• No results found

The effectiveness of combining multiple online communication tools : synergy and the role of sequence

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Share "The effectiveness of combining multiple online communication tools : synergy and the role of sequence"

Copied!
50
0
0

Bezig met laden.... (Bekijk nu de volledige tekst)

Hele tekst

(1)

M

ASTER'S

T

HESIS

The effectiveness of combining multiple

online communication tools

Synergy and the role of sequence

Marieke Kooiman

Student ID: 10172165

University of Amsterdam

Graduate School of Communication

Research Master’s programme Communication Science

Supervisor: Hilde Voorveld

(2)

1

ABSTRACT

The aim of the current study is to examine synergy in combining multiple online

communication tools (i.e., display advertising and content-driven advertising). Additionally,

the role of sequence in affecting brand evaluation is analysed for sequential exposure and

simultaneous exposure to these online communication tools. Sequence effects are explored

and three underlying mechanisms are proposed in an attempt to explain these effects.

Memory performance is proposed as underlying mechanism for differences between

sequential exposure and simultaneous exposure. Persuasion knowledge and source credibility

are proposed as underlying mechanisms for differences between sequential exposure and the

reverse order. An experiment was conducted with exposure to a banner and sponsored blog,

either sequentially (banner – sponsored blog vs. sponsored blog – banner) or simultaneously.

Results provide evidence for synergy and sequence effects, although sequence effects are not

found within the sequential exposure format. Findings indicate greater effectiveness for the

combination of banners and sponsored blogs in a sequential exposure format rather than a

simultaneous exposure format. Sequence effects could however not be explained by the

(3)

2

SYNERGY AND THE ROLE OF SEQUENCE

“The future of digital advertising is being built on content; that’s what’s going to fund this

medium in the future,” according to O’Brien, senior manager at U.K.’s Internet Advertising

Bureau (Southern, 2015). Although content-driven advertising is gaining ground, display

advertising is still the most popular form of digital advertising with banner ads as most

commonly used (IAB NL, 2015).

Content-driven advertising is one of the many labels to define (paid) advertising that

blends with the specific form and appearance of editorial content (Robinson, 2015; Sternberg,

2013; Wojdynski & Evans, 2016). Ad blockers and progressive awareness of banners'

ineffectiveness stimulate this shift toward content-driven advertising. One important form of

advertising integrated in editorial content are so-called sponsored blogs; bloggers writing paid posts about a brand’s services and products. Sponsored blogs have become and are

expected to persist a popular venue in the digital advertising market (Jaekel, 2016; Sehl,

2013).

The promising influence of content-driven advertising along with increasing spends in

display advertising (IAB NL, 2015) is important for this study. It often occurs that a

combination of display advertising (e.g., banners) and content-driven advertising (e.g.,

sponsored blogs) is embedded in campaigns (Van den Berg, 2015). Exposure to these

different communication tools may elicit brand experiences that result in more positive brand

evaluations than the effects of repeated exposure to one single communication tool. This

assumption stems from previous literature about synergy (Chang & Thorson, 2004; Dijkstra,

Buijtels, & Van Raaij, 2005; Naik & Raman, 2003; Tang, Newton, & Wang (2007). Synergy

is defined as the greater effect compared to the sum of the individual effects. Although

synergy is examined primarily with multiple communication channels (i.e., cross-media), few

(4)

3 (e.g., advertising, publicity, content, direct marketing) (Jin, 2003; Stammerjohan, Wood,

Chang, & Thorson, 2005). However, synergy effects are not proven in a combination of

display advertising and content-driven advertising despite its increasing use in online

campaigns. Therefore, the first aim of the current study is to extend synergy research to the

specific context of banners and sponsored blogs.

Although synergy leads to more positive outcomes, these effects could be

strengthened or weakened by the sequence of exposing multiple communication tools. Prior

research assigns importance to study the role of sequential and simultaneous exposure in

investigating synergy effects (Assael, 2011; Bronner, 2006; Neijens & Voorveld, 2015), but

studies about these sequence effects remain limited, either in cross-media research (e.g.,

Voorveld, Neijens, & Smit, 2012) as multiple-tool research (e.g., Loda & Coleman, 2005).

However, it is becoming increasingly essential to gain insight into sequence effects in online

campaigns because improved marketing techniques make it possible to target and retarget

people with brand messages in a specific sequence (Lambrecht & Tucker, 2013). The second

aim therefore is to examine effects on brand evaluation of sequential exposure to a banner

and sponsored blog (i.e., exposure to different webpages) and simultaneous exposure to a

banner and sponsored blog (i.e., exposure to one webpage).

Furthermore, processes underlying sequence effects will be considered as there is no

knowledge to date about such mechanisms. First, memory performance will be examined as

underlying mechanism for differences between sequential exposure and simultaneous

exposure. In earlier studies memory performance has been related to both sequential

exposure, by explaining synergy processes improving memory performance (e.g., Voorveld,

Neijens, & Smit, 2011), as well as simultaneous exposure, by explaining limited cognitive

resources declining memory performance (e.g., Lang, 2000). Second, persuasion knowledge

(5)

4 sequential exposure and the reverse order (banner – sponsored blog vs. sponsored blog –

banner). Earlier studies suggest the importance of both mechanisms by showing that blog

performance in particular depends largely on perceptions of persuasive intent and credibility

(Colliander & Dahlén, 2011; Eisend & Küster, 2010). Thus, the third aim of the current study

is to provide a deeper understanding of the role of sequence effects on brand evaluation by

examining three underlying mechanisms. Memory performance is proposed to explain a

greater predicted effectiveness for sequential exposure compared to simultaneous exposure.

Persuasion knowledge and source credibility are proposed to explain a greater predicted

effectiveness for the sequential exposure sponsored blog – banner compared to the sequential

exposure banner – sponsored blog.

THEORETICAL FRAMEWORK Synergy

Although synergy is already demonstrated in combining multiple communication tools (Jin,

2003; Micu & Pentina, 2015; Stammerjohan et al., 2005; Wang, 2006), it is not yet studied in

the online context of banners and sponsored blogs. Studies that explain processes underlying

synergy are discussed to argue for synergy effects in this study.

Backward retrieval is one such synergy process that is explained in cross-media

research (Edell & Keller, 1989; Neijens & Voorveld, 2015). It suggests that information

retrieval from exposure to a first communication tool is positively affected during exposure to

a second communication tool. People store information in memory after seeing the first tool

and retrieve this stored information when seeing the second tool as this tool differs from the

first tool. Retrieval does not take place, or to a lesser extent, when people are repeatedly

exposed to a brand message in the same tool. Then, people are more inclined to process the

message again. Thus, backward retrieval occurs during exposure to multiple communication

(6)

5 After exposure to multiple communication tools a brand message is stored in memory in a

more complex way, resulting in a strong, clear and accessible information network. This

process is known as encoding variability (Stammerjohan et al., 2005; Voorveld et al., 2011;

Young & Bellezza, 1982). Such an enhanced network is even more likely to occur when

communication tools have different presentation modalities, as is true for banners and blogs.

Specifically, banners are highly visual whereas blogs mainly comprise verbal information

(Micu & Pentina, 2015; Voorveld, et al., 2012). Eventually, the information network resulting

from the complex encoding process increases accessibility of information in memory.

Thus, researchers argue that improved retrieval ability and information accessibility

ensure a better evaluation of the advertised brand and, thus, explain the greater effectiveness

of exposure to multiple communication tools (i.e., synergy) compared to exposure to a single

communication tool repeatedly. To examine whether synergy is achieved in combining

banners and sponsored blogs and replicate prior findings from other contexts, the following

hypothesis is formulated:

H1: Exposure to a banner and sponsored blog will result in a more positive brand

evaluation than either (a) exposure to two times a banner or (b) exposure to

two times a sponsored blog.

Earlier empirical research on the effectiveness of sequence

Differential effects in brand evaluation as a result of sequence (i.e., sequence effects) are

found for multiple media channels (Fortin, Grimwood, & Finsterwalder, 2011; Voorveld et

al., 2012) as well as multiple communication tools (Jin, Zhao, & An, 2006; Loda & Coleman,

2005; Micu & Pentina, 2015).

Studies about sequential exposure with multiple communication tools generally show

that a better effectiveness is achieved with a sequence in which publicity is followed by

(7)

6 public relations efforts. Sponsored blogs could be the result of such efforts as these are being

made towards bloggers to cooperate and develop blog posts.

Empirical studies about simultaneous exposure are conducted in relation to media

multitasking and context effects (Chowdhury, Finn, & Olsen, 2007; Jeong & Hwang, 2012;

Segev, Wang, & Fernandes, 2014; Voorveld, 2011). From multitasking research it appears

that simultaneity challenges people’s information processing activity and, thereby, reducing

positive outcomes. However, disagreement persists about the effectiveness of simultaneity in

contextual effect research depending on the degree of congruency between the ad and the

surrounded content.

This short overview demonstrates that sequence effects on brand evaluation have to

be considered in both sequential as simultaneous exposure to multiple communication tools.

However, it remains unclear what processes underlie the differential effects caused by

sequence. Therefore, three underlying mechanisms are discussed in an attempt to

theoretically explain sequence effects.

Underlying mechanisms for the effectiveness of sequence

Memory performance

Memory performance is the first proposed underlying mechanism for explaining differential

effects between sequential and simultaneous exposure.

Synergy suggests the effectiveness of exposure to multiple communication tools

through enhanced retrieval ability (i.e., backward retrieval) and information accessibility (i.e.,

encoding variability) resulting in improved memory performance. It is this improved memory

performance that might be more plausible in sequential exposure rather than simultaneous

exposure. In simultaneous exposure, memory performance can be hindered by distraction

effects and limited cognitive resources (Bergen, Grimes, & Potter, 2006; Chowdhury et al.,

(8)

7 states that people in simultaneous exposure have to divide attention between sources to

understand and process information. Partial attention results in more difficulty to store

information and retrieve it from memory. Consequently, cognitive responses are reduced for

both simultaneously exposed sources. For sequential exposure, it is expected that the

processes of backward retrieval and encoding variability are more powerful (Voorveld et al.,

2011). People confronted with exposures that follow one another are less likely to be

distracted and are not forced to divide attention between multiple sources of the same brand

message. Brand name and brand propositions are therefore better stored in memory.

However, one could argue that distraction also occurs in sequential exposure as

banners are always shown in conjunction with other content (Chowdhury et al., 2007).

Nevertheless, it is argued that improved memory performance in sequential exposure prevails

this distraction effect because higher levels of information retrieval are found in sequential

exposure instead of simultaneous exposure (Janssens, De Pelsmacker, & Geuens, 2012). In

addition, sequential exposure to a banner with unrelated content is less likely to distract

people from the brand message than simultaneous exposure to a banner with related content

(i.e., a sponsored blog). In turn, the improved memory performance resulted from sequential

exposure, influences brand evaluation in a positive way (Keller, 1987; Voorveld et al., 2011).

Specifically, people are more likely to remember the brand and its advertised propositions

and are therefore more likely to evaluate the brand with regard to those propositions.

To examine whether sequential exposure benefit more from memory performance

than simultaneous exposure, with positive evaluations as a result, the following hypothesis is

proposed:

H2: (a) Sequential exposure to a banner and sponsored blog will result in better

memory performance than simultaneous exposure, which (b) positively affects

(9)

8

Persuasion knowledge

A second possible underlying mechanism for explaining differential effects between

sequential exposure and the reverse order is persuasion knowledge. Persuasion knowledge refers to people’s personal knowledge about goals and tactics of persuaders and about the

skills of how to deal with these goals and tactics. People need to be aware of persuasion

attempts before they can activate persuasion knowledge (i.e., Persuasion Knowledge Model)

(Friestad & Wright, 1994). Persuasion knowledge can result in more distrust toward the

content and can stimulate the use of resistance mechanisms to cope with the persuasive

intent.

In the field of brand placements (i.e., brand integration in media programming),

studies show differences in activation of persuasion knowledge between prominent and subtle

placements. Prominent placements (e.g., plot connection, frequent mentioning) are

considered as having more obvious persuasive goals than subtle placements (e.g., no plot

connection, limited mentioning). Consequently, prominent placements stimulate the

activation of persuasion knowledge more than subtle placements (Cowley & Barron, 2008;

Dens, De Pelsmacker, Wouters, & Purnawirawan, 2012; Van Reijmersdal, 2009). The

presence of differences in activation through different levels of prominence can also be

argued in the context of this study. Banners are comparable to prominent placements because

persuasive goals are obvious and sponsored blogs are comparable to subtle placements

because persuasive goals are covered. Persuasive goals in blogs are often covered by

informational or entertaining purposes (Hsu & Lin, 2008; Huang, Chou, & Lin, 2008). In

other words, people exposed to a banner are more likely to be aware of the persuasive attempt

and more inclined to use their persuasion knowledge while the opposite holds true for people

(10)

9 Although prominent and subtle brand placements are merely contrasted and not examined in

subsequent exposures, it could be argued that different sequential exposures make persuasion

knowledge more or less activated. In the sequential exposure banner – sponsored blog, the

initial exposure to a banner could prime the persuasive intent of the brand’s message making

persuasion knowledge more likely to be activated (Cowley & Barron, 2008). Activation in an

initial exposure to the message helps people recognize the persuasive intent of the sponsored

blog in a subsequent exposure. As a result of activated knowledge of persuasion tactics,

evaluations of the brand are negatively influenced (Boerman, Van Reijmersdal, & Neijens,

2012; Wei, Fisher, & Main, 2008). In the sequential exposure sponsored blog – banner,

persuasion knowledge is however less likely to be already activated in the initial exposure because of a blog’s subtle or covered persuasive intent. Persuasion knowledge could be

activated in the subsequent exposure to a banner but is expected to be less pronounced than

activation in exposure to a banner preceding a blog, as people are primed in that case.

Thus, the following hypothesis is proposed to examine sequential exposure effects

with persuasion knowledge as explaining factor:

H3: (a) Exposure to a sponsored blog followed by a banner will result in a less

pronounced activation of persuasion knowledge than exposure to a banner

followed by a sponsored blog, which (b) positively affects brand evaluation.

Source credibility

If persuasion knowledge is activated people are able to evaluate the source and that is when

source credibility comes into play (Boerman et al., 2012). Source credibility is the third

underlying mechanism proposed to explain differential effects between sequential exposure

and the reverse order.

Since banners and blogs are part of a fast and transitory online environment, people

(11)

10 (Choi & Rifon, 2002; Metzger, Flanagin, & Medders, 2010). Source credibility is one such

heuristic cue. In other words, banners and blogs are evaluated based on perceptions of

credibility. Generally, publicity (e.g., blogs) is perceived as more credible than advertising

(e.g., banners) (Johnson & Kaye, 2004; Kim, Yoon, & Lee, 2010; Micu & Pentina, 2015).

Taking this into account, exposure to a blog results in greater perceived source credibility.

Since the blog is trusted, the information is integrated into people’s thoughts and attitudes

(Stammerjohan et al., 2005). This suggests that source credibility generated from initial

exposure to a blog is more likely to retain during subsequent exposure to a banner. In turn,

trusted information resulting from credible sources such as blogs is seen as more persuasive

in terms of positive brand evaluations (Pornpitakpan, 2004). An opposite effect is expected

for the sequential exposure with a banner as initial exposure because information is less

trusted and not integrated in memory in such a way. Credibility perceptions in this particular

sequential exposure will therefore decline (Loda & Coleman, 2005).

The following hypothesis is proposed to examine the explaining role of source

credibility in sequential exposure effects:

H4: (a) Exposure to a sponsored blog followed by a banner will result in higher

levels of source credibility than exposure to a banner followed by a sponsored

blog, which (b) positively affects brand evaluation.

FIGURE 1. The three hypothesized underlying mechanisms for sequence effects1

1

H1ab is examined as verification of synergy and is therefore not included in this model.

Sequential exposure o banner – blog o blog - banner Simultaneous exposure Persuasion knowledge Source credibility Memory performance H2a H2b H3a H4a H3b H4b Brand evaluation

(12)

11

METHOD Design and participants

In this study an experiment is used to test synergy effects in combining banners and

sponsored blogs and, more importantly, to test the effectiveness of sequential and

simultaneous exposure to both communication tools. The experiment had a single factor

between-subjects design. Specifically, five conditions were employed with the following

exposures: (1) banner – sponsored blog, (2) sponsored blog – banner, (3) banner and

sponsored blog, (4) banner – banner, and (5) sponsored blog – sponsored blog. The final two

conditions served as baseline conditions to verify synergy effects.

A total of 183 participants (67.2% female) took part in this experiment with an

average age of 27.3 years (SD = 10.82). The participants were recruited via online

networking (i.e., convenience sample) and were sent an URL link that led them to the online

questionnaire. The majority of the participants was Dutch (84.7%) and had a University Bachelor’s degree (36.1%) or Master’s degree (37.7%). Participation was voluntary.

Procedure

The experiment was conducted online. First, participants were provided with minimal

required information about the study and were asked for their voluntary participation (i.e.,

informed consent). After permission, participants were told that they were being confronted

with one or more webpages and were asked to read and view the content in a conventional

manner. Participants were randomly assigned then to one of five experimental conditions.

They were exposed to twice a webpage in the sequential exposure conditions. In those

conditions with multiple communication tools, participants were either exposed to a banner

followed by a sponsored blog or a sponsored blog followed by a banner. Participants assigned

to those conditions with a single communication tool were either exposed to a banner

(13)

12 condition the banner was slightly changed the second time (i.e., minor text change) and in the

blog condition another blog post was used the second time. These changes have been made to

expose participants to varied stimuli in all sequential exposure conditions. Because banners

are always presented in combination with other content (Chowdhury et al., 2007), filler

content was included whenever participants were exposed to a banner. This filler content

comprised a news article or a page view of a weather forecast to create a natural online

viewing environment. In the simultaneous exposure condition participants were exposed to

one single webpage including a banner and sponsored blog.

Afterwards, questions were asked to measure evaluations (i.e., attitude toward the

banner, attitude toward the blog, brand attitude, and purchase intention). In addition, memory

performance, persuasion knowledge, and source credibility were measured to examine their

potential as underlying mechanisms. Finally, some general (demographic) questions were

asked and participants were debriefed and thanked for their participation.

Stimuli material

Fictitious banners and sponsored blogs were designed for a relatively new and therefore

unknown brand in the category renewable energy. The brand ‘Bio-bean’ was chosen as

renewable energy provider because it was assumed that participants had no preexisting

attitudes toward this brand that could confound results (Campbell & Keller, 2003).

Nevertheless, prior knowledge was measured to control for this. In addition, this brand was chosen to broaden the scope of the study’s population. Since people make use of energy

sources daily, the brand was assumed to be relevant for everyone.

The banners were created from text and image derived from the brand’s website. The brand’s core business, an additional brand proposition, and the name of the brand were used

as text elements. The blogs were a combination of authentic lifestyle blogs (i.e.,

(14)

13 presented Bio-bean as renewable energy provider and explained its core business as

responsible and sustainable. The brand name was mentioned a few times. The blog’s

formatting was based on regular formatting of blogs: headline, name of the blogger,

sponsorship disclosure, and most recent date above the text (Kaplan & Haenlein, 2010).

Measures

Dependent variables

Attitude toward the banner. Attitude toward the banner was measured by asking participants

to indicate how they evaluated the banner. Participants had to answer this question on a

three-item 7-point semantic differential scale (not very likeable – very likeable / bad – good / not

appealing – appealing) (based on Chang & Thorson, 2004; Voorveld et al., 2012). The items

loaded on one factor which proved to be reliable (EV = 2.53; R² = 84; α = .91; M = 4.04, SD

= 1.31).

Attitude toward the blog. Attitude toward the blog was measured with the same three items as

attitude toward the banner. All items loaded on one factor (EV = 2.60; R² = 87; α = .92; M =

4.54, SD = 1.15).

Brand attitude. Brand attitude was measured on a six-item 7-point semantic differential scale

by asking participants to indicate how they evaluated the brand (based on Boerman et al.,

2012; Campbell, 1995). The following items were included: ‘dislike’ – ‘like’, ‘unfavorable’ – ‘favorable’, ‘unpleasant’ – ‘pleasant’, ‘negative’ – ‘positive’, ‘poor quality’ – ‘high quality’,

and ‘bad’ – ‘good’. Items loaded on one factor (EV = 4.83; R² = 81; α = .95; M = 5.17, SD =

1.16).

Purchase intention. Purchase intention was measured with the following statements ranging

on a scale from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree): ‘I will look for Bio-bean online’,

(15)

14 provider’, and ‘I intent to use Bio-bean as energy provider’ (EV = 3.17; R² = 79; α = .91; M =

3.27, SD = 1.43) (based on Spears & Singh, 2004).

Underlying mechanisms

Memory performance. Memory performance was measured with three questions. First,

participants were asked to recall the name of the brand (i.e., brand recall). Participants were

then asked to write down everything they remembered about the brand and the related

messages (banner and/or blog) (i.e., correctly recalled elements). The number of correctly

recalled elements was used to assess the strength of participants’ memory performance.

Furthermore, it was measured to which extent participants were certain of the fact that they

had noticed specific brand propositions (i.e., certainty brand propositions). A four-item

7-point scale was used ranging from 1 (very uncertain) to 7 (very certain) (based on Dijkstra et

al., 2005). The items contained two propositions that were mentioned and two propositions

that were not mentioned (mentioned propositions: r = .18; M = 4.68, SD = 1.38).

Persuasion knowledge. Persuasion knowledge was measured with two items corresponding

the conceptual dimension of persuasion knowledge (Boerman et al., 2012). Participants were

asked to indicate to what extent they thought the banner was commercial and to what extent

they thought the banner was advertising, on a scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7

(strongly agree) (r = .82; M = 5.14, SD = 1.44). The same items were asked for the blog (r =

.86; M = 4.49, SD = 1.36) (based on Van Reijmersdal et al., 2015).

Source credibility. Source credibility was measured by asking participants to rate the

credibility of the source (banner and/or blog) on a 7-point semantic differential scale with the following six items: ‘untrained’ – ‘trained’, ‘not open minded’ – ‘open minded’, ‘no expert’ –

‘expert’, ‘bad’ – ‘good’, ‘not experienced’ – ‘experienced’, and ‘not trustworthy’ –

(16)

15 The items loaded on one reliable factor (banner: EV = 3.93; R² = 66; α = .89; M = 4.62, SD =

.99; blog: EV = 3.89; R² = 65; α = .89; M = 4.74, SD = .91).

Control variables

Prior knowledge and product involvement were included as control measures because these

might have been correlated with the dependent variables (Edell & Keller, 1989; Voorveld et

al., 2012).

Prior knowledge. Prior knowledge was measured to determine whether participants had

preexisting knowledge or attitudes toward the banner, blog, and brand. Participants were

asked to indicate to what extent they agreed with the following two statements on a 7-point Likert scale (1 = strongly disagree, 7 = strongly agree): ‘I have never seen the banner of

Bio-bean before’ and ‘The banner of Bio-Bio-bean was new to me’ (based on Chang & Thorson,

2004). The same two statements were used to determine participants’ prior knowledge of the

blog and brand. All statements loaded on one reliable factor (reversed: EV = 4.70; R² = 78; α

= .94; M = 1.45, SD = .85).

Product involvement. Product involvement was measured by asking participants to indicate

how they thought about renewable energy (i.e., Bio-bean’s product). A two-item 7-point

semantic differential scale was used (very uninteresting – very interesting / very unimportant – very important) (r = .85; M = 5.19, SD = 1.44) (based on McQuarrie & Munson, 1992).

RESULTS Data analysis procedure

Prior to data analysis, the data set was screened for missing values, outliers, and erroneous

values. Respondents with missing values due to dropout were removed from the data set. Few

(17)

16 containing useful information (Orr, Sacket, & Dubois, 1991). Skewness was reduced by using

log transformations.2

The experimental conditions did not differ with respect to gender (χ²(4, N = 183) =

1.27, p = .866), age (F(4, 178) = 0.95, p = .438), nationality (χ²(56) = 67.04, p = .148), and education (χ²(24) = 26.49, p = .329). So, it was concluded that demographics were equally

distributed across conditions.

To prevent that results would be falsely attributed to the experimental conditions, it

was checked whether the dependent evaluative measures were correlated with demographic

variables and control variables. No correlations were found with demographic variables

(except for nationality3). However, prior knowledge was significantly correlated with brand

attitude (r = -0.18) and product involvement with all evaluative measures (attitude banner: r =

-0.27, attitude blog: r = -0.42, brand attitude: r = -0.55, purchase intention: r = -0.44).

It was also checked whether groups could be combined to one multiple-tool condition

and one single-tool condition to give a more comprehensive overview of the results for H1

and H2. No differences were found between the multiple-tool conditions with regard to the

evaluative measures (x1: p = .772; x2: p = .574; x3: p = .312; x4: p = .671) and underlying

mechanisms (y1: p = .820; y2: p = .185; y3: p = .691; y4: p = .946; y5: p = .496; y6: p = .626; y7:

p = .903).4 However, the single-tool conditions differed from each other (x3: p = .001; y2: p <

.001; y3: p = .004). Therefore, the multiple-tool conditions were combined into one sequential

2

Log transformations were conducted for prior knowledge and product involvement.

3 Although correlations between nationality and evaluative measures were indicated (see table 1 in Appendix A),

nationality was not included as covariate since it was already tested to be of similar influence across conditions.

4

x1 = attitude banner; x2 = attitude blog; x3 = brand attitude; x4 = purchase intention; y1 = brand recall; y2 =

correctly recalled elements; y3 = certainty brand propositions; y4 = persuasion knowledge banner; y5 =

(18)

17 exposure condition for testing H1 and H2 whereas the single-tool conditions (i.e., banner

condition and blog condition) were treated separately in testing H1.

Synergy in combining banners and sponsored blogs

The study’s first hypothesis stated that exposure to a banner and sponsored blog

(multiple-tool condition) will result in a more positive brand evaluation than either exposure to two

times a banner or two times a sponsored blog (single-tool condition). To test whether brand

evaluation is more positive in the multiple-tool condition, a univariate analysis of covariance

(ANCOVA) was conducted with the conditions as independent variables and evaluative

measures as dependent variables. This analysis showed that there were no significant

differences with regard to attitude toward the banner (F(1, 105) = .927, p = .338), attitude

toward the blog (F(1, 107) = .21, p = .647), and purchase intention (F(2, 143) = 1.48, p =

.231).5 However, the analysis revealed a significant overall difference for brand attitude

between conditions (F(2, 143) = 13.13, p < .001). The post-hoc Bonferroni test showed that

there was a significant difference between the multiple-tool condition, Mbanner&blog = 5.56, SD

= 0.64, and the single-tool condition with banners, Mbanners = 4.28, SD = 1.41, but not between

the multiple-tool condition and the single-tool condition with sponsored blogs (Mblogs = 5.38,

SD = 1.34) (see table 2 in Appendix A). Thus, participants who were exposed to a banner and

sponsored blog evaluated the brand more positively than participants who were exposed to

two times a banner.

In sum, participants did not differ in attitude toward the exposed communication

tools. However, participants did have a more positive attitude toward the brand in the

multiple-tool condition than in the single-tool condition with banners. In other words,

5

The degrees of freedom for the F-tests differed since attitude toward the banner is measured in the banner condition only and attitude toward the blog is measured in the blog condition only.

(19)

18 synergy was demonstrated in combining banners and sponsored blogs in terms of brand

attitude. Therefore, H1a is partially supported and H1b is not supported.

Role of memory performance in sequential and simultaneous exposure

The second hypothesis proposed that sequential exposure to a banner and sponsored blog will

result in better memory performance than simultaneous exposure, which positively affects

brand evaluation. Memory performance as underlying mechanism was tested with the

mediator model format of Hayes’ PROCESS macro (2013). PROCESS uses an ordinary least

squares or logistic regression-based path analytic framework for estimating the direct and

indirect effect in a mediator model. Model 4 of this macro allows to uncover whether the

mediator (i.e., memory performance) played a role in the differential effect of sequential and

simultaneous exposure on brand evaluation. Bootstrap confidence intervals included in the

PROCESS allow one to make inferences regarding these indirect effects. The current study

used 1,000 bootstrap samples for the bias-corrected bootstrap confidence intervals (BCBCIs).

So, the macro was conducted with the sequential and simultaneous exposure

conditions as independent variable, memory performance as mediator, and evaluative

measures as dependent variables. Since memory performance was measured by means of

several variables (i.e., brand recall, correctly recalled elements, certainty brand propositions),

the analysis will be discussed for each variable separately. However, before focusing on

memory performance as underlying mechanism, direct effects of exposure are discussed.

As expected, the PROCESS macro showed a significant difference for brand attitude

between the sequential exposure condition and simultaneous exposure condition (b = -0.36,

SE = 0.15, p = .016, 95% BCBCI [-0.65, -0.07]). Participants who were exposed to multiple

tools in a sequential exposure format evaluated the brand more positively (M = 5.56, SD =

0.64) than participants who were exposed to multiple tools in a simultaneous exposure format

(20)

19 found (attitude banner = 0.29, SE = 0.23, p = .221, 95% BCBCI [-0.17, 0.75]; attitude blog =

0.18, SE = 0.21, p = .384, 95% BCBCI [-0.23, 0.60]; purchase intention = 0.11, SE = 0.25, p

= .657, 95% BCBCI [-0.39, 0.62]).

Brand recall. The first measure of memory performance, brand recall, could not be tested in

Hayes’ PROCESS macro as it was a binary variable. Thus, both paths which allow for

mediation were examined individually. To test the effect of exposure on brand recall, a

logistic regression was conducted. Subsequently, an ANCOVA was used to test the other

path; the effect of brand recall on evaluative measures.

The logistic regression analysis showed a significant difference between the

sequential exposure condition and simultaneous exposure condition, Wald χ²(1) = 4.84, p =

.028. The result was however in conflict with what was hypothesized. The probability of

recalling the name of the brand was significantly lower in the sequential exposure condition

(34.7%) than in the simultaneous exposure condition (54.3%). To see whether the higher

recall resulted in a more positive brand evaluation, an ANCOVA was conducted. The

analysis revealed no significant differences. Brand evaluation of participants who did recall

the brand was not different from brand evaluation of participants who did not recall the brand

(attitude banner: F(1, 103) = 0.07, p = .793, attitude blog: F(1, 103) = 1.08, p = .302, brand

attitude: F(1, 103) = 0.362, p = .549, purchase intention: F(1, 103) = 1.23, p = .271).

Correctly recalled elements. The amount of correctly recalled elements was the second

measure for memory performance. In contrast to what was expected, the mediator models

showed no significant effects on attitude toward the banner (indirect effect = 0.01, SE = 0.04,

95% BCBCI [-0.04, 0.16]), attitude toward the blog (indirect effect = 0.00, SE = 0.03, 95%

BCBCI [-0.07, 0.06]), brand attitude (indirect effect = 0.00, SE = 0.02, 95% BCBCI [-0.04,

(21)

20

Certainty brand propositions. Third, the certainty with which participants indicated to have

noticed the brand propositions was examined. Again, no significant effects were

demonstrated for the evaluative measures: attitude toward the banner (indirect effect = 0.01,

SE = 0.03, 95% BCBCI [-0.03, 0.11]), attitude toward the blog (indirect effect = -0.01, SE =

0.04, 95% BCBCI [-0.15, 0.03]), brand attitude (indirect effect = -0.01, SE = 0.03, 95%

BCBCI [-0.10, 0.02]), and purchase intention (indirect effect = 0.00, SE = 0.03, 95% BCBCI

[-0.05, 0.07]).

Summing up, the results demonstrated that the role of memory performance is not

considered a mediating role in the effect of exposure on brand evaluation. H2 is therefore not

supported. However, there is some support, in terms of brand attitude, for the assumption that

sequential exposure to multiple communication tools is more effective than simultaneous

exposure to these tools.

Role of persuasion knowledge in sequential exposure

The third hypothesis stated that sequential exposure to a sponsored blog followed by a banner

will result in a less pronounced activation of persuasion knowledge than sequential exposure

to a banner followed by a sponsored blog, which positively affects brand evaluation. This

hypothesis was tested using Hayes’ PROCESS macro again, in which persuasion knowledge

toward the banner and persuasion knowledge toward the blog were treated as mediators. In

conflict with the hypothesis, the models indicated no significant effects on evaluative

measures. In addition to these insignificant mediation effects, no differential direct effects of

exposure sequence were found (see table 3). In other words, the sequence in which

participants were exposed to a banner and sponsored blog was irrespective of how they

evaluated the brand afterwards. This means that examination of underlying mechanisms was

(22)

21 TABLE 3. Results of Hayes’ PROCESS mediation model via persuasion knowledge

Y b S.E. (Boot)LLCI (Boot)ULCI

Attitude banner Directᵇ 0.08 0.28 0.779 -0.49 0.64

Indirectᵇ Banner 0.01 0.06 -0.09 0.18

Blog 0.00 0.05 -0.08 0.14

Attitude blog Directᵇ -0.17 0.22 0.456 -0.61 0.28

Indirectᵇ Banner 0.02 0.11 -0.17 0.26

Blog 0.03 0.05 -0.04 0.16

Brand attitude Directᵇ -0.15 0.13 0.262 -0.42 0.12

Indirectᵇ Banner 0.00 0.02 -0.03 0.07

Blog 0.01 0.02 -0.02 0.08

Purchase intention Directᵇ -0.16 0.30 0.594 -0.76 0.44

Indirectᵇ Banner 0.00 0.04 -0.05 0.12

Blog 0.01 0.05 -0.05 0.19

Note: Y = evaluative measures.

ª Significance level: * p < .05, ** p < .01, *** p < .001.

ᵇ Both direct sequence effects and indirect sequence effects via persuasion knowledge of the banner and persuasion knowledge of the blog are reported.

Role of source credibility in sequential exposure

The final hypothesis predicted that exposure to a sponsored blog followed by a banner will

result in higher levels of source credibility than exposure to a banner followed by a sponsored

blog, which positively affects brand evaluation. The same analysis (i.e., PROCESS) was

conducted with source credibility toward the banner and source credibility toward the blog,

each included as mediator. The analysis showed that there were no significant effects for

(23)

22 knowledge it became already clear that direct effects were not found either. Brand evaluation

was thus similar across conditions. Therefore, H4 is also not supported.

TABLE 4. Results of Hayes’ PROCESS mediation model via source credibility

Y b S.E. (Boot)LLCI (Boot)ULCI

Attitude banner Directᵇ 0.18 0.25 0.476 -0.32 0.68

Indirectᵇ Banner -0.09 0.17 -0.49 0.17

Blog 0.00 0.04 -0.12 0.07

Attitude blog Directᵇ -0.15 0.22 0.491 -0.59 0.28

Indirectᵇ Banner 0.02 0.04 -0.03 0.14

Blog 0.01 0.12 -0.20 0.27

Brand attitude Directᵇ -0.12 0.13 0.333 -0.38 0.13

Indirectᵇ Banner -0.02 0.04 -0.13 0.03

Blog 0.00 0.03 -0.05 0.09

Purchase intention Directᵇ -0.12 0.29 0.680 -0.70 0.46

Indirectᵇ Banner -0.03 0.08 -0.30 0.07

Blog 0.00 0.06 -0.09 0.16

Note: Y = evaluative measures.

ª Significance level: * p < .05, ** p < .01, *** p < .001.

ᵇ Both direct sequence effects and indirect sequence effects via source credibility of the banner and source credibility of the blog are reported.

DISCUSSION

The current study proposes that synergy is the result of exposure to multiple communication

tools in the context of banners and sponsored blogs. Specifically, synergy is determined by

means of evaluative measures, being attitude toward the banner, attitude toward the blog,

brand attitude, and purchase intention. The results from this study validate the belief about

synergy; exposure to multiple communication tools is more effective than exposure to a

(24)

23 Next, the study proposes sequence effects for exposure to multiple communication tools.

Effects of sequential exposure are compared to simultaneous exposure and effects of the

sequential exposure banner – sponsored blog are compared to the sequential exposure

sponsored blog – banner. Findings from the first comparison indicate that exposure to

multiple communication tools is more effective when people are exposed to these tools

sequentially rather than simultaneously. For the second comparison, results imply no

differences in effectiveness. Exposure to a sponsored blog followed by a banner or exposure

to a banner followed by a sponsored blog is considered equally effective.

Finally, underlying mechanisms are proposed to understand the role of sequence in

exposure to multiple communication tools. Since sequence effects within the sequential

exposure format (banner – sponsored blog vs. sponsored blog – banner) are not present, there

is no need to provide underlying mechanisms such as persuasion knowledge and source

credibility. On the other hand, the demonstrated differential effects between sequential

exposure and simultaneous exposure require an explanation. Memory performance was

therefore examined as underlying mechanism. Findings however suggest that differences in

brand evaluation do not depend upon improved or hindered memory performance.

Thus, this study is unable to provide evidence to incorporate the underlying

mechanisms memory performance, persuasion knowledge, and source credibility in further

synergy and sequence research. Future research might examine other mechanisms to explain

sequence effects, such as (un)conscious processing. In fact, it could be argued that a more

unconscious process takes place in sequential exposure resulting in positive evaluations

without remembering the message explicitly (e.g., no brand recall) (Yoo, 2008). The less

positive evaluations in simultaneous exposure could be the result of a conscious process

(25)

24 the necessity of providing explanations for sequence effects as differences are demonstrated,

at least between sequential and simultaneous exposure.

Next to a lack of confirmatory findings, some unexpected results are revealed. The

finding that exposing people to the sponsored blog – banner sequence is as effective as

exposing people to the banner – sponsored blog sequence is in contrast to findings of earlier

sequence studies. Prior research generally shows that publicity (e.g., blogs) followed by

advertising (e.g., banners) is more effective than the reverse order (Loda & Coleman, 2005;

Micu & Pentina, 2015). The contrasting result could be explained by the fact that the type of

blog used in this study (i.e., sponsored blogs) might not be fully considered as publicity.

Although the message in the sponsored blog is endorsed by a third party, which is a

distinctive feature of publicity, the sponsorship disclosure may suggest paid advertising

(Cameron, 1994, Eisend & Küster, 2011). This could have led to a comparable evaluation of

both the banner as the blog, ruling out sequence effects.

With regard to the underlying mechanisms, it was expected that memory performance

would be determined in sequential exposure. However, the opposite holds true. In terms of

brand recall, memory performance is stronger in simultaneous exposure. In other words, by

being exposed to a brand multiple times at once the brand is better recalled than viewing the

brand multiple times sequentially. This study argued that memory performance would be

hindered in simultaneous exposure due to distraction and limited cognitive resources (Bergen

et al., 2006; Chowdhury et al., 2007; Smit et al., 2015; Voorveld, 2011). A possible

explanation for not being hindered could be related to the fact that exposure was in one

medium (i.e., online) and on one screen. Distraction effects and limited resources, on the

other hand, are derived from multitasking research into the use of multiple media or multiple

screens. An empirical study about multitasking within one medium (i.e., online) found no

(26)

25 environment was even found to be beneficial for recall (Tran, Carrillo, & Subrahmanyam,

2013). Another explanation for the unexpected higher recall in simultaneous exposure to a

banner and sponsored blog, can be found in context-related studies (Angell, Gorton, Sauer,

Bottomley, & White, 2016; Chun, Song, Hollenbeck, & Lee, 2014). One such study argues

that brand recall results in a greater effect when the message is presented in a

content-relevant website (Yaveroglua & Donthu, 2008). In the current study, the banner was

embedded in highly relevant content as it included a sponsored blog.

Limitations

Some limitations to this study should be noted. Along with limitations that come with

experimental research (e.g., forced-exposure setting, direct response), there are limitations

inherent to this study that researchers should bear in mind in follow-up studies. First, the

stimuli are self-produced and could therefore be perceived as artificial in the sense that

participants could not click on the banner to be redirected to the website or comment on the

blog post. However, it is important to include such interactive features as this characterizes

the online environment (Chung & Zhao, 2004). Second, the measurement of attitude toward

the banner and attitude toward the blog was dealt with after exposure to both communication

tools. Accurate measurement is hindered when blog-evoked responses might have overridden

banner-evoked responses or vice versa, because of measuring the attitudes at once. An

alternative suggestion would be to measure attitudes directly after the corresponding

exposure. However, in that case, participants could become more aware of the purpose of the

study. A final limitation has to do with the possibility to conduct the questionnaire either on a

desktop or mobile phone. Despite a potential confounding effect due to these different

(27)

26

Implications

As with every study, the findings of the current study have some implications for online

media research and advertising effectiveness. Although synergy is examined and confirmed

in earlier studies, this study is the first to demonstrate the effect in the context of banners and

sponsored blogs. In addition, it is the first to demonstrate sequence effects upon exposure to

these tools. Finally, underlying mechanisms are proposed and examined in a first attempt to

understand and evaluate the role of sequence. On the one hand, the study fails to provide

explanations for sequence effects, on the other hand, the study contributes to knowledge in

the field in terms that the unsupported mechanisms could be eliminated in future research.

Furthermore, a contribution is made with regard to the role of sequence in affecting

brand evaluation. The greater effect of sequential exposure to multiple communication tools

compared to simultaneous exposure to multiple communication tools implies that sequential

exposure benefits from synergy more. Synergy effects in sequential exposure can however

not be linked to backward retrieval and encoding variability – processes that underlie synergy – as that would improve memory performance, which is not demonstrated in the study. On

the contrary, simultaneous exposure results in better memory performance. A suggestion for

future research is to investigate other processes that underlie synergy (e.g., multiple source

perception) that could explain the greater advantage of sequential exposure over simultaneous

exposure. Within the sequential exposure format no sequence effects are found, although

suggested by prior studies (e.g., Loda & Coleman, 2005). Future research is needed to

investigate whether the absence of sequence effects exclusively holds true for the

combination of banners and sponsored blogs or for other online communication tools as well

(e.g., video, social media advertising).

A more methodological contribution concerns the stimuli in the single-tool

(28)

27 same medium compared to varied advertisements in multiple media. Synergy is then

attributed to the change in media exposure whereas the effect could (also) have been caused

by the change in information (identical versus varied) (Voorveld et al., 2011). This type of

false attribution is ruled out here by having varied stimuli in the single-tool conditions as well

as the multiple-tool conditions. Since synergy effects are demonstrated, the current study

provides an even stronger verification for the existence of the phenomenon.

Finally, some practical implications are discussed. Media agencies that aim to

increase recall of the brand are advised to apply a campaign with exposure to a banner and

sponsored blog on the same webpage. However, if practitioners aim at increasing evaluative

(more affective) responses, they should consider independent exposures. Independent

exposures entail banners that could be seen across several (unrelated) websites and blog posts

that could be seen across websites of relevant bloggers. This will increase the probability of

being exposed to a banner and blog sequentially.

At least, it becomes evident from this study that brands will benefit from combining

banners and sponsored blogs. It is therefore recommended to use multiple communication

tools if an online campaign is conducted.

Since it is the first study that focused on the role of sequence in this online context

and confirmatory findings fall short, future research is warranted. Suggestions are provided

and could serve as starting point in further studies. Specifically, the investigation to

(un)conscious processes as underlying mechanisms for sequence effects would be an

interesting follow-up to the current study.

REFERENCES

Angell, R., Gorton, M., Sauer, J., Bottomley, P., & White, J. (2016). Don’t distract me when I’m media multitasking: Toward a theory for raising advertising recall and

(29)

28 Assael, H. (2011). From silos to synergy: A fifty-year review of cross-media research shows

synergy has yet to achieve its full potential. Journal of Advertising Research, 51(1),

42-48.

Bergen, L., Grimes, T., & Potter, D. (2006). How attention partitions itself during

simultaneous message presentations. Human Communication Research, 31(3),

311-336.

Berlo, D. K., Lemert, J. B., & Mertz, R. J. (1969). Dimensions for evaluating the

acceptability of message sources. The Public Opinion Quarterly, 33(4), 563-576.

Boerman, S. C., Van Reijmersdal, E. A., & Neijens, P. C. (2012). Sponsorship disclosure:

Effects of duration on persuasion knowledge and brand responses. Journal of

Communication, 62(6), 1047-1064.

Bronner, A. E. (2006). Multimedia-synergie in reclamecampagnes [Cross-media synergy in

advertising campaigns]. Amsterdam: Stichting Wetenschappelijk Onderzoek

Commerciële Communicatie (SWOCC).

Cameron, G. T. (1994). Does publicity outperform advertising? An experimental test of the

third-party endorsement. Journal of Public Relations Research, 6(3), 185-207.

Campbell, M. C. (1995). When attention-getting advertising tactics elicit consumer inferences

of manipulative intent: The importance of balancing benefits and investments. Journal

of Consumer Psychology, 4(3), 225-254.

Campbell, M. C., & Keller, K. L. (2003). Brand familiarity and advertising repetition effects.

Journal of Consumer Research, 30(2), 292-304.

Chang, Y., & Thorson, E. (2004). Television and web advertising synergies. Journal of

(30)

29 Choi, S. M., & Rifon, N. J. (2002). Antecedents and consequences of web advertising

credibility: A study of consumer responses to banner ads. Journal of Interactive

Advertising, 3(1), 12-24.

Chowdhury, R. M. M. I., Finn, A., & Olsen, G. D. (2007). Investigating the simultaneous

presentation of advertising and television programming. Journal of Advertising, 36(3),

85-96.

Chun, K. Y., Song, J. H., Hollenbeck, C. R., & Lee, J. H. (2014). Are contextual

advertisements effective? The moderating role of complexity in banner advertising.

International Journal of Advertising, 33(2), 351-371.

Chung, H., & Zhao, X. (2004). Effects of perceived interactivity on web site preference and

memory: Role of personal motivation. Journal of Computer-Mediated

Communication, 10(1), article 7. doi: 10.1111/j.1083-6101.2004.tb00232.x

Colliander, J., & Dahlén, M. (2011). Following the fashionable friend: The power of social

media. Journal of Advertising Research, 51(1), 313-320.

Cowley, E., & Barron, C. (2008). When product placement goes wrong: The effects of

program liking and placement prominence. Journal of Advertising, 37(1), 89-98.

Dens, N., De Pelsmacker, P., Wouters, M., & Purnawirawan, N. (2012). Do you like what

you recognize? Journal of Advertising, 41(3), 35-54.

Dijkstra, M., Buijtels, H., & Van Raaij, W. F. (2005). Separate and joint effects of medium

type on consumer responses: a comparison of television, print, and the internet.

Journal of Business Research, 58(3), 377-386.

Edell, J. A., & Keller, K. L. (1989). The information processing of coordinated media

(31)

30 Eisend, M., & Küster, F. (2010). The effectiveness of publicity versus advertising: A

meta-analytic investigation of its moderators. Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science,

39(6), 906-921.

Fortin, D., Grimwood, S., & Finsterwalder, J. (2011). An empirical investigation into the

combined effect of sequence and multiple-media exposure on audience attitudes. In

Paper presented at the Australian and New Zealand Marketing Academy Conference (ANZMAC). Perth, Australia.

Friestad, M., & Wright, P. (1994). The persuasion knowledge model: How people cope with

persuasion attempts. Journal of Consumer Research, 21(1), 1-31.

Hayes, A. F. (2013). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process

analysis: A regression-based approach. New York, NY: Guilford Press.

Hsu, C. L., & Lin, J. C. C. (2008). Acceptance of blog usage: The roles of technology

acceptance, social influence and knowledge sharing motivation. Information

Management, 45(1), 65-74.

Huang, L. S., Chou, Y. J., & Lin, C. H. (2008). The influence of reading motives on the

responses after reading blogs. CyberPsychology & Behavior, 11(3), 351-355.

IAB NL (2015, October 12). Online Ad Spend study 2015 H1 [PDF file]. Retrieved from

http://iab.nl/nieuws/online-ad-spend-study-2015-h1.

Jaekel, B. (2016, February 26). Emerald Nuts, Nature's Bounty tap blog popularity to drive

consumer connections [Blog post]. Retrieved from http://www.mobilemarketer.com/

cms/news/media/22334.html/.

Janssens, W., De Pelsmacker, P., & Geuens, M. (2012). Online advertising and congruency

effects: It depends on how you look at it. International Journal of Advertising, 31(3),

(32)

31 Jeong, S. H., & Hwang, Y. (2012). Does multitasking increase or decrease persuasion?

Effects of multitasking on comprehension and counterarguing. Journal of

Communication, 62(4), 571-587.

Jin, H. S. (2003). Compounding consumer interest: Effects of advertising campaign publicity

on the ability to recall subsequent advertisements. Journal of Advertising, 32(4),

29-41.

Jin, H. S., & Zhao, X., & An, S. (2006). Examining effects of advertising campaign publicity

in a field study. Journal of Advertising Research, 46(2), 171-182.

Johnson, T. J., & Kaye, B. K. (2004). Wag the blog: How reliance on traditional media and

the internet influence credibility perceptions of weblogs among blog users.

Journalism & Mass Communication Quarterly, 81(3), 622-642.

Kaplan, A. M., & Haenlein, M. (2010). Users of the world, unite! The challenges and

opportunities of social media. Business Horizons, 53(1), 59-68.

Keller, K. L. (1987). Memory factors in advertising: The effect of advertising retrieval cues

on brand evaluations. Journal of Consumer Research, 14(3), 316-333.

Kim, J., Yoon, H. J., & Lee, S. Y. (2010). Integrating advertising and publicity. Journal of

Advertising, 39(1), 97-114.

Lambrecht, A., & Tucker, C. (2013). When does retargeting work? Information specificity in

online advertising. Journal of Marketing Research, 50(5), 561-576.

Lang, A. (2000). The limited capacity model of mediated message processing. Journal of

Communication, 50(1), 46-70.

Liebe, U., Glenk, K., Oehlmann, M., & Meyerhoff, J. (2015). Does the use of mobile devices

(tablets and smartphones) affect survey quality and choice behaviour in web surveys?

(33)

32 Loda, M. D., & Coleman, B. C. (2005). Sequence matters: A more effective way to use

advertising and publicity. Journal of Advertising Research, 45(4), 362-372.

McQuarrie, E. F., & Munson, J. M. (1992). A revised product involvement inventory:

Improved usability and validity. In J. F. Sherry Jr. & B. Sternthal (Eds.), Advances in

Consumer Research, 19 (pp. 108-115). Provo, UT: Association for Consumer

Research.

Metzger, M. J., Flanagin, A. J., & Medders, R. B. (2010). Social and heuristic approaches to

credibility evaluation online. Journal of Communication, 60(3), 413-439.

Micu, A. C., & Pentina, I. (2015). Examining search as opposed to experience goods when

investigating synergies of internet news articles and banner ads. Internet Research,

25(3), 378-398.

Naik, P. A., & Raman, K. (2003). Understanding the impact of synergy in multimedia

communications. Journal of Marketing Research, 40(4), 375-388.

Neijens, P., & Voorveld, H. (2015). Cross-platform advertising: Current practices and issues

for the future. Journal of Advertising Research, 55(4), 362-367.

Orr, J. M., Sackett, P. R., & Dubois, C. L. Z. (1991). Outlier detection and treatment in I/O

psychology: A survey of researcher beliefs and an empirical illustration. Personnel

Psychology, 44(3), 473-486.

Pornpitakpan, C. (2004). The persuasiveness of source credibility: A critical review of five decades’ evidence. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 34(2), 243-281.

Robinson, Z. (2015, March 11). Native advertising pt. 2: Bridging the gap between content,

social & SEO [Blog post]. Retrieved from

http://www.add3.com/insights/social-

(34)

33 Segev, S., Wang, W., & Fernandes, J. (2014). The effects of ad-context congruency on

responses to advertising in blogs. International Journal of Advertising, 33(1), 17-36.

Sehl, K. (2013, October 17). Sponsored blogs: Where native advertising gets personal [Blog

post]. Retrieved from

http://sparksheet.com/sponsored-blogs-where-native-advertising-gets-personal/.

Smit, E. G., Boerman, S. C., & Van Meurs, L. (2015). The power of direct context as

revealed by eye tracking: A model tracks relative attention to competing editorial and

promotional content. Journal of Advertising Research, 55(2), 216-227.

Southern, L. (2015, October 8). The IAB UK: “Banners don’t work” [Blog post]. Retrieved

from http://digiday.com/publishers/u-k-s-iab-banners-dont-work/.

Spears, N., & Singh, S. N. (2004). Measuring attitude toward the brand and purchase

intentions. Journal of Current Issues & Research in Advertising, 26(2), 53-66.

Stammerjohan, C., Wood, C. M., Chang, Y., & Thorson, E. (2005). An empirical

investigation of the interaction between publicity, advertising, and previous brand

attitudes and knowledge. Journal of Advertising, 34(4), 55-67.

Sternberg, J. (2013, April 18). Time to define native advertising [Blog post]. Retrieved from

http://digiday.com/publishers/time-to-define-native-advertising.

Sternthal, B., Dholakia, R., & Leavitt, C. (1978). The persuasive effect of source credibility:

Tests of cognitive responses. Journal of Consumer Research, 4(4), 252-260.

Tang, T., Newton, G. D., & Wang, X. (2007). Does synergy work? An examination of

cross-promotion effects. The International Journal on Media Management, 9(4), 127-134.

Tran, P., Carrillo, R., & Subrahmanyam, K. (2013). Effects of online multitasking on reading

comprehension of expository text. Cyberpsychology: Journal of Psychosocial

(35)

34 Van den Berg, M. (2015, July 3). Outbound & inbound marketing: Wat is de ideale

combinatie? [Blog post]. Retrieved from https://www.frankwatching.com/archive/

2015/07/03/outbound-inbound-marketing-wat-is-de-ideale-combinatie.

Van Reijmersdal, E. A. (2009). Brand placement prominence: Good for memory! Bad for

attitudes? Journal of Advertising Research, 49(2), 151-153.

Van Reijmersdal, E. A., Fransen, M. L., Van Noort, G., Opree, S., Vandeberg, L., Reusch, S.,

Van Lieshout, F., & Boerman, S. C. (2015). Effects of disclosing sponsored content in

blogs: How the use of resistance strategies mediates effects on persuasion. In Paper

presented at the Etmaal van de Communicatiewetenschap. Antwerp, Belgium.

Voorveld, H. A. M. (2011). Media multitasking and the effectiveness of combining online

and radio advertising. Computers in Human Behavior, 27(6), 2200-2206.

Voorveld, H. A. M., Neijens, P. C., & Smit, E. G. (2011). Opening the black box:

Understanding cross-media effects. Journal of Marketing Communications, 17(2),

69-85.

Voorveld, H. A. M., Neijens, P. C., & Smit, E. G. (2012). The interacting role of media

sequence and product involvement in cross-media campaigns. Journal of Marketing

Communications, 18(3), 203-216.

Wang, A. (2006). When synergy in marketing communication online enhances audience

response: The effects of varying advertising and product publicity messages. Journal

of Advertising Research, 46(2), 160-170.

Wei, M. L., Fischer, E., & Main, K. J. (2008). An examination of the effects of activating

persuasion knowledge on consumer response to brands engaging in covert marketing.

(36)

35 Wojdynski, B. W., & Evans, N. J. (2016). Going native: Effects of disclosure position and

language on the recognition and evaluation of online native advertising. Journal of

Advertising, 45(2), 157-168.

Yaveroglu, I., & Donthu, N. (2008). Advertising repetition and placement issues in online

environments. Journal of Advertising, 37(2), 31-43.

Yoo, C. Y. (2008). Unconscious processing of web advertising: Effects on implicit memory,

attitude toward the brand, and consideration set. Journal of Interactive Marketing,

22(2), 2-18.

Young, D. R., & Bellezza, F. S. (1982). Encoding variability, memory organization, and the

repetition effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and

(37)

36

APPENDIX Appendix A - Tables

TABLE 1. Correlation coefficients of demographic variables and control variables Demographics Chi-square (χ²)

Attitude banner Attitude blog Brand attitude

Purchase intention Gender 12.94 11.68 25.48 16.87 Age 373.20 356.69 620.45 523.17 Nationality 149.76 169.99 606.91*** 454.99*** Education 79.50 85.46 160.83 131.03

Control variables Pearson's r

Attitude banner Attitude blog Brand attitude

Purchase intention Prior knowledge -0.02 -0.14 -0.18* 0.01 Product involvement -0.27*** -0.42*** -0.55*** -0.44***

Note: * p < .05, ** p < .01, *** p < .001. The variables prior knowledge and product involvement are log transformed.

(38)

37 TABLE 2. Mean scores and standard deviations of all evaluative measures as function of multiple-/single-tool exposure

Multiple-tool condition Single-tool condition

Banners Sponsored blogs

(n = 72) (n = 37) (n = 39)

Attitude banner ª 4.09 (1.15) 3.64 (1.70) -

Attitude blog ª 4.51 (1.09) - 4.55 (1.36)

Brand attitude ª 5.56 (0.64) 4.28 (1.41) 5.38 (1.34)

Purchase intention ª 3.45 (1.25) 2.68 (1.61) 3.35 (1.53)

Note: Attitude toward the blog is not measured in the banner condition and attitude toward the banner is not measured in the blog condition.

(39)

38

Referenties

GERELATEERDE DOCUMENTEN

This is a valuable contribution to the literature since Voorveld (2011) stated that the influence of media multitasking on affective and behavioral responses is less

This study shows that people who are repeatedly exposed to online banner ads show more positive cognitive responses in terms of brand recognition than people who

Implications are far reaching, since a particular business model does not yield a superior financial performance, future research should shift its focus in a number of

touches at emotional story moments were perceived to be mostly unrelated to the story. Even though these touch points were marked as emotional high points in a previous study of Wang

Concordance between preoperative computed tomography angiographic mapping and intraoperative perforator selection for deep inferior epigastric artery perforator flap breast

We have studied the small strain behavior of granular materials, by building stress and fabric response en- velopes for isotropic and anisotropic samples. From our analysis, we

De ondernemingsraad heeft instemmingsrecht op de spelregels bij het individueel roosteren maar niet op de roosters zelf, omdat deze individueel zijn – en dus niet betrekking

This is based on the fact that size has a negative effect on click-through rate and that all the attention grabbing design elements such as high number of colors,