769-792.

Green, L., Myerson, J., Lichtman, D., Rosen, S., & Fry, A. (1996). Temporal discounting

in choice between delayed rewards: The role of age and income. Psychology and

Aging, 11, 79-84.

Greenfield, T. K., Graves, K. L., & Kaskutas, L. A. (1999). Long-term effects of alcohol

warning labels: Findings from a comparison of the United States and Ontario,

Canada. Psychology and Marketing, 16, 261-282.

Hardisty, D. J., & Weber, E. U. (2009). Discounting future green: Money versus the

environment. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 138, 329-340.

Hagger, M. S., & Chatzisarantis, N. L. (2005). First- and higher- order models of

attitudes, normative influence, and perceived behavioural control in the theory of

planned behaviour. British Journal of Social Psychology, 44, 513-535.

Hendriks, H., de Bruijn, G. J., Meehan, O., & van den Putte, B. (2016). Online and

offline conversations about alcohol: Comparing the effects of familiar and

(41)

Hermens, D. F., Lagopoulos, J., Tobias-Webb, J., De Regt, T., Dore, G., Juckes, L., ...

Hickie, I. B. (2013). Pathways to alcohol-induced brain impairment in

young people: A review. Cortex, 49, 3-17.

Hu, L. T., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure

analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation

Modeling, 6, 1-55.

Huh, D., Mun, E. Y., Larimer, M. E., White, H. R., Ray, A. E., Rhew, I. C., … Atkins, D.

C. (2015). Brief motivational interventions for college student drinking may not

be as powerful as we think: An individual participant level data meta‐

analysis. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 39, 919-931.

Johnston, L. D., O'malley, P. M., Bachman, J. G., Schulenberg, J. E., & Miech, R.A.

(2015). Monitoring the future national survey results on drug use, 1975-2015.

Volume II, College Students & Adults Ages 19-55. Institute for Social Research,

112, 279-287

Keer, M., van den Putte, B., & Neijens, P. (2012). The interplay between affect and

theory of planned behavior variables. American Journal of Health Behavior, 36,

107-115.

Kees, J. (2010). Temporal framing in health advertising: The role of risk and future

orientation. Journal of Current Issues & Research in Advertising, 32, 33-46.

Kingsbury, J. H., Gibbons, F. X., & Gerrard, M. (2015). The effects of social and health

consequence framing on heavy drinking intentions among college

students. British Journal of Health Psychology, 20, 212-220.

Referenties

GERELATEERDE DOCUMENTEN

The main purpose of this study was to answer the following question: “What is the influence of positive and negative message framing in an advertisement on online purchase

Voor geen van beide onderwerpen werd een significant effect van message framing op message engagement gevonden, maar de teksten waarin gebruik werd gemaakt van gain-framing bleken

Het mythische verhaal is in zekere zin net zo’n alibi, maar deze minimale verhalen zijn door de schrijver zelf bedacht of ontleend aan zijn persoonlijke belevenissen.. Het gevaar

The MCDA model Clinical trial data Approximation Patient preferences Uncertainty Uncertainty Preference?. studies

Neem een schoon washandje en maak het nat met stromend water uit de kraan (niet uit een teil).. Geen

- -Future research: using a neutral image in a color that is not already associated with nature and pro-environmentally friendly products and nature imagery.

Using nature imagery in advertisements will, therefore, lead to a more positive brand and product attitude due to the easy processing, which might indirectly lead to an

• H1: A message in a negative frame will result in higher donation intentions than a message in a positive frame • H2: Information specificity.. moderates the negative effect of