• No results found

Stress and control

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Share "Stress and control"

Copied!
12
0
0

Bezig met laden.... (Bekijk nu de volledige tekst)

Hele tekst

(1)

Bachelorproject:  Stress  and  Control  

Femke  Heemskerk  

26  mei  2016  

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

Studentnumber:  10365877  

University  of  Amsterdam  

Guided  by:  Henk  Cremers  

Words:  3574

(2)
(3)

The  Perceived  Relation  Between  Stress  and  Control  (A  Network  Approach)  

 

Femke  Heemskerk  

University  of  Amsterdam  

26  mei  2016                                                                       Introduction    

Stress  has  long  been  of  interest  in  many  different   fields   of   research   because   of   its   implications   for   health.   Many   people   perceive   stress   as   a   change   in   emotional   and   physical   functioning,   such   as   irritations  and  frustrations,  sometimes  even  resulting   in   physical   problems   such   as   flu,   sore   throats,   headaches,   and   backaches   (DeLongis,   Folkman   &   Lazarus   (1988)   or   psychological   problems   such   as   anxiety  and  depression  (Lazarus,  1999;  Zautra,  2003   both   cited   in   Almeida,   2005).   There   are   many   situations   (stressors)   that   can   cause   people   to   experience  stress.  Daily  stressors,  for  example,  could   be   concerns   of   work,   caring   for   other   people,   and   commuting   between   work   and   home   (Almeida,   2005).    

Control  can  be  defined  as  the  belief  that  someone   has  the  opportunity  of  influencing  the  aversiveness  of   an   event   (Thompson,   1981).   Averill   (1973)   discovered  that  control  could  have  stress-­‐inducing  or   stress-­‐reducing  properties,  distinguishing  three  main   types  of  control:  (a)  behavioural  (direct  action  on  the   environment),   (b)   cognitive   (the   interpretation   of   events),   and   (c)   decisional   (having   a   choice   among   alternative   courses   of   action).   Averill   (1973)   concluded  that  the  stress-­‐inducing  or  stress-­‐reducing   properties   of   personal   control   depend   upon   the   experience   of   control   (e.g.,   too   many   response   options   may   lead   to   conflict   and   feelings   of   helplessness,   or   a   person   of   limited   competence   might   still   experience   considerable   control   provided   that   his   goals   were   not   set   beyond   his   capabilities).     Research  of  Diehl  and  Hay  (2010)  shows  that  control   can  be  a  mediating  factor  in  coping  with  daily  stress:   not  only  do  individuals  report  higher  negative  affect  

on  days  that  they  experience  more  stress  than  usual,   they   also   do   so   on   days   that   they   experience   less   control   than   usual.   The   results   of   both   studies   are   interesting   because   they   suggest   that   one   could   reduce   the   negative   effects   of   stress   by   increasing   control.    

The  biological  mechanism  of  stress  possibly  gives   one   more   understanding   of   the   effects   of   perceived   control   on   stress.   For   example   subject   A   is   being   stressed   by   event   B.   Event   B   activates   the   stress   response   system   (SRS),   a   complex,   integrated   biological   network   of   central   neural   and   hormonal   responses   (Ellis,   Jackson   &   Boyce,   2006).   This   network  plays  an  important  role  in  multiple  adaptive   functions   that   prepare   subject   A   (the   organism)   for   the   challenge   or   threat   (event   B)   by   directing   the   body’s   energy,   resulting   in   changes   in   affect,   motivation   and   cognition   (Boyce   &   Ellis,   2005;   Ellis,   Jackson  &  Boyce,  2006;  Del  Giudice,  Ellis  &  Shirtcliff,   2011;   Greenberg,   Carr   &   Summers,   2002).   Two   important  stress  structures  that  are  activated  during   stressful  events  will  be  discussed  below.  

The   first   structure   activated   by   stressful   situations   is   the   hypothalamic-­‐pituitary-­‐ adrenocortical   (HPA)   axis,   regulating   the   release   of   cortisol,   an   important   hormone   associated   with   psychological,   physiological,   and   physical   health   functioning   (Dickerson   &   Kemeny,   2004).   The   HPA   axis   is   helpful   in   threatening   situations;   when   activated  for  an  extended  period  of  time,  however,  it   can   have   serious   health   implications   (McEwen,   199;   Heim   &   Nemeroff,   1999;   Wingenfeld   &   Wolf,   2011).     Hence  stress  can  result  in  physical  and  psychological   problems;   it   would   therefore   be   of   interest   to   research   ways   the   negative   consequences   of   stress   can  be  reduced.    

Both   psychological   and   biological   researchers   have   argued   that   there   is   a   relationship   between  stress  and  control.  It  is  yet  unclear  whether  this  relationship  can  be  perceived  and  if   so,   how   people   perceive   this   relation.   The   present   study   developed   a   novel   (Dutch)   psychological  questionnaire  based  on  the  Perceived  Stress  Scale  (PSS)  10  item  inventory  for   measuring  the  Perceived  Causal  Relations  (PCR)  between  stress  and  control.   91  Psychology   students   of   the   University   of   Amsterdam   participated   (19   male,   67   female).   The   results   indicate  that  people  perceive  a  causal  relation  between  stress  and  control:  control  leads  to  a   decrease  in  stress  and  stress  leads  to  a  decrease  of  control.  The  network  analysis  shows  that   the   PCR   between   control   and   stress   is   stronger   than   the   PCR   between   stress   and   control.   Control  has  indeed  a  stress-­‐reducing  effect.  Future  research  should  focus  on  a  more  diverse   sample  group,  keeping  into  account  age,  life  phase  and  educational  level.  It  is  also  possible  to   link  psychological  to  biological  research  and  to  develop  a  longitudinal  research  design.    

(4)

As   mentioned   before,   multiple   researchers   have   discovered   that   control   can   have   stress-­‐reducing   properties.   These   findings   are   in   line   with   biological   research   of   Dickerson   and   Kemeny   (2004)   showing   that   cortisol   activation   can   be   effected   by   perceived   loss   of   control   (i.e.   loss   of   control   can   have   negative   physiological   and   psychological   effects;   Frazier,   Berman  &  Steward,  2002;  Hofmann,  2005).    

The   second   structure   affected   by   stressful   situations   is   the   ventromedial   prefrontal   cortex   (vmPFC).   The   vmPFC   is   a   brain   region   that   plays   an   important   role   in   cognitive   control   (Dedovic,   Duchesne,   Andrews,   Engert   &   Pruessner,   2009;   McKlveen,   Myers   &   Herman,   2015;   Miller   &   Cohen,   2001)   regulating   emotion,   thought   and   behaviour   under  non-­‐stress  conditions  (Arnsten,  2009).    Under   conditions   of   stress   the   higher-­‐order   abilities   of   the   vmPFC   are   impaired,   resulting   in   uncontrolled   and   rapid  emotional  responses  (Arnsten,  2009).    

Research   of   Maier   and   Watkins   (2010)   suggests   vmPFC   plasticity;   it   appears   that   the   experience   of   control   alters   the   vmPFC   in   such   a   way   that   later   uncontrollable   stressors   now   activate   the   vmPFC   circuitry,   leading   to   stressor   resistance,   i.e.   control   would   result   in   a   decrease   of   stress.   This   finding   could  have  positive  implications  for  further  research.   It  could  be  possible  to  increase  our  sense  of  control,   which   would   offer   protection   against   the   negative   effects  of  stress.    

 Overall   psychological   and   biological   research   shows   that   there   is   a   relation   between   stress   and   control;   some   suggesting   that   stress   has   a   negative   effect  on  control  and  others  arguing  that  control  has  a   positive  effect  on  stress.  Due  to  the  fact  that  up  to  the   present   there   has   been   no   scientific   research   on   the   perceived   causal   relation   (PCR)   between   stress   and   control,   it   is   unclear   if   and   how   people   perceive   this   relation.  The  present  study  examines  whether  people   perceive  a  causal  relation  between  stress  and  control.   When  a  PCR  is  present  a  network  is  visualized.  Based   on   the   research   mentioned   above   we   hypothesize   that  stressful  situations  cause  people  to  experience  a   loss  of  control,  resulting  in  an  increase  in  stress.  It  is   expected   that   perceived   loss   of   control   causes   a   rise   in  the  perception  of  stress,  or  stated  differently,  that   perceived  control  causes  a  decrease  of  stress.  

   

Method     Participants  

91  psychology  (19  men  and  67  women)  students   from  the  University  of  Amsterdam  in  the  Netherlands   completed   our   questionnaire   in   a   battery   of   questionnaires  as  part  of  a  broader  research  program   that  all  first  year  students  have  to  participate  in.  The   ethical  commission  has  approved  this  study.  

   

Instruments  

Perceived   Stress   Scale   (PSS).   The   PSS   (Cohen,   Kamarck   &   Mermelstein,   1983)   is   a   diagnostic   questionnaire  used  to  measure  perceived  stress.  The   PSS   10   item   inventory   is   translated   to   Dutch   and   extended  with  20  follow-­‐up  questions,  as  the  present   study   examines   the   perceived   causal   relation   between  stress  and  control.    

Perceived   Causal   Relations   (PCR)   Scaling.   The   series   of   follow-­‐up   questions   is   developed   based   on   the   systematic   approach   from   Frewen,   Allen,   Lanius   and   Neufeld   (2012)   known   as   Perceived   Causal   Relation   scaling   (PCR).   First,   a   person   indicates   on   the   PSS   which   of   the   thoughts   and   feelings   were   present   during   the   last   month.   Secondly,   each   combination  of  presents  thoughts  and  feelings  (i,j)  is   combined   in   a   question   that   assesses   whether   i   caused  j  (Borsboom  &  Cramer,  2013).  Answers  on  the   first   ten   questions   (the   PSS)   were   included   in   the   follow-­‐up   questions,   so   that   the   formulation   became   evident.  For  example;  when  people  report  they  have   been   ‘fairly   often’   upset   in   the   last   month   and   they   have  ‘almost  never’  felt  like  they  were  in  control,  they   would   be   asked   in   the   follow-­‐up   question   to   report   the   extent   in   which   the   first   influenced   the   latter   selecting  an  answer  scaling  from  ‘much  less’  to  ‘much   more’  (e.g.,  ‘because  I  have  been  upset  ‘fairly  often’  I   felt  ‘much  less’  like  I  was  in  control’).    

The   items   used   in   questions   one   to   ten   were   assigned   to   either   the   construct   ‘stress’   or   ‘control’   Items   1,   3   and   7   were   assigned   to   ‘stress’   as   they   reflect   emotional   reactions;   item   8   was   assigned   to   ‘control’.  Follow-­‐up  questions  based  on  items  4,  5,  6,   9,   and   10   were   excluded,   as   it   was   not   possible   to   assign   these   to   either   variables   ‘stress’   or   ‘control’   due   to   ambiguities   in   formulation   (for   further   information  see  Appendix  A  and  B).  

Network  Analysis.  Due  to  the  use  of  the  use  of  PCR   it  was  possible  to  build  a  matrix  of  the  self-­‐reported   relations   between   all   items.   In   this   way   a   network   analysis  emerges  that  represents  the  causal  structure   of   stress   and   control.   A   network   analysis   can   be   of   value   to   discover   and   visualize   the   most   important   PCRs.    

Measures.  The  scores  for  the  first  10  questions  are   obtained   by   scaling   the   answers   on   a   5-­‐point   Likert   scale  (e.g.,  0  =  never,  1  =  almost  never,  2  =  sometimes,   3  =  fairly  often,  4  =  very  often).  The  scores  of  the  20   follow-­‐up   questions   are   also   obtained   on   a   5-­‐point   Likert   scale   (e.g.,   -­‐2   =   much   less,   -­‐1   =   less,   0   =   not   more  or  less,  1  =  more,  2  =  much  more).  

 

Procedure  

Participants  were  tested  in  a  university  computer   hall.  The  time  required  to  complete  the  questionnaire   was   approximately   5-­‐10   minutes.   Participants   were   asked  to  fill  in  informed  consent  and  were  debriefed   following  completion.  

(5)

Statistical  procedure  

The  hypothesis  has  been  tested  using  one-­‐sample   and   independent-­‐samples   T   tests,   allowing   to   (a)   compare   whether   the   PCRs   we   found   were   significantly   greater   than   zero,   and   (b)   to   simultaneously  check  for  effects  of  sex.  A  significance   level  of  0.05  is  used.    

Three  different  analyses  have  been  used  in  order   to   examine   whether   perceived   control   causes   a   decrease   of   stress.  First,   a   one-­‐sample   T   test   is   used   to   analyse   the   PCRs   between   the   two   most   relevant   items  (‘nervous/stress’  and  ‘control’).  Second,  a  one-­‐ sample  T  test  is  used  to  analyse  the  PCRs  between  the   three   main   stress-­‐related   items   (‘upset’,   ‘nervous/stress’   and   ‘irritable’)   and   control.   Third,   the   PCRs   between   stress   and   control   items   were   presented   in   a   network   analysis.   No   correction   is   performed  for  multiple  comparisons.  

   

Results    

The  results  from  86  participants  were  used  in  the   statistical   analysis.   Five   participants   have   been   excluded;   one   participant   due   to   old   age   (age   52.8),   two   participants   due   to   missing   values   and   two   participants   due   to   a   deviation   of   zero   when   answering   question   11   to   30.   Age   is   relevant   to   compare   the   total   PSS   score   of   the   Dutch   questionnaire   used   with   the   norm   table   (age   18-­‐29)   for  the  PSS  10  item  inventory.  Checking  the  variation   within   the   answers   is   important   to   analyse   whether   there   was   honest   participation   (i.e.,   participants   not   reporting  the  same  answer  to  every  question  due  to   lack  of  motivation).  

 

   

Table   1   presents   descriptive   statistics   of   the   between-­‐person  variables  (i.e.,  sex,  age  and  total  PSS   score).   Calculating   age   is   important   because   of   the   use  of  the  norm  table  (age  18-­‐29)  of  the  PSS  10  item   inventory.  In  this  way  the  total  PSS  score  of  the  Dutch   translation   (M   =   15.56,   SD   =   6.22)   can   be   compared   with   the   total   PSS   score   from   the   norm   table   (M   =   14.2,   SD   =   6.2).   This   is   required   when   checking   whether  the  Dutch  questionnaire  measures  the  same   construct   (stress)   as   the   original   PSS   10   item   inventory.   The   results   of   a   independent-­‐samples   T   test   show   that   women   score   significantly   higher   on  

the   PSS   10   inventory   than   men,   t(84)   =   -­‐2.57,   p   =   0.012,  95%  CI  [-­‐7.14,  -­‐0.91].  As  the  total  PSS  score  is   only  used  as  a  reference  to  the  PSS  10  item  inventory,   these  results  will  not  obstruct  further  analysis.  They   are,   however,   a   reminder   to   evaluate   differences   of   sex  throughout  the  rest  of  the  statistical  analysis.    

   

First,   the   PCR   between   the   two   most   relevant   items,   ‘nervous/stressed’   and   ‘control’,   are   analysed.   Figure  1  presents  the  mean  score  on  question  17  (the   PCR   between   ‘nervous/stressed’   and   ‘control’)   and   the   mean   score   on   question   24   (the   PCR   between   ‘control’  and  ‘nervous/stressed’).  

Results   of   a   one-­‐sample   T   test   show   participants   reported   a   significant   negative   PCR   between   ‘nervous/stressed’  and  ‘control’,  M  =  -­‐0.28,  SD  =  0.89,   t(85)   =   -­‐2.91,   p=   0.005,   95%   CI   [-­‐0.47,   -­‐0.09].   They   also   reported   a   significant   negative   PCR   between   ‘control’  on  ‘nervous/stressed’,  M  =  -­‐0.65,  SD  =  0.97,   t(85)  =  -­‐6.24,  p  <  0.001,  95%  CI  [-­‐0.86,  -­‐0.44].  There   was   no   significant   difference   between   men   and   women   on   both   questions   (Q17:   t(84)   =   1.56,   p   =   0.122,   95%   CI   [-­‐0.10,   0.81];   Q24:   t(84)   =   .10,   p   =   0.921,   95%   CI   [-­‐0.48,   0.53]).   These   results   indicate   that  there  is  a  PCR  between  stress  and  control;  stress   results   in   a   loss   of   control   and   control   results   in   a   reduction  of  stress.  

Second,  two  other  stress  related  items,  ‘upset’  and   ‘irritable’,   were   included,   as   stress   can   be   experienced   in   different   ways.   A   new   variable   ‘mean_stress’   was   computed   for   the   mean   PCR   between  the  three  most  relevant  stress  related  items   (‘upset’,   ‘nervous/stressed’   and   ‘irritable’)   and   ‘control’.   A   second   variable   mean_control’   was   computed   for   the   mean   PCRs   between   ‘control’   and   the  three  stress  items.    

Results   of   a   one-­‐sampe   T   test   show   that   participants   reported   a   significant   negative   PCR   between  ‘mean_stress’  and  ‘mean_control’,  M  =  -­‐0.14,   SD   =   0.60,   t(85)   =   -­‐2.08,   p   =   0.040,   95%   CI   [-­‐0.27,   -­‐ 0.01].   There   was   a   significant   difference   between   men   and   women,   t(84)   =   2.01,   p   =   0.048,   95%   CI   [0.003,   0.62].   Women   reported   a   negative   PCR   between  ‘mean_stress’  and  ‘mean_control’  (M  =  -­‐0.20,   SD   =   0.58),   while   men   reported   a   positive   PCR   between  ‘mean_stress’  and  ‘mean_control’  (M  =  0.11,  

Table&1. Descriptive*statistics*men*and*women N Mean&age SD&age Mean&total& PSS&score SD&total&PSS& score Men 19 21,775 2,204 12,4211 6,18525 Women 67 20,3686 1,294 16,4478 5,98048 Total 86 20,679 1,635 15,5581 6,22066 Sex Figure'1. PCR'between'stress'and'control. M  =  -­‐0.65,  SD  =  0.97   M  =  -­‐0.28,  SD  =  0.89  

(6)

SD   =   0.63).   Participants   also   reported   a   significant   negative   PCR   between   ‘mean_control’   and   ‘mean_stress’,  M  =  -­‐0.64,  SD  =  0.89,  t(84)  =  -­‐6.64,  p  <   0.001,  95%  CI  [-­‐0.83,  -­‐0.45].  There  was  no  significant   difference  between  men  and  women,  t(84)  =  0.33,  p  =   0.740.  95%  CI  [-­‐0.39,  0.54].  

The   results   above   provide   additional   support   for   our   hypotheses:   people   perceive   a   causal   relation   between  stress  and  control.  Stress  causes  a  decrease   of  control  and  control  causes  a  decrease  of  stress.    

   

 

   

To   examine   whether   an   increase   in   stress   causes   people  to  experience  a  greater  loss  of  control,  and  if   an  increase  of  control  results  in  a  greater  reduction  in   stress,  the  results  were  plotted  in  graphs  (see  figure  2   and  3;  the  item  score  is  placed  on  the  x-­‐axis  and  the   PCR  on  the  y-­‐axis).    

Figure  2  suggests  that  when  participants  report  a   higher   level   of   ‘nervous/stresses’,   they   perceive   a   stronger   negative   relation   between   stress   and   control.   When   participants   report   a   low   level   of   ‘nervous/stressed’,   they   report   a   positive   relation   between   stress   and   control.   These   results   indicate   that   when   people   experience   a   low   level   of   stress,   they  feel  like  they  have  more  control,  and  when  they   experience   a   high   level   of   stress,   they   feel   like   they   have  less  control  (Spearman’s  rho  =  -­‐0.42,  p  <  0.001,   95%  CI  [-­‐0.60,  -­‐0.21]).    

Figure   3   shows   that   when   participants   report   a   low  level  of  control,  they  perceive  a  negative  relation   between  control  and  stress.  When  they  report  a  high   level   of   control,   participants   perceive   a   slightly   positive   relation   between   control   and   stress.   When   people  experience  less  control,  they  experience  more   stress,   and   when   they   feel   like   they   have   more   control,   they   experience   less   stress   (Spearman’s   rho   =  0.41,  p  >  0.001,  95%  CI  [0.18,  0.61]).            

Third,   a  network  analysis  is  shown  to  detect  and   visualize   the   most   important   PCRs   and   the   most   central  item.  The  descriptive  statistics  and  the  results   of   a   one-­‐sample   T   test   are   presented   in   table   2.   The   results   of   the   T   test   for   questions   related   to   item   9   (angry)   were   not   significant   so   these   PCRs   have   not   been  included  in  the  network.  

Qgraph   for   ‘R’,   developed   by   S.   Epskamp   (2016),   was   used   to   plot   a   network   from   the   correlations   presented   in   table   3   (i.e.,   the   PCR   between   ‘upset’,   ‘nervous/stressed’,  ‘irritable’,  and  ‘control’).  Figure  4   presents   the   network   analysis.   Participants   report   the   strongest   PCR   between   control   and   stress;   ‘control’  has  the  strongest  adverse  effect  on  all  other   items.            

This   network,   together   with   the   correlation   matrix   in   table   3,   also   shows   the   importance   of   ‘irritable’.  Interestingly  there  is  a  small  positive  effect   of   ‘upset’   on   ‘control’.   It   is   possible   that   people   experience   more   control   when   they   are   less   upset,   resulting  in  a  positive  PCR.  

  Figure'2. Correlation'between'the'score'on'nervous/stressed' and'the'PCR'between'nervous/stressed'and'control. Figure'3. Correlation'between'the'score'on'control'and'the' PCR'between'control'and'nervous/stressed.'''''' Note:&the'score'on'Q8'is'recoded,'ranging'from''0'=' 'fairly'often''and'4'=''almost'never'. Table&3. Perceived(relations(between(stress(and(control(items

Upset Nervous/Stressed Irritated Control

Upset 1 :0,38 :0,42 0,22

Nervous/Stressed 0,25 1 0,28 :0,28

Irritated 0,28 0,26 1 :0,37

(7)

          Discussion    

The   present   study   focused   on   one   objective.   The   PCR   between   stress   and   control   was   examined,  

hypothesizing   that   perceived   control   causes   a   decrease  of  stress.  Results  show  that  people  perceive   a   causal   relation   between   stress   and   control;   more   stress   results   in   a   decrease   of   control,   more   control   results   in   a   decrease   of   stress.   These   findings   are   consistent   with   previous   psychological   research   (Averill,   1973;   Diehl   &   Hay,   2010)   and   biological   research   (Dickerson   &   Kemeny,   2004;   Maier   &   Watkins,   2010)   suggesting   that   perceived   control   could   offer   protection   against   the   negative   effects   of   stress.  It  is  important  to  keep  in  mind  that  perceived   causal   relations   have   been   analysed   therefore   the   conclusions   are   based   on   self-­‐reports.   Actual   causality   (and   the   role   of   the   HPA-­‐axis   and   the   vmPFC)  cannot  be  determined.    

 

Implications  

The   results   of   the   current   study   are   of   value   due   to   implications   specified   below.   As   is   mentioned   in   the   introduction,   stress   can   be   helpful   for   an   organism,   but   prolonged   stress   can   have   negative   physical   and   psychological   consequences.   The   conducted   research   shows   that   perceived   control   could   have   stress-­‐reducing   properties.   Biological   conclusions   cannot   be   drawn,   but   the   results   of   this   research   can   possibly   link   psychological   with   biological  research.    

First,   Dickerson   and   Kemeny   (2004)   show   that   cortisol  activation  can  be  effected  by  perceived  loss  of   control.   It   is   therefore   possible   that   an   increase   of   perceived   control   results   in   a   decrease   of   cortisol;   control   could   work   as   a   protection   mechanism  

Ups N/S

Irr Cnt

Figure'4.

Network of the perceived relation between control (Cnt), Irritable (Irr), Nervous/Stressed (N/S) and Upset (Ups).

Note. (a) The edges represent the relations. The bigger

the effect, the thicker the edge. (b) The colour of the edge represent the direction of the perceived relation, red is negative,'green'is'positive.'

Table&2.

Discriptive*statistics*and*one/sample*T*test*for*the*PCRs.

Question Mean Std.&Deviation t df Sig.&(25tailed) &95%&Confidence&Interval&of&the&Difference Lower Upper Q11:&PCR&between&upset&and&nervous/stressed 5.41 1.182 53.193 85 .002 5.66 5.15 Q12:&PCR&between&upset&and&irritable 5.44 1.102 53.719 85 .000 5.68 5.21 Q13:&PCR&between&upset&and&control .24 .957 2.366 85 .020 .04 .45 Q14:&PCR&between&upset&and&angry 5.43 1.069 53.733 85 .000 5.66 5.20 Q15:&PCR&between&nervous/stressed&and&upset .24 .993 2.280 85 .025 .03 .46 Q16:&PCR&between&nervous/stressed&and&irritable .28 1.144 2.262 85 .026 .03 .52 Q17:&PCR&between&nervous/stressed&and&control 5.28 .890 52.908 85 .005 5.47 5.09 Q18:&PCR&between&nervous/stressed&and&angry .21 .922 2.106 85 .038 .01 .41 Q19:&PCR&between&irritable&and&upset .28 .903 2.866 85 .005 .09 .47 Q20:&PCR&between&irritable&and&nervous/stressed .26 .843 2.815 85 .006 .08 .44 Q21:&PCR&between&irritable&and&control 5.37 .736 54.686 85 .000 5.53 5.21 Q22:&PCR&between&irritable&and&angry .38 .856 4.155 85 .000 .20 .57 Q23:&PCR&between&control&and&upset 5.58 .951 55.667 85 .000 5.79 5.38 Q24:&PCR&between&control&and&nervous/stressed 5.65 .967 56.243 85 .000 5.86 5.44 Q25:&PCR&between&control&and&irritable 5.69 .936 56.795 85 .000 5.89 5.49 Q26:&PCR&between&control&and&angry 5.59 .845 56.505 85 .000 5.77 5.41 Q27:&PCR&between&angry&and&upset 5.19 1.035 51.667 85 .099 5.41 .04 Q28:&PCR&between&angry&and&nervous/stressed 5.13 .918 51.292 85 .200 5.32 .07 Q29:&PCR&between&angry&and&irritable 5.16 1.105 51.367 85 .175 5.40 .07 Q30:&PCR&between&angry&and&control 5.02 .958 5.225 85 .822 5.23 .18

(8)

against   the   negative   health   implications   of   extended   HPA  axis  activation.    

Second,   research   of   Maier   and   Watkins   (2010)   suggests   plasticity   of   the   vmPFC;   it   appears   that   the   experience  alters  the  vmPFC.  This  could  suggest  that   developing   a   stronger   sense   of   control   would   offer   protection   against   the   negative   effects   of   stress.     A   relevant   subsequent   research   may   look   at   the   possibilities   to   train   our   sense   of   control.   Longitudinal   brain   imaging   could   map   possible   changes  in  the  vmPFC  structures  in  order  to  confirm   plasticity.  

 

Limitations  

Four   limitations   are   discussed   below.   First,   the   sample   group   lacked   diversity   in   terms   of   age   and   background.   Only   psychology   students   with   a   mean   age  of  20.70  (SD  =  1.64)  participated.  Because  of  such   a  select  sample  group  it  is  difficult  to  extrapolate  the   results  to  the  whole  of  society.  Below  is  argued  why.   (a)   The   number   of   male   participants   was   much   smaller  than  the  number  of  female  participants.  This   difference   can   have   an   effect   on   the   PCR   between   stress   and   control.   Women,   for   example,   report   that   they   experience   more   stress   than   men.   (b)   It   is   possible   that   students   experience   less   stress   than   people   in   another   phase   of   life.   Students   could   perceive   a   different   causal   relation   between   stress   and   control.   For   example   reporting   a   stronger   PCR   between   control   and   stress   (e.g.,   they   may   report   more  control  because  they  can  plan  their  study-­‐time   according  to  their  own  schedule).  On  the  other  hand,   may   it   be   possible   that   the   PCR   between   stress   and   control   is   fairly   weak   for   retirees,   as   they   might   not   experience   as   much   stress   as   businessmen   and   students.  (c)  The  educational  level  of  the  participants   is   above   average,   possibly   resulting   in   higher   reflection   skills   (Dochy,   Segers   &   Sluijsmans,   1999;   Mann,   Gordon   &   MacLeod,   2009).   Accordingly   they   can  have  more  knowledge  about  the  relation  between   their   thoughts,   actions   and   emotions,   which   can   result  in  a  clearer  PCR  than  when  people  don’t  have   these   abilities.  (d)   It   is   possible   the   students   are   biased  as  they  might  already  have  some  psychological   and  biological  knowledge  about  stress  and  control.  It   is  possible  that  psychology  students  therefore  report   a   stronger   PCR   more   often,   resulting   in   significant   results.   People   without   psychological   background   might   not   have   this   knowledge   and   can   therefore   report   a   weaker   PCR   or   no   PCR   at   all.     Future   research   could   best   gather   a   group   of   participants   with   approximately   as   much   men   as   women   to   minimize   the   effects   of   sex.   It   is   also   important   to   include   people   of   different   ages,   life   phases,   educational  levels  and  fields  of  profession,  to  reduce   these  possible  biases.    

Second,   the   (English)   PSS   10   item   inventory   formed  the  base  of  the  novel  Dutch  questionnaire.  A   lot  of  the  terms  used  in  the  English  PSS  were  difficult  

to  translate  to  Dutch.  Due  to  these  uncertainties  it  is   not   entirely   sure   whether   the   same   construct   was   measured   or   whether   our   follow   up   questions   were   correct.   Future   research   may   use   an   official   Dutch   translation   of   the   PSS   10   item   inventory   or   can   enable  assistance  from  Dutch  and  English  translators   to  ensure  measuring  the  right  construct.  

Third,   the   participants   were   explicitly   asked   to   report   the   strength   of   the   PCR   between   stress   and   control.  The  formulation  of  the  questions  might  have   directed   the   participant   subconsciously   into   reporting  a  PCR  while  they  otherwise  might  not  have   noticed   this   relation.   Future   research   may   use   a   longitudinal   design:   participants   would   be   asked   to   report   whether   they   perceive   different   stress-­‐   and   control-­‐related  emotions  and  feelings  each  day  for  an   extended   period   of   time.   Based   on   the   perceived   emotions   and   feelings   a   relation   between   stress   and   control  may  be  visualized.  

Fourth,   the   questionnaire   was   conducted   at   a   set   time,  most  likely  when  the  participants  experienced  a   low  level  of  stress.  Hence  they  had  to  recall  stressful   events  from  the  last  month  and  re-­‐evaluate  whether   they   experienced   stress   and/or   control   and   in   what   extend   these   influenced   each   other.   As   time   has   passed,  it  is  possible  the  memory  of  the  experienced   level   of   stress   and   control   has   faded;   therefore   making  it  more  difficult  to  precisely  report  the  PCRs.   Future   research   could   use   a   longitudinal   design   to   tackle   this   limitation   also   by   asking   participants   to   report  the  PCR  between  stress  and  control  right  after   a  stressful  event  took  place.    

 

  Conclusion    

Using  a  novel  (Dutch)  psychological  questionnaire   based   on   the   Perceived   Stress   Scale   (PSS)   10   item   inventory,   the   present   study   examined   the   PCR   between   stress   and   control.   The   results   are   in   line   with   psychological   a   biological   research   indicating   a   relationship  between  stress  and  control;  when  people   experience   control,   they   experience   less   stress,   and   when   people   experience   stress,   they   experience   less   control.   A   network   analysis   visualized   these   results   and   concluded   that   the   effect   of   control   on   stress   is   stronger   than   the   effect   of   stress   on   control.   However,   due   to   limitations   further   research   is   needed   to   support   these   findings   and   in   order   to   extrapolate   the   results   to   the   whole   of   society.   In   summary,   the   findings   from   this   study   suggest   that   people   perceive   a   causal   relation   between   control   and  stress.    

   

References    

Abelson,  J.  L.,  Khan,  S.,  Liberzon,  I.,  Erickson,  T.  M.,  &   Young,   E.   A.   (2008).   Effects   of   perceived   control  

(9)

and   cognitive   coping   on   endocrine   stress   responses   to   pharmacological   activation.   Biological  psychiatry,  64(8),  701-­‐707.  

Almeida,  D.M.  (2005).  Resilience  and  vulnerability  to   daily   stressors   asses   via   diary   methods.   Current   Directions  in  Psychological  Science,  14(2),  64-­‐68.    Arnsten,  A.  F.  (2009).  Stress  signalling  pathways  that  

impair   prefrontal   cortex   structure   and   function.   Nature  Reviews  Neuroscience,  10(6),  410-­‐422.   Averill,   J.   R.   (1973).   Personal   control   over   aversive  

stimuli  and  its  relationship  to  stress.  Psychological   bulletin,  80,  286-­‐303.  

Borsboom,   D.,   &   Cramer,   A.   O.   (2013).   Network   analysis:  an  integrative  approach  to  the  structure   of   psychopathology.   Annual   review   of   clinical   psychology,  9,  91-­‐121.  

Boyce,   W.T.   &   Ellis,   B.J.   (2005).   Biological   sensitivity   context   I:   An   evolutionary-­‐developmental   theory   of   the   origins   and   functions   of   stress   reactivity.   Development  and  Psychopathology,  17,  271-­‐301.   Cohen,   S.,   Kamarck,   T.,   &   Mermelstein,   R.   (1983).   A  

global   measure   of   perceived   stress.   Journal   of   health  and  social  behavior,  24,  385-­‐396.  

Dedovic,   K.,   Duchesne,   A.,   Andrews,   J.,   Engert,   V.,   &   Pruessner,   J.   C.   (2009).   The   brain   and   the   stress   axis:  the  neural  correlates  of  cortisol  regulation  in   response  to  stress.  Neuroimage,  47(3),  864-­‐871.   DeLongis,  A.,  Folkman,  S.,  &  Lazarus,  R.  S.  (1988).  The  

impact   of   daily   stress   on   health   and   mood   psychological   and   social   resources   as   mediators.   Journal  of  personality  and  social  psychology,  54(3),   486.  

Del   Giudice,   M.,   Ellis,   B.   J.,   &   Shirtcliff,   E.   A.   (2011).   The   adaptive   calibration   model   of   stress   responsivity.   Neuroscience   &   Biobehavioral   Reviews,  35(7),  1562-­‐1592.  

Dickerson,   S.   S.,   &   Kemeny,   M.   E.   (2004).   Acute   stressors   and   cortisol   responses:   a   theoretical   integration   and   synthesis   of   laboratory   research.   Psychological  bulletin,  130(3),  355.    

Diehl,   M.,   &   Hay,   E.   L.   (2010).   Risk   and   resilience   factors   in   coping   with   daily   stress   in   adulthood:   the   role   of   age,   self-­‐concept   incoherence,   and   personal   control.   Developmental   psychology,   46(5),  1132.

   

Dochy,  F.  J.  R.  C.,  Segers,  M.,  &  Sluijsmans,  D.  (1999).   The  use  of  self-­‐,  peer  and  co-­‐assessment  in  higher   education:   A   review.   Studies   in   Higher   education,   24(3),  331-­‐350.

 

Ellis,   B.   J.,   Jackson,   J.   J.,   &   Boyce,   W.   T.   (2006).   The   stress   response   systems:   Universality   and   adaptive   individual   differences.   Developmental   Review,  26(2),  175-­‐212.  

Epskamp,  S.  (n.d.).  ‘qgraph’  and  ‘Tutorials’.  Consulted   on  May  3rd  2016,  from  http://sachaepskamp.com/  

Frewen,  P.  A.,  Allen,  S.  L.,  Lanius,  R.  A.,  &  Neufeld,  R.   W.  (2012).  Perceived  Causal  Relations  Novel   Methodology  for  Assessing  Client  Attributions   About  Causal  Associations  Between  Variables   Including  Symptoms  and  Functional  Impairment.   Assessment,  19(4),  480-­‐493.  

Greenberg,  N.,  Carr,  J.  A.,  &  Summers,  C.  H.  (2002).   Causes  and  consequences  of  stress.  Integrative   and  Comparative  Biology,  42(3),  508-­‐516.  

Hofmann,   S.   G.   (2005).   Perception   of   control   over   anxiety   mediates   the   relation   between   catastrophic   thinking   and   social   anxiety   in   social   phobia.   Behaviour   Research   and   Therapy,   43(7),   885-­‐895.  

Maier,   S.   F.,   &   Watkins,   L.   R.   (2010).   Role   of   the   medial  prefrontal  cortex  in  coping  and  resilience.   Brain  research,  1355,  52-­‐60.    

Mann,  K.,  Gordon,  J.,  &  MacLeod,  A.  (2009).  Reflection   and   reflective   practice   in   health   professions   education:  a  systematic  review.  Advances  in  health   sciences  education,  14(4),  595-­‐621.  

McEwen,  B.  S.  (1998).  Protective  and  damaging   effects  of  stress  mediators.  New  England  journal  of   medicine,  338(3),  171-­‐179.  

McKlveen,  J.  M.,  Myers,  B.,  &  Herman,  J.  P.  (2015).  The   medial   prefrontal   cortex:   coordinator   of   autonomic,   neuroendocrine   and   behavioural   responses  to  stress.  Journal  of  neuroendocrinology,   27(6),  446-­‐456.  

Miller,   E.   K.,   &   Cohen,   J.   D.   (2001).   An   integrative   theory   of   prefrontal   cortex   function.   Annual   review  of  neuroscience,  24(1),  167-­‐202.  

Thompson,   S.   C.   (1981).   Will   it   hurt   less   if   i   can   control  it?  A  complex  answer  to  a  simple  question.   Psychological  bulletin,  90,  89-­‐101.  

Wingenfeld,   K.,   &   Wolf,   O.   T.   (2011).   HPA   axis   alterations   in   mental   disorders:   impact   on   memory   and   its   relevance   for   therapeutic   interventions.   CNS   neuroscience   &   therapeutics,   17(6),  714-­‐722.  

   

(10)

APPENDIX  A:  Dutch  questionnaire  used  in  the  study    

Q1:  Hoe  vaak  was  u  de  afgelopen  maand  van  streek  omdat  er  iets  onverwachts  gebeurde?    

Q2:  Hoe  vaak  had  u  de  afgelopen  maand  het  gevoel  dat  u  geen  controle  had  over  de  belangrijke  dingen  in   uw  leven?    

Q3:  Hoe  vaak  voelde  u  zich  de  afgelopen  maand  nerveus  of  gespannen?    

Q4:  Hoe  vaak  heeft  u  zich  de  afgelopen  maand  zeker  gevoeld  over  uw  vermogen  om  met  persoonlijke   problemen  om  te  kunnen  gaan?    

Q5:  Hoe  vaak  had  u  de  afgelopen  maand  het  gevoel  dat  de  dingen  verliepen  zoals  u  wilde?    

Q6:  Hoe  vaak  had  u  de  afgelopen  maand  het  gevoel  dat  u  niet  om  kon  gaan  met  alle  dingen  die  u  moest   doen?    

Q7:  Hoe  vaak  was  u  de  afgelopen  maand  in  staat  om  irritaties  in  uw  leven  onder  controle  te  houden?     Q8:  Hoe  vaak  had  u  de  afgelopen  maand  het  gevoel  dat  u  alles  onder  controle  had?  Nooit  Bijna  nooit  Soms     Q9:  Hoe  vaak  was  u  de  afgelopen  maand  boos  om  dingen  die  buiten  uw  controle  lagen?    

Q10:  Hoe  vaak  had  u  de  afgelopen  maand  het  gevoel  dat  problemen  zich  zo  hoog  opstapelde  dat  u  ze  niet   aan  kon?    

 

Q11:  U  voelde  zich  de  afgelopen  maand  …  van  streek  omdat  er  iets  onverwachts  gebeurde  en  u  voelde  zich   …  nerveus  of  gespannen.  Geef  aan  in  welke  mate  dit  elkaar  beïnvloedde.  Doordat  ik  …  van  streek  was,   was  ik  ...  nerveus  of  gespannen.    

 

Q12:  U  voelde  zich  de  afgelopen  maand  …  van  streek  omdat  er  iets  onverwachts  gebeurde  en  u  was  …  in   staat  om  irritaties  in  uw  leven  onder  controle  te  houden.  Geef  aan  in  welke  mate  dit  elkaar  beïnvloedde.   Doordat  ik  …  van  streek  was,  was  ik  ...  geïrriteerd.    

 

Q13:  U  voelde  zich  de  afgelopen  maand  …  van  streek  omdat  er  iets  onverwachts  gebeurde  en  u  had  …  het   gevoel  dat  u  alles  onder  controle  had.  Geef  aan  in  welke  mate  dit  elkaar  beïnvloedde.  Doordat  ik  …  van   streek  was,  had  ik  ...  alles  onder  controle.    

 

Q14:  U  voelde  zich  de  afgelopen  maand  …    van  streek  omdat  er  iets  onverwachts  gebeurde  en  u  was  …     boos  om  dingen  die  buiten  uw  controle  lagen.  Geef  aan  in  welke  mate  dit  elkaar  beïnvloedde.  Doordat  ik   …  van  streek  was,  was  ik  ...  boos.    

 

Q15:  U  voelde  zich  de  afgelopen  maand  …  nerveus  of  gespannen  en  u  was  …  van  streek  omdat  er  iets   onverwachts  gebeurde.  Geef  aan  in  welke  mate  dit  elkaar  beïnvloedde.  Doordat  ik  …  nerveus  of   gespannen  was,  was  ik  ...  van  streek.    

 

Q16:  U  voelde  zich  de  afgelopen  maand  …  nerveus  of  gespannen  en  u  was  …  in  staat  om  irritaties  in  uw   leven  onder  controle  te  houden.  Geef  aan  in  welke  mate  dit  elkaar  beïnvloedde.  Doordat  ik  …  nerveus  of   gespannen  was,  was  ik  ...  geïrriteerd.    

 

Q17:  U  voelde  zich  de  afgelopen  maand  …  nerveus  of  gespannen  en  u  had  …  het  gevoel  dat  u  alles  onder   controle  had.  Geef  aan  in  welke  mate  dit  elkaar  beïnvloedde.  Doordat  ik  …  nerveus  of  gespannen  was,   had  ik  ...  alles  onder  controle.    

 

Q18:  U  voelde  zich  de  afgelopen  maand  …  nerveus  of  gespannen  en  u  was  …  boos  omdat  dingen  buiten  uw   controle  lagen.  Geef  aan  in  welke  mate  dit  elkaar  beïnvloedde.  Doordat  ik  …  nerveus  of  gespannen  was,   was  ik  ...  boos.    

 

Q19:  U  was  de  afgelopen  maand  …  in  staat  om  irritaties  in  uw  leven  onder  controle  te  houden  en  u  was  …   van  streek  omdat  er  iets  onverwachts  gebeurde.  Geef  aan  in  welke  mate  dit  elkaar  beïnvloedde.  Doordat   ik  …  geïrriteerd  was,  was  ik  ...  van  streek.    

 

Q20:  U  was  de  afgelopen  maand  …  in  staat  om  irritaties  in  uw  leven  onder  controle  te  houden  en  u  voelde   zich  …  nerveus  of  gespannen.  Geef  aan  in  welke  mate  dit  elkaar  beïnvloedde.  Doordat  ik  …  geïrriteerd   was,  was  ik  ...  nerveus  of  gespannen.    

 

Q21:  U  was  de  afgelopen  maand  …  in  staat  om  irritaties  in  uw  leven  onder  controle  te  houden  en  u  had  …   het  gevoel  dat  u  alles  onder  controle  had.  Geef  aan  in  welke  mate  dit  elkaar  beïnvloedde.  Doordat  ik  …  

(11)

geïrriteerd  was,  had  ik  ...  alles  onder  controle.      

Q22:  U  was  de  afgelopen  maand  …  in  staat  om  irritaties  in  uw  leven  onder  controle  te  houden  en  u  was  …   boos  om  dingen  die  buiten  uw  controle  lagen.  Geef  aan  in  welke  mate  dit  elkaar  beïnvloedde.  Doordat  ik   …  geïrriteerd  was,  was  ik  ...  boos.    

 

Q23:  U  had  de  afgelopen  maand  …  het  gevoel  dat  u  alles  onder  controle  had  en  u  …  van  streek  omdat  er   iets  onverwachts  gebeurde.  Geef  aan  in  welke  mate  dit  elkaar  beïnvloedde.  Doordat  ik  …  alles  onder   controle  had,  was  ik  ...  van  streek.    

 

Q24:  U  had  de  afgelopen  maand  …  het  gevoel  dat  u  alles  onder  controle  had  en  u  voelde  zich  …  nerveus  of   gespannen.  Geef  aan  in  welke  mate  dit  elkaar  beïnvloedde.  Doordat  ik  …  alles  onder  controle  had,  was   ik  ...  nerveus  of  gespannen.    

 

Q25:  U  had  de  afgelopen  maand  …  het  gevoel  dat  u  alles  onder  controle  had  en  u  was  …  in  staat  om  

irritaties  in  uw  leven  onder  controle  te  houden.  Geef  aan  in  welke  mate  dit  elkaar  beïnvloedde.  Doordat  ik   …  alles  onder  controle  had,  was  ik  ...  geïrriteerd.    

 

Q26:  U  had  de  afgelopen  maand  …  het  gevoel  dat  u  alles  onder  controle  had  en  u  was  …  boos  om  dingen   die  buiten  uw  controle  lagen.  Geef  aan  in  welke  mate  dit  elkaar  beïnvloedde.  Doordat  ik  …  alles  onder   controle  had,  was  ik  ...  boos.    

 

Q27:  U  was  de  afgelopen  maand  …  boos  om  dingen  die  buiten  uw  controle  lagen  en  u  was  …  van  streek   omdat  er  iets  onverwachts  gebeurde.  Geef  aan  in  welke  mate  dit  elkaar  beïnvloedde.  Doordat  ik  …  boos   was,  was  ik  ...  van  streek.    

 

Q28:  U  was  de  afgelopen  maand  …  boos  om  dingen  die  buiten  uw  controle  lagen  en  u  was  …  nerveus  of   gespannen.  Geef  aan  in  welke  mate  dit  elkaar  beïnvloedde.  Doordat  ik  …  boos  was,  was  ik  ...  nerveus  of   gespannen.    

 

Q29:  U  was  de  afgelopen  maand  …  boos  om  dingen  die  buiten  uw  controle  lagen  en  u  was  …  in  staat  om   irritaties  in  uw  leven  onder  controle  te  houden.  Geef  aan  in  welke  mate  dit  elkaar  beïnvloedde.  Doordat  ik   …  boos  was,  was  ik  ...  geïrriteerd.    

 

Q30:  U  was  de  afgelopen  maand  …  boos  om  dingen  die  buiten  uw  controle  lagen  en  u  had  …  het  gevoel  dat   u  alles  onder  controle  had.  Geef  aan  in  welke  mate  dit  elkaar  beïnvloedde.  Doordat  ik  …  boos  was,  had  ik   ...  alles  onder  controle.    

   

(12)

APPENDIX  B:  Englisch  translation  of  the  items  used  in  the  Dutch  questions    

Q1:  Upset.   Q2:  No  control.  

Q3:  Nervous  and  “stressed”  

Q4:  Able  to  handle  personal  problems.   Q5:  Feeling  things  were  going  your  way.  

Q6:  Not  able  to  cope  wit  hall  the  things  that  had  to  be  done.   Q7:  Irritable.  

Q8:  Control.   Q9:  Angered.  

Q10:  Could  not  overcome  difficulties.    

Q11:  PCR  between  upset  and  nervous/stressed.   Q12:  PCR  between  upset  and  irritable.  

Q13:  PCR  between  upset  and  control.   Q14:  PCR  between  upset  and  angry.  

Q15:  PCR  between  nervous/stressed  and  upset.   Q16:  PCR  between  nervous/stressed  and  irritable.   Q17:  PCR  between  nervous/stressed  and  control.   Q18:  PCR  between  nervous/stressed  and  angry.   Q19:  PCR  between  irritable  and  upset.  

Q20:  PCR  between  irritable  and  nervous/stressed.   Q21:  PCR  between  irritable  and  control.  

Q22:  PCR  between  irritable  and  angry.   Q23:  PCR  between  control  and  upset.  

Q24:  PCR  between  control  and  nervous/stressed.   Q25:  PCR  between  control  and  irritable.  

Q26:  PCR  between  control  and  angry.   Q27:  PCR  between  angry  and  upset.  

Q28:  PCR  between  angry  and  nervous/stressed.   Q29:  PCR  between  angry  and  irritable.  

Q30:  PCR  between  angry  and  control.    

Referenties

GERELATEERDE DOCUMENTEN

Het middel kan in het pakket voor een subpopulatie maar dan moeten er garanties zijn dat alleen deze patiënten het middel krijgen.. Een lid vult daarop aan dat dit niet alleen

When a management control package is perceived as predominately negative, hence more constraining controls relatively to facilitating controls, it could negatively affect management

a on what grounds are UMAs selected to be placed in the pilot, and what indications are there after their placement, that these are really youngsters who have been brought to

Therefore, a negative moderating effect of transactional leadership is being expected on all management control systems and the basic psychological needs, whereby a

In particular, the effects of Simons’ levers-of-control (i.e. beliefs systems, boundary systems, diagnostic control systems and interactive control systems) for two different

To measure the relation between formal and informal environmental management control systems (EMCS) and the score on the TB, and the moderating effect of the processing

In this paper, we presented a Networked Control System (NCS) framework for analyzing the effects of network- induced impairments on Cooperative Adaptive Cruise Con- trol (CACC)

• Rodney Brooks: “The generalisation we are facing is that we people are machines, and as such subjected to the same technological manipulation that we now routinely apply