• No results found

Stepping back while staying engaged: On the cognitive effects of obstacles - References

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Share "Stepping back while staying engaged: On the cognitive effects of obstacles - References"

Copied!
24
0
0

Bezig met laden.... (Bekijk nu de volledige tekst)

Hele tekst

(1)

UvA-DARE is a service provided by the library of the University of Amsterdam (https://dare.uva.nl)

Stepping back while staying engaged: On the cognitive effects of obstacles

Marguc-Steck, J.

Publication date 2012

Link to publication

Citation for published version (APA):

Marguc-Steck, J. (2012). Stepping back while staying engaged: On the cognitive effects of obstacles.

General rights

It is not permitted to download or to forward/distribute the text or part of it without the consent of the author(s) and/or copyright holder(s), other than for strictly personal, individual use, unless the work is under an open content license (like Creative Commons).

Disclaimer/Complaints regulations

If you believe that digital publication of certain material infringes any of your rights or (privacy) interests, please let the Library know, stating your reasons. In case of a legitimate complaint, the Library will make the material inaccessible and/or remove it from the website. Please Ask the Library: https://uba.uva.nl/en/contact, or a letter to: Library of the University of Amsterdam, Secretariat, Singel 425, 1012 WP Amsterdam, The Netherlands. You will be contacted as soon as possible.

(2)

            References    

(3)

Adriaanse,  M.  A.,  Oettingen,  G.,  Gollwitzer,  P.  M.,  Hennes,  E.  P.,  De  Ridder,  D.  T.  D.  ,  &  De     Witt,  J.  B.  F.  (2010).    When   planning   is   not   enough:   Fighting   unhealthy   snacking     habits   by   mental   contrasting   with   implementation   intentions   (MCII).  

  European  Journal  of  Social  Psychology,  40,  1277–1293.  doi:  10.1002/ejsp.730  

Aiken,   L.   S.,   &   West,   S.   G.   (1991).   Multiple   Regression:   Testing   and   interpreting  

  interactions.  Newbury  Park,  CA:  Sage.  

Alter,   A.   L.   &   Oppenheimer,   D.   M.   (2008).   Effects   of   fluency   on   psychological   distance     and  mental  construal  (or  why  New  York  is  a  large  city,  but  New  York  is  a  civilized     jungle).   Psychological   Science,   19,   161-­‐167.   doi:10.1111/j.1467-­‐9280.2008.   02062.x  

Alter,   A.   L.,   &   Oppenheimer,   D.   M.   (2009).   Uniting   the   tribes   of   fluency   to   form   a     metacognitive   nation.   Personality  and  Social  Psychology  Review,  13(3),   219-­‐235.  

  doi:10.1177/1088868309341564  

Atkinson   (1953).   The   achievement   motive   and   recall   for   interrupted   and   completed     tasks.  Journal  of  Experimental  Psychology,  46,  381-­‐390.  

Ayduk,   O.,   &   Kross,   E.   (2009).   Asking   ‘why’   from   a   distance   facilitates   emotional     processing:  A  reanalysis     of   Wimalaweera   and   Moulds   (2008).   Behaviour  

  Research  and  Therapy,  47,  88–92.  doi:10.1016/j.brat.2008.06.014  

Ayduk,  O.,  &  Kross,  E.  (2010a).  Analyzing  negative  experiences  without  ruminating:  The     role  of  self-­‐distancing  in  enabling  adaptive  self-­‐reflection.  Social  and  Personality  

  Psychology  Compass,  4(10),  841-­‐854.  doi:  10.1111/j.1751-­‐9004.2010.00301.x  

Ayduk,   O.,   &   Kross,   E.   (2010b).   From   a   distance:   Implications   of   spontaneous   self-­‐   distancing   for   adaptive   self-­‐reflection.   Journal   of   Personality   and   Social  

  Psychology,  98,  809–829.  doi:  10.1037/a0019205  

Baas,  M.,  De  Dreu,  C.K.W.,  &  Nijstad,  B.A.  (2008).  A  meta-­‐analysisof  25  years  of  mood-­‐   creativity  research:    Hedonic   tone,   activation,   or   regulatory   focus?   Psychological  

  Bulletin,  134,  779-­‐806.  doi:  10.1037/a0012815  

Bandura,   A.   (1993).   Perceived   self-­‐efficacy   in   cognitive   development   and   functioning.  

(4)

Bandura,  A.  (1998).  Personal  and  collective  efficacy  in  human  adaptation  and  change.  In     J.  G.  Adair,  D.    Belanger,   &   K.   L.   Dion   (Eds.),   Advances   in   psychological   science:     Vol.   1.   Personal,   social   and   cultural   aspects   (pp.   51-­‐71).   Hove,   UK:   Psychology     Press.    

Bar-­‐Anan,  Y.,  Liberman,  N.,  &  Trope,  Y.  (2006).  The  association  between  psychological     distance  and    construal  level:  Evidence  from  an  implicit  association  test.  Journal  

  of   Experimental   Psychology:   General,   135,   609-­‐622.   doi:10.1037/0096-­‐3445.

  135.4.609  

Barsalou,  L.W.  (2008).  Grounded  cognition.  Annual  Review  of  Psychology,  59,  617-­‐645.  

  doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093639  

Bauer,   D.   J.   &   Curran,   P.   J.   (2005).   Probing   interactions   in   fixed   and   multilevel     regression:   Inferential   and   graphical   techniques.  Multivariate   Behavioral  

  Research,  40,  373–400.  doi:10.1207/s15327906mbr4003_5  

Beckman,  J.,  &  Kazén,  M.  (1994).  Action  and  state  orientation  and  the  performance  of     top   athletes.   In   J.   Kuhl   &   J.   Beckmann   (Eds.),   Volition   and   personality:   Action  

  versus  state  orientation  (pp.  439-­‐451).   Göttingen:  Hogrefe.  

Beeman,   M.   (1998).   Coarse   semantic   coding   and   discourse   comprehension.     In   M.     Beeman   &   C.   Chiarello   (Eds.),   Right   hemisphere   language   comprehension:  

  Perspectives  from  cognitive  neuroscience  (pp.     255-­‐284).  Mahwah,  NJ:  Erlbaum.      

Berkowitz,   L.   (1989).   Frustration-­‐aggression   hypothesis:   Examination   and     reformulation.   Psychological   Bulletin,   106,   59-­‐73.   doi:10.1037/0033-­‐2909.   106.1.59  

Boekaerts,  M.  (1994)  Action  control:  How  relevant  is  it  for  classroom  learning?  J.  Kuhl  &     J.  Beckmann  (Eds.),    Volition   and   personality:   Action   versus   state   orientation   (pp.     427-­‐435).  Göttingen:  Hogrefe.  

Bongers,   K.   C   .   A.,   Dijksterhuis,   A.   &   Spears,   R.   (2008).   On  the  role  of  consciousness  in  

  goal  pursuit.    Unpublished  manuscript.  

Boroditsky,   L.   (2000).   Metaphoric   structuring:   Understanding   time   through   spatial     metaphors.  Cognition,  75,  1–28.  doi:10.1016/S0010-­‐0277(99)00073-­‐6  

(5)

Bowden,   E.   M.   &   Jung-­‐Beeman,   M.   (2003)   Aha!   Insight   experience   correlates   with     solution  activation  in  the     right   hemisphere.   Psychonomic   Bulletin   &   Review,   10,     730-­‐737.  doi:10.3758/BF03196539  

Boyce,  P.  R.  (1974).  Users'  assessments  of  a  landscaped  office.  Journal  of  Architectural  

  Research,  3,  44-­‐62.  

Brehm,   J.   W.,   &   Self,   E.   A.   (1989).   The   intensity   of   motivation.   Annual   Review   of  

  Psychology,  40,  109-­‐131.  doi:10.1146/annurev.ps.40.020189.000545  

Burgess,  C.,  &  Simpson,  G.  B.  (1988).  Cerebral  hemispheric  mechanisms  in  the  retrieval     of  ambiguous  word    meanings.   Brain   and   Language,   33,   86-­‐103.     doi:10.1016/0093-­‐934X(88)90056-­‐9    

Burnstein,  E.  &  Worchel,  P.  (1962).  Arbitrariness  of  frustration  and  its  consequences  for     aggression   in   a   social   situation.   Journal   of   Personality,   30,   528-­‐541.   doi:10.   1111/j.1467-­‐6494.1962.tb01687.x  

Butterfield,   E.   C.   (1964).   The   interruption   of   tasks:   Methodological,   factual   and     theoretical  issues.  Psychological  Bulletin,  62,  309–322.  doi:10.1037/h0039873   Cacioppo,  J.  T.,  Berntson,  G.  G.,  &  Crites,  S.  L.,  Jr.,  (1996).  Social  neuroscience:  Principles     of  psychophysiological  arousal  and  response.  In  E.  T.  Higgins  &  A.  W.  Kruglanski     (Eds.),  Social    psychology:   Handbook   of   basic   principles   (pp.   72-­‐101).   New   York,     NY:  Guilford.  

Carver,   C.   S.   (1996).   Cognitive   interference   and   the   structure   of   behavior.   In   I.   G.     Sarason,   G.   R.   Pierce,   &   B.   R.   Sarason   (Eds.),   Cognitive   interference:   Theories,  

  methods,  and  findings  (pp.  25–45).  Mahwah,  NJ:  Erlbaum.  

Carver,  C.  S.  &  Scheier,  M.  F.  (1990).  Origins  and  functions  of  positive  and  negative  affect:     A   control-­‐process   view.   Psychological   Review,   97,   19-­‐35.   doi:10.1037/0033-­‐   295X.97.1.19  

Coenen,  L.  H.  M.,  Hedebouw,  L.,  &  Semin,  G.  R.  (2006).  Measuring  language  abstraction:     The   Linguistic   Category   Model   (LCM).   Retrieved   March   29,   2011,   from  

  http://www.cratylus.org/Text/1111548454250-­‐3815/pC/1111473983125-­‐

(6)

De  Dreu,  C.  K.  W.,  Baas,  M.  ,  &  Nijstad,  B.  A.  (2008).  Hedonic  tone  and  activation  in  the     mood-­‐creativity   link:   Towards   a   dual   pathway   to   creativity   model.   Journal  

  of   Personality   and   Social   Psychology,   94,   739-­‐756.   doi:   10.1037/0022-­‐

  3514.94.5.739  

De   Dreu,   C.   K.   W.,   &   Carnevale,   P.   J.   D.   (2003).   Motivational   bases   for   information     processing  and  strategic  choice  in  conflict  and  negotiation.  In  M.P.  Zanna  (Ed.),  

  Advances   in   Experimental   Social   Psychology   (Vol.   35,   pp.   235   -­‐291).   New   York:  

  Academic  Press.  

De  Dreu,  C.  K.  W.,  Giacomantonio,  M.,  Shalvi,  S.,  &  Sligte,  D.  J.  (2009).  Getting  stuck  or     stepping   back:   Effects   of   obstacles   and   construal   level   in   the   negotiation   of     creative   solutions.   Journal   of   Experimental   Social   Psychology,   45,   542–548.     doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2009.01.001  

De   Dreu,   C.   K.   W.   &   Nijstad,   B.   A.   (2008).   Mental   set   and   creative   thought   in   social     conflict:  Threat  rigidity  versus  motivated  focus.  Journal  of  Personality  and  Social  

  Psychology,  95,  648-­‐661.  doi:  10.1037/0022-­‐3514.95.3.648  

Derryberry,  D.,  &  Reed,  M.  A.  (1998).  Anxiety  and  attentional  focusing:  Trait,  state  and     hemispheric    infuences.   Personality   and   Individual   Differences,   25,   745–761.     doi:10.1016/S0191-­‐8869(98)00117-­‐2  

Dollard,  J.,  Doob,  L.,  Miller,  N.,  Mowrer,  O.,  &  Sears,  R.  (1939).  Frustration  and  aggression.     New  Haven,  CT:  Yale  University  Press.    

Driscoll,  R.,  Davis,  K.  E.,  &  Lipetz,  M.  E.  (1972).  Parental  interference  and  romantic  love:     The  Romeo  and  Juliet  effect.  Journal  of  Personality  and  Social  Psychology,  24,  1-­‐10.  

  doi:10.1037/h0033373  

Duckworth,   A.   L.,   Grant,   H.,   Loew,   B.,   Oettingen,   G.,   &   Gollwitzer,   P.   M.   (2011).   Self-­‐   regulation   strategies   improve   self-­‐discipline   in   adolescents:   Benefits   of   mental     contrasting  and  implementation    intentions.   Educational   Psychology,   31,   17-­‐26.     doi:  10.1080/01443410.2010.506003  

Duckworth,   A.   L,   &   Quinn,   P.   D.   (2009).   Development   and   validation   of   the   Short   Grit     Scale   (Grit-­‐   S).   Journal   of   Personality   Assessment,   91,   166-­‐174.  

(7)

Easterbrook,  J.  A.  (1959).  The  effect  of  emotion  on  cue  utilization  and  the  organization     of  behavior.  Psychological  Review,  66,  183-­‐201.  doi:  10.1037/h0047707  

Eyal,   T.,   Sagristano,   M.   D.,   Trope,   Y.,   Liberman,   N.,   &   Chaiken,   S.   (2009).   When   values     matter:  Expressing    values  in  behavioral  intentions  for  the  near  vs.  distant  future.  

  Journal  of  Experimental  Social  Psychology,  45,  35–43.    doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2008.07.

  023  

Fazio,   R.   H.   (1990).   A   practical   guide   to   the   use   of   response   latencies   in   social     psychological  research.  In  C.  Hendrick  &  M.  S.  Clark  (Eds.),  Review  of  personality  

  and  social  psychology  (Vol.  11,  pp.  74–97).  Newbury  Park:  Sage.  

Finke,   R.   A.   (1995).   Creative   insight   and   preinventive   forms.   In   R.   J.   Sternberg   &   J.   E.     Davidson  (Eds.),  The  nature  of  insight  (pp.  255–280).  Cambridge,  MA:  MIT  Press.   Fishbach,   A.   (2009).   The   function   of   value   in   self-­‐regulation.   Journal   of   Consumer  

  Psychology,  19,129-­‐133.  doi:10.1016/j.jcps.2009.02.005  

Fishbach,   A.,   Friedman,   R.   S.,   &   Kruglanski,   A.   W.   (2003).   Leading   us   not   unto     temptation:   Momentary   allurements   elicit   overriding   goal   activation.   Journal  of  

  Personality   and   Social   Psychology,   84,   296–309.   doi:   10.1037/0022-­‐3514.

  84.2.296  

Fishbach,  A.,  &  Trope,  Y.  (2005).  The  substitutability  of  external  control  and  self-­‐control     in  overcoming  temptation.  Journal  of  Experimental  Social  Psychology,  41,  256-­‐270.     doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2004.07.002    

Fiske,   S.   T.,   &   Neuberg,   S.   L.   (1990).   A   continuum   of   impression   formation,   from     category-­‐based   to   individuating   processes:   Influences   of   information   and     motivation   on   attention   and   interpretation.   In   M.   P.   Zanna   (Ed.),   Advances   in  

  experimental  social  psychology  (Vol.  23,  pp.  1-­‐74).  New  York,  NY:     Academic  

  Press.  

Fitzsimons,   G.   M.,   &   Fishbach,   A.   (2010).   Shifting   closeness:   Interpersonal   effects   of     personal  goal  progress.    Journal  of  Personality  and  Social  Psychology.  98,  535–549.  

  doi:10.1037/a0018581  

Folkman,   S.   (1984).   Personal   control   and   stress   and   coping   processes:   A   theoretical     analysis.  Journal  of    Personality   and   Social   Psychology,   46(4),   839-­‐852.   doi:     10.1037/0022-­‐3514.46.4.839  

(8)

Förster,   J.   (2009).   Relations   between   perceptual   and   conceptual   scope:   How   global     versus  local  processing  fits  a  focus  on  similarity  versus  dissimilarity.  Journal  of  

  Experimental  Psychology:  General,  138,  88-­‐111.  doi:10.1037/a0014484  

Förster   J.   (in   press).   Local   and   global   cross-­‐modal   influences   between   vision   and     hearing,  tasting,  smelling     or   touching.   Journal   of   Experimental   Psychology:  

  General.  doi:10.1037/a0023175    

Förster   ,   J.   &   Dannenberg   ,   L.   (2010a).   GLOMOsys:   A   systems   account   of   global   versus     local  processing.     Psychological  Inquiry,  21,  175–197.doi:    10.1080/1047840X.

  2010.487849  

Förster,   J.   &   Dannenberg,   L.   (2010b).   GLOMOsys:   Specifications   of   a   global   model   on     processing  styles.  Psychological  Inquiry,  21,  257-­‐269.    doi:10.1080/1047840X.

  2010.507989  

Förster,  J.  &  Denzler,  M.  (in  press).  Sense  Creative!  The  impact  of  global  and  local  vision,     hearing,  touching,     tasting  and  smelling  on  creative  and  analytic  thought.  Social  

  Psychological  and  Personality  Science.  doi:10.1177/1948550611410890  

Förster,  J.,  Friedman,  R.,  &  Liberman,  N.  (2004).  Temporal  construal  effects  on  abstract     and  concrete  thinking:  Consequences  for  insight  and  creative  cognition.  Journal  

  of   Personality   and   Social   Psychology,   87,   177-­‐189.   doi:10.1037/0022-­‐

  3514.87.2.177  

Förster,  J.,  Friedman,  R.,  Özelsel,  A.,  &  Denzler,  M.  (2006).  Enactment  of  approach  and     avoidance  behavior  influences  the  scope  of  perceptual  and  conceptual  attention.  

  Journal   of   Experimental   Social   Psychology,   42,   133-­‐146.   doi:10.1016/

  j.jesp.2005.02.004  

Förster,  J.,  &  Higgins,  E.  T.  (2005).  How  global  vs.  local  processing  fits  regulatory  focus.  

  Psychological  Science,  16,  631-­‐636.  doi:  10.1111/j.1467-­‐9280.2005.01586.x  

Förster,   J.,   &   Liberman,   N.   (2009).   Goal   gradients:   Challenges   to   a   basic   principle   of     motivation.   In   J.   Forgas,   R.   Baumeister,   &   D.   Tice   (Eds.),   The  Psychology  of  Self-­‐

  Regulation  (pp.  147-­‐162).  The  Sydney  Symposium  of  Social  Psychology.  

Förster,  J.,  Liberman,  N.,  &  Friedman,  R.  (2007).  Seven  principles  of  goal  activation:  A     systematic   approach   to   distinguishing   goal   priming   from   priming   of   non-­‐goal  

(9)

  constructs.  Personality  and  Social  Psychology  Review,  11,  211-­‐233.    doi:10.1177/

  1088868307303029  

Förster,   J.,   Liberman,   N.,   &   Higgins,   E.T.   (2005).   Accessibility   from   active   and   fulfilled     goals.  Journal  of  Experimental  Social  Psychology,  41,  220-­‐239.  doi:    10.1016/j.jesp.   2004.06.009  

Förster,  J.,  Liberman,  N.,  &  Shapira.  O.  (2009).  Preparing  for  novel  versus  familiar  events:     Shifts  in  global  and  local  processing.  Journal  of  Experimental  Psychology:  General,  

  138,  383-­‐399.  doi:10.1037/a0015748  

Förster,   J.,   Marguc,   J.,   Gillebaart,   M.   (2010).   Novelty   Categorization   Theory.  Social  and  

  Personality   Psychology   Compass,   4,   736–755.   doi:10.1111/j.1751-­‐9004.2010.

  00289.x  

Fredrickson,   B.   L.   (2001).   The   role   of   positive   emotions   in   positive   psychology:   The     broaden-­‐and-­‐build    theory  of  positive  emotions.  American  Psychologist,  56,  218-­‐   226.  doi:10.1037/0003-­‐066X.56.3.218  

Fredrickson,   B.   L.,   &   Branigan,   C.   (2005).   Positive   emotions   broaden   the   scope   of     attention   and   thought-­‐action   repertoires.   Cognition   and   Emotion,   19,   313-­‐332.  

  doi:10.1080/02699930441000238  

Friedman,  R.,  Fishbach,  A.,  Förster,  J.,  &  Werth,  L.  (2003).  Attentional  priming  effects  on     creativity.   Creativity   Research   Journal,   15,   277-­‐286.   doi:10.1207/   S15326934CRJ152&3_18  

Friedman,  R.,  &  Förster,  J.  (2000).  The  effects  of  approach  and  avoidance  motor  actions     on  the  elements  of     creative  insight.  Journal  of  Personality  and  Social  Psychology,  

  79,  477-­‐492.  doi:10.1037/0022-­‐3514.79.4.477  

Friedman,  R.,  &  Förster,  J.  (2008).  Activation  and  measurement  of  motivational  states.     In   Elliott   (Ed.),   Handbook  of  approach  and  avoidance  motivation   (pp.   235-­‐246).     Mawah,  NJ:  Lawrence  Erlbaum  Associates.  

Friedman   ,   R.   &   Förster   ,   J.   (2010).   Implicit   affective   cues   and   attentional   tuning:   An     integrative  review.    Psychological  Bulletin,  136,  875–893.  doi:10.1037/a0020495  

(10)

Fujita,  K.  (2008).  Seeing  the  forest  beyond  the  trees:  A  construal  level  approach  to  self-­‐   control.  Social  and  Personality  Psychology  Compass,  2,  1475-­‐1496.    doi:10.1111/   j.1751-­‐9004.2008.00118.x  

Fujita,   K.,   Eyal,   T.,   Chaiken,   S.,   Trope,   Y.,   &   Liberman,   N.   (2008).   Influencing   attitudes     toward   near   and   distant   objects.   Journal   of   Experimental   Social   Psychology,   44,  

  562-­‐572.  doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2007.10.005  

Fujita,   K.,   Gollwitzer,   P.   M.,   &   Oettingen,   G.   (2007).   Mindsets   and   pre-­‐conscious   open-­‐   mindedness  to  incidental  information.  Journal  of  Experimental  Social  Psychology,  

  43,  48-­‐61.  doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2005.12.004    

Fujita,   K.   &   Han,   H.   A.   (2009).   Moving   beyond   deliberative   control   of   impulses:   The     effect   of   construal   levels   on   evaluative   associations   in   self-­‐control   conflicts.  

  Psychological  Science,  20,  799-­‐804.  doi:10.1111/j.1467-­‐9280.2009.02372.x  

Fujita,   K.,   &   Roberts,   J.   C.   (2010).   Promoting   prospective   self-­‐control   through     abstraction.   Journal   of   Experimental   Social   Psychology,   46,   1049-­‐1054.   doi:     10.1016/j.jesp.2010.05.013    

Fujita,   K.,   &   Sasota,   J.   A.   (2011).   The   effects   of   construal   levels   on   asymmetric   t   emptation-­‐goal   associations.   Social   Cognition,   29,   125-­‐146.   doi:   10.1521/soco.   2011.29.2.125  

Fujita,   K.,   Trope,   Y.,   Liberman,   N.,   &   Levin-­‐Sagi,   M.   (2006).   Construal   levels   and   self-­‐   control.   Journal   of   Personality   and   Social   Psychology,   90,   351-­‐367.     doi:10.1037/0022-­‐3514.90.3.351  

Gable,   P.   &   Harmon-­‐Jones,   E.   (2008).   Approach-­‐motivated   positive   affect   reduces     breadth   of   attention.   Psychological   Science,   19,   476-­‐482.   doi:10.1111/j.1467-­‐   9280.2008.02112.x  

Gable,   P.,   &   Harmon-­‐Jones,   E.   (2010a).   The   blues   broaden,   but   the   nasty   narrows:     Attentional  consequences    of   negative   affects   low   and   high   in   motivational     intensity.  Psychological  Science,  21,  211-­‐215.  doi:10.1177/0956797609359622   Gable,   P.   &   Harmon-­‐Jones,   E.   (2010b).   The   motivational   dimensional   model   of   affect:     Implications   for   breadth   of   attention,   memory,   and   cognitive   categorization.  

(11)

Gasper,   K.   (2004).   Do   you   see   what   I   see?   Affect   and   visual   information   processing.  

  Cognition  and  Emotion,  18,  405-­‐421.  doi:10.1080/02699930341000068  

Gasper,  K.  &  Clore,  G.  L.  (2002).  Attending  to  the  big  picture:  Mood  and  global  vs.  local     processing   of   visual   information.   Psychological   Science,   13,   34-­‐40.     doi:10.1111/1467-­‐9280.00406  

Geen,   R.   G.   (1968).   Effects   of   frustration,   attack,   and   prior   training   in   aggressiveness     upon   aggressive   behavior.   Journal   of   Personality   and   Social   Psychology,   9,   316-­‐   321.  doi:10.1037/h0026054  

Geen   R.   G.,   &   Berkowitz,   L.   (1967).   Some   conditions   facilitating   the   occurrence   of     aggression  after  the  observation  of  violence.  Journal  of  Personality,  35,  666-­‐676.     doi:10.1111/j.1467-­‐6494.1967.tb01455.x  

Giacomantonio,  M.,  De  Dreu,  C.K.W.,  &  Manneti,  L.  (2010).  Now  you  see  it,  now  you  don’t:     Interests,  issues,  and  psychological  distance  in  integrative  negotiation.  Journal  of  

  Personality  and  Social  Psychology,  98,  761  –  774.  doi:  10.1037/a0017879  

Gollwitzer,   P.   M.   (1990).   Action   phases   and   mind-­‐sets.   In   E.   T.   Higgins   &   R.   M.     Sorrentino   (Eds.),   The   handbook   of   motivation   and   cognition:   Foundations   of     social  behavior  (Vol.  2,  pp.  53-­‐92).  New  York:  Guilford  Press.    

Gollwitzer,  P.  M.,  &  Kirchhof,  O.  (1998).  The  wilful  pursuit  of  identity.  In  J.  Heckhausen     &   C.   S.   Dweck   (Eds.),  Life-­‐span  perspectives  on  motivation  and  control  (pp.   389-­‐   423).  New  York:  Cambridge  University    Press.    

Gollwitzer,   P.M.   (1999).   Implementation   intentions:   Strong   effects   of   simple   plans.  

  American  Psychologist,  54,  493–503.  doi:  10.1037/0003-­‐066X.54.7.493  

Harmon-­‐Jones,   E.,     Gable,   P.   A.,   Price,   T.   F.  (2011).   Toward   an   understanding   of   the     influence   of   affective   states   on   attentional   tuning:   Comment   on   Friedman   and     Förster  (2010).  Psychological  Bulletin,  508–512.  doi:10.1037/a0022744  

Hayes,  A.  F.,  &  Matthes,  J.  (2009).  Computational  procedures  for  probing  interactions  in     OLS  and  logistic  regression:  SPSS  and  SAS  implementations.    Behavior  Research  

(12)

Heckhausen,   H.   &   Gollwitzer,   P.   M.   (1987).   Thought   contents   and   cognitive     functioning   in   motivational   versus   volitional   states   of   mind.   Motivation   and  

  Emotion,  11,  101-­‐120.  doi:  10.1007/BF00992338  

Henderson,   M.D.   (2011).   Mere   physical   distance   and   integrative   agreements:   When     more   space   improves   negotiation   outcomes.   Journal   of   Experimental   Social  

  Psychology,  47,  7–15.  doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2010.07.011    

Henderson,  M.D.,  Trope,  Y.,  &  Carnevale,  P.J.  (2006).  Negotiation  from  a  near  and  distant     time   perspective.   Journal   of   Personality   and   Social   Psychology,   91,   712-­‐729.     doi:10.1037/0022-­‐3514.91.4.712  

Higgins,  E.  T.  (2000).  Making  a  good  decision.  Value  from  fit.  American  Psychologist,  55,     1217-­‐1230.  doi:  10.1037/0003-­‐066X.55.11.1217  

Higgins,   E.   T.   (2005).   Value   from   regulatory   fit.   Current   Directions   in   Psychological  

  Science,  14,  209-­‐213.  doi:10.1111/j.0963-­‐7214.2005.00366.x  

Higgins,   E.   T.   (2006).   Value   from   hedonic   experience   and   engagement.   Psychological  

  Review,  113,  439-­‐460.  doi:10.1037/0033-­‐295X.113.3.439  

Higgins,  E.  T.,  Friedman,  R.  S.,  Harlow,  R.  E.,  Idson,  L.  C.,  Ayduk,  O.  N.,  &  Taylor,  A.  (2001).     Achievement  orientations  from  subjective  histories  of  success:  Promotion  pride     versus   prevention   pride.   European   Journal   of   Social   Psychology,   31,   3-­‐23.     doi:10.1002/ejsp.27  

Higgins,   E.   T.,   Marguc,   J.,   &   Scholer,   A.   A.   (2011).   Working   under   adversity:   How     opposing   versus   coping   affects   value.   Unpublished   working   paper.   Columbia     University.    

Higgins,  E.  T.,  &  Scholer,  A.  A.  (2009).  Engaging  the  consumer:  The  science  and     art   of     the  value  creation     process.   Journal   of   Consumer   Psychology,   19,   100-­‐114.     doi:10.1016/j.jcps.2009.02.002    

Hogg,   M.   A.   (2001).   A   social   identity   theory   of   leadership.   Personality   and   Social  

  Psychology  Review,  5,  184–200.  doi:  10.1207/S15327957PSPR0503_1  

Hogg,  M.  A.,  &  Reid,  S.  A.  (2001).  Social  identity,  leadership,  and  power.  In  A.  Y.  Lee-­‐Chai     &  J.  A.  Bargh  (Eds.),    The   use   and   abuse   of   power:   Multiple   perspectives   on   the  

(13)

Isen,  A.  M.,  &  Daubman,  K.  A.  (1984).  The  influence  of  affect  on  categorization.  Journal  of  

  Personality   and   Social   Psychology,   47,   1206-­‐1217.   doi:10.1037/0022-­‐

  3514.47.6.1206  

Isen,  A.  M.,  Daubman,  K.  A.,  &  Nowicki,  G.  P.  (1987).  Positive  affect  facilitates  creative     problem  solving.   Journal   of   Personality   and   Social   Psychology,   52,   1122-­‐1131.     doi:10.1037/0022-­‐3514.52.6.1122  

Jia,  L.,  Hirt,  E.,  &  Karpen,  S.  (2009).  Lessons  from  a  faraway  land:  The  effect  of  spatial     distance   on   creative   cognition.   Journal   of   Experimental   Social   Psychology,   45,     1127–1131.  doi:  10.1016/j.jesp.2009.05.015    

Johnson,   P.   O.,   &   Neyman,   J.   (1936).   Tests   of   certain   linear   hypotheses   and   their     applications  to  some  educational  problems.  Statistical  Research  Memoirs,  1,  57–   93.  

Jostmann,  N.  B.,  &  Koole,  S.  L.  (2009).  When  persistence  is  futile:  A  functional  analysis  of     action  orientation  and  goal  disengagement.  In  G.  B.     Moskowitz   and   H.   Grant     (Eds.),  The  psychology  of  goals  (pp.  337-­‐  361).    New  York,  NY:  Guilford.  

Judd,   C.   M.,   &   McClelland,   G.   H.   (1989).   Data   analysis:   A   model   comparison   approach.     New  York,  NY:  Harcourt  Brace  Jovanovich.  

Kashdan,  T.  B.,  Gallagher,  M.  W.,  Silvia,  P.  J.,  Winterstein,  B.  P.,  Breen,  W.  E.,     Terhar,     D.,  &  Steger,  M.  F.  (2009).     The   Curiosity   and   Exploration   Inventory-­‐II:     Development,   factor   structure,   and   psychometrics.   Journal   of   Research   in  

  Personality,  43,  987-­‐998.  doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2009.04.011    

Kashdan,  T.  B.,  Rose,  P.,  &  Fincham,  F.  D.  (2004).  Curiosity  and  exploration:  Facilitating     positive  subjective    experiences   and   personal   growth   opportunities.   Journal   of  

  Personality  Assessment,  82,  291-­‐305.  doi:10.1207/s15327752jpa8203_05  

Kazén,   M.,   Baumann,   N.,   &   Kuhl,   J.   (2005).   Self-­‐regulation   after   mortality   salience:     National  pride  feelings  of     action-­‐oriented   German   participants.   European  

  Psychologist,  10,  218-­‐228.  doi:10.1027/1016-­‐9040.10.3.218  

Kimchi,   R.,   &   Palmer,   S.   E.   (1982).   Form   and   texture   in   hierarchically   constructed     patterns.  Journal  of    Experimental   Psychology:   Human   Perception   and  

(14)

Kinchla,   R.   A.,   &   Wolfe,   J.   M.   (1979).   The   order   of   visual   processing:   “Top-­‐down,”     “bottom-­‐up”,  or  “middle-­‐   out”.   Perception   &   Psychophysics,   25(3),   225-­‐231.  

  doi:10.3758/BF03202991  

Kivetz,  Y.,  &  Tyler,  T.  R.  (2007).  Tomorrow  I'll  be  me:  The  effect  of  time  perspective  on     the   activation   of   idealistic   versus   pragmatic   selves.   Organizational   Behavior  

  and  Human  Decision  Processes,  102,  193-­‐211.  doi:10.1016/j.obhdp.2006.07.002  

Klinger,  E.  (1975).  Consequences  of  commitment  to  and  disengagement  from  incentives.  

  Psychological  Review,  82,  1-­‐25.  doi:10.1037/h0076171  

Koch,   S.,   Holland,   R.   W.,   Hengstler,   M.,   &   van   Knippenberg,   A.   (2009).   Body     locomotion  as  regulatory     process:   Stepping   backward   enhances   cognitive     control.   Psychological   Science,   20,   549-­‐550.   doi:10.1111/j.1467-­‐9280.2009.   02342.x  

Kray,   L.   J.,   Galinsky,   A.   D.,   &   Wong,   E.   (2006).   Thinking   within   the   box.   The     relational   processing   style   elicited   by   counterfactual   mind-­‐sets.   Journal   of  

  Personality  and  Social  Psychology,  91,  33-­‐48.  doi:10.1037/0022-­‐3514.91.1.33  

Kross,  E.,  &  Ayduk,  O.  (2008).  Facilitating  adaptive  emotional  analysis:  Short-­‐term  and     long-­‐term  outcomes  distinguishing  distanced-­‐analysis  of  negative  emotions  from     immersed-­‐analysis  and  distraction.  Personality  and  Social  Psychology  Bulletin,  34,     924-­‐938.  doi:10.1177/0146167208315938  

Kross,   E.,   &   Ayduk,   O.   (2011).   Making   meaning   out   of   negative   experiences   by   self-­‐   distancing.  Current    Directions   in   Psychological   Science,   20(3),   187-­‐191.  

  doi:10.1177/0963721411408883  

Kruglanski,   A.,   &   Jaffe,   Y.   (1988).   Curing   by   Knowing:   The   epistemic   approach   to     cognitive  therapy.  In  L.  Abramson  (Ed.),  Social  Cognition  and  Clinical  Psychology.     New  York:  Guilford  Press.  

Kruglanski,  A.  W.,  Shah,  J.  Y.,  Fishbach,  A.,  Friedman,  R.,  Chun,  W.  Y.,  &  Sleeth-­‐Keppler,  D.     (2002).  A  theory  of     goal-­‐systems.   In   M.   Zanna   (Ed.),   Advances   in   experimental  

  social  psychology  (Vol.  34,  pp.  331–376).  New  York:  Academic  Press.  

Kruglanski,  A.  W.,  Thompson,  E.  P.,  Higgins,  E.  T.,  Atash,  M.  N.,  Pierro,  A.,  Shah,  J.  Y.,  &     Spiegel,   S.   (2000).   To   "do   the   right   thing"   or   to   "just   do   it":   Locomotion   and     assessment   as   distinct   self-­‐regulatory   imperatives.   Journal   of   Personality   and  

(15)

  Social  Psychology,  79,  793-­‐815.  doi:10.1037/0022-­‐3514.79.5.793  

Kuhl,   J.   (n.d.)   Kurzanweisung   zum   Fragebogen   HAKEMP-­‐90   [Abbreviated     instructions  for  the  ACS-­‐90].   Retrieved   from   www.psychologie.uzh.ch/

  fachrichtungen/motivation/Studium/lehre/alt/ws0506/methodenIWS0506/

  hakemp90.pdf  

Kuhl,   J.   (1983).   Motivation,   Konflikt   und   Handlungskontrolle   [Motivation,   Conflict,   and     Action  Control].  Berlin.  Heidelberg,  New  York:  Springer.    

Kuhl,  J.  (1994a).  A  theory  of  action  and  state  orientation.  In  J.  Kuhl  &  J.  Beckmann  (Eds.),  

  Volition  and     personality:   Action   versus   state   orientation   (pp.   9-­‐46).   Göttingen:  

  Hogrefe.  

Kuhl,  J.  (1994b).  Action  versus  state  orientation:  Psychometric  properties  of  the  Action-­‐   Control-­‐Scale  (ACS-­‐  90).  In  J.  Kuhl  &  J.  Beckmann  (Eds.),  Volition  and  personality:  

  Action  versus  state  orientation  (pp.  47-­‐56).  Göttingen:  Hogrefe.  

Lamb,  M.  R.  &  Robertson,  L.  C.  (1988).  The  processing  of  hierarchical  stimuli:  Effects  of     retinal   locus,   locational   uncertainty   and   stimulus   identity.   Perception   &  

  Psychophysics,  44,  172-­‐181.  doi:10.3758/BF03208710  

Latham,  G.  P.,  &  Locke,  E.  A.  (1991).  Self-­‐regulation  through  goal  setting.  Organizational  

  behavior   and   human   decision   processes,   50,   212-­‐247.   doi:10.1016/0749-­‐

  5978(91)90021-­‐K  

Lazarus,  R.  S.,  &  Folkman,  S.  (1984).  Stress,  appraisal,  and  coping.  New  York:  Springer.   Lewin,   K.   (1935).   A   dynamic   theory   of   personality:   Selected   papers.   New   York,   NY:     McGraw  Hill.  

Lewis,   M.,   &   Ramsay,   D.   (2005).   Infant   emotional   cortisol   responses   to   goal   blockage.  

  Child  Development,  76,  518-­‐530.  doi:10.1111/j.1467-­‐8624.2005.00860.x  

Liberman,   N.,   &   Förster,   J.   (2009a).   The   effect   of   psychological   distance   on     perceptual   construal.   Cognitive   Science,   33,   1330-­‐1341.   doi:10.1111/j.1551-­‐   6709.2009.01061.x  

(16)

Liberman,  N.,  &  Förster,  J.  (2009b).  Distancing  from  experienced  self:  How  global  versus     local   perception   affects   estimation   of   psychological   distance.   Journal   of  

  Personality  and  Social  Psychology,  97,  203-­‐216.  doi:10.1037/a0015671  

Liberman,  N.,  Sagristano,  M.  D.,  &  Trope,  Y.  (2002).  The  effect  of  temporal  distance  on     level  of  mental  construal.  Journal  of  Experimental  Social  Psychology  ,  38,  523-­‐534.     doi:  10.1016/S0022-­‐1031(02)00535-­‐8    

Liberman,  N.,  &  Trope,  Y.  (1998).  The  role  of  feasibility  and  desirability  considerations     in  near  and  distant    future  decisions:  A  test  of  temporal  construal  theory.  Journal  

  of  Personality  and  Social  Psychology,  75,  5-­‐18.  doi:10.1037/0022-­‐3514.75.1.5  

Liberman,   N.   &   Trope,   Y.   (2008).   The   psychology   of   transcending   the   here   and   now.  

  Science,  322,  1201-­‐   1205.  doi:10.1126/science.1161958  

Liberman,   N.,   Trope,   Y.,   McCrea,   S.   M.,   &   Sherman,   S.   J.   (2007).   The   effect   of   level   of     construal  on  the  temporal  distance  of  activity  enactment.  Journal  of  Experimental  

  Social  Psychology,  43,  143-­‐149.  doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2005.12.009    

Liberman,   N.,   Trope,   Y.,   &   Stephan,   E.   (2007).   Psychological   distance.   In   A.   W.     Kruglanski  &  E.  T.  Higgins    (Eds.),   Social  psychology:  Handbook  of  basic  principles     (Vol.  2,  pp.  353-­‐383).  New  York,  NY:  Guilford  Press.  

Macrae,  C.  N.,  Bodenhausen,  G.  V.,  &  Milne,  A.  B.,  &  Jetten,  J.  (1994).  Out  of  mind  but  back     in  sight:  Stereotypes  on  the  rebound.  Journal  of  Personality  and  Social  Psychology,  

  67,  808–817.  doi:10.1037/0022-­‐3514.67.5.808  

Macrae,   N.   C.,   &   Lewis,   H.   L.   (2002).   Do   I   know   you?   Processing   orientation   and   face     recognition.    Psychological  Science,  13,  194-­‐196.  doi:  10.1111/1467-­‐9280.00436   Marguc,   J.,   Förster,   J.,   &   Van   Kleef,   G.   A.   (2011).   Stepping   back   to   see   the   big   picture:     When   obstacles   elicit   global   processing.   Journal   of   Personality   and   Social  

  Psychology.  101,  883-­‐901.  doi:  10.1037/a0025013  

Marguc,   J.,   Förster,   J.   &   Van   Kleef,   G.   A.   (2011).   A   camel   is   a   camel   is   a   vehicle...How  

  obstacles  promote  creative  goal  pursuit.  Manuscript  submitted  for  publication.    

Marguc,  J.,  Van  Kleef,  G.  A.,  &  Förster,  J.  (2010).  Obstacles:  Their  impact  on  thinking  and     beyond  thinking.  In     D.   A.   Contreras   (Ed.),   Psychology   of   thinking   (pp.   97-­‐120).     New  York:  Nova  Publishers.    

(17)

Marguc,  J.,  Van  Kleef,  G.  A.,  Förster,  J.  (in  press).  Stepping  back  while  staying  engaged:     When  facing  an  obstacle  increases  psychological  distance.  Social  Psychological  and  

  Personality  Science.  

Marsh,   R.   L.,   Bink,   M.   L.,   &   Hicks,   J.   L.   (1999).   Conceptual   priming   in   a   generative     problem-­‐solving   task.   Memory   and   Cognition,   27,   355-­‐363.   doi:10.3758/   BF03211419  

Marsh,  R.  L.,  Hicks,  J.  L.  &  Bink,  M.  L.  (1998).  Activation  of  completed,  uncompleted,  and     partially  completed     intentions.   Journal   of   Experimental   Psychology:   Learning,  

  Memory,  and  Cognition,  24,  350-­‐361.  doi:10.1037/0278-­‐7393.24.2.350  

Martin,   L.   L.,   &   Tesser,   A.   (1989).   Towards   a   motivational   and   structural   theory   of     ruminative  thought.  In  J.  S.  Uleman  &  J.  A.  Bargh  (Eds.),  Unintended  thought  (pp.     306-­‐326).  New  York,  NY:  Guilford  Press.  

Martin,   L.   L.,   &   Tesser,   A.   (1996).   Some   ruminative   thoughts.   In   R.   S.   Wyer   (Ed.),     Ruminative  thoughts  (pp.   1–47).  Hillsdale,  NJ:  Erlbaum.    

Martindale,  C.  (1981).  Cognition  and  consciousness.  Homewood,  IL:  Dorsey.  

Martindale,   C.   (1995).   Creativity   and   connectionism.   In   S.   M.   Smith,   T.   B.   Ward,   &   R.     Finke  (Eds.),  The  creative  cognition  approach  (pp.  249-­‐268).  Cambridge,  MA:  MIT     Press.  

McCrea,   S.   M.,   Liberman,   N.,   Trope,   Y.,   Sherman,   S.   J.   (2008).   Construal   level   and     procrastination.  Psychological  Science,  19,  1308-­‐1314.  doi:10.1111/j.1467-­‐9280.   2008.02240.x  

Mednick,   S.   A.   (1962).   The   associative   basis   of   the   creative   process.   Psychological  

  Review,  69,  220-­‐232.  

Metcalfe,  J.,  &  Mischel,  W.  (1999).  A  hot/cool  system  analysis  of  delay  of  gratification:     Dynamics  of    willpower.   Psychological   Review,   106,   3-­‐19.   doi:   10.1037/0033-­‐   295X.106.1.3  

Mischel,  W.,  Shoda,  Y.,  &  Rodriguez,  M.  L.  (1989).  Delay  of  gratification  in    children.  

(18)

Mischel,   W.   &   Masters,   J.   C.   (1966).   Effects   of   probability   of   reward   attainment   on     responses   to   frustration.   Journal   of   Personality   and   Social   Psychology,   3,   390-­‐   396.  doi:10.1037/h0022966  

Navon,   D.   (1977).   Forest   before   trees:   The   precedence   of   global   features   in   visual     perception.   Cognitive   Psychology,   9,   353-­‐383.   doi:10.1016/0010-­‐0285(77)   90012-­‐3  

Nemecek,   J.,   &   Grandjean,   E.   (1973).   Results   of   an   ergonomic   investigation   of   large-­‐   space  offices.  Human  Factors,  15,  111-­‐124.  

Novemsky,  N.,  Dhar,  R.,  Schwarz,  N.,  &  Simonson,  I.  (2007).  Preference  fluency  in  choice.  

  Journal  of  Marketing  Research,  44,  347-­‐356.  

Oatley   K.,   &   Johnson-­‐Laird,   P.   N.   (1987).   Towards   a   cognitive   theory   of   emotions.  

  Cognition  and  Emotion,  1,     29-­‐50.  doi:10.1080/02699938708408362  

Oettingen,  G.  (2000).  Expectancy  effects  on  behavior  depend  on  self-­‐regulatory  thought.  

  Social  Cognition,  18,  101-­‐129.  doi:10.1521/soco.2000.18.2.101  

Oettingen,   G.,   Hönig,   G.,   &   Gollwitzer,   P.   M.   (2000).   Effective   self-­‐regulation   of   goal     attainment.     International   Journal   of   Educational   Research,   33,   705-­‐732.     doi:10.1016/S0883-­‐0355(00)00046-­‐X  

Oettingen,   G.,   Pak,   H.,   &   Schnetter,   K.   (2001).   Self-­‐regulation   of   goal   setting:   Turning     free   fantasies   about   the   future   into   binding   goals.   Journal   of   Personality   and  

  Social  Psychology,  80,  736-­‐753.  doi:10.1037/0022-­‐3514.80.5.736  

Oettingen,   G.,   &   Stephens,   E.   J.   (2009).   Fantasies   and   motivationally   intelligent   goal     setting.  In  H.  Grant    &   B.B.   Moskowitz   (Eds.),   The   psychology   of   goals   (pp.   153-­‐   178).  New  York,  NY:  Guilford  Press.    

Oppenheimer,  D.  M.  (2008).  The  secret  life  of  fluency.  Trends  in  Cognitive    Science,   12(6),     237-­‐241.  doi:  10.1016/j.tics.2008.02.014  

Patton,   J.   H.,   Stanford,   M.   S.   &   Barratt,   E.   S.     (1995).   Factor   structure   of   the   Barratt     impulsiveness   scale.  Journal   of   Clinical   Psychology,   51,   768-­‐774.   doi:10.1002/   1097-­‐4679(199511)51:6<768::AID  JCLP2270510607>3.0.CO;2-­‐1  

(19)

Piaget,   J.   (1952).   The   origins   of   intelligence   in   children.   New   York:   International     Universities  Press.  

Piaget,  J.  (1980).  Adaptation  and  Intelligence.  London:  University  of  Chicago  Press.     Polman,  E.,  &  Emich,  K.  J.  (2011).  Decisions  for  others  are  more  creative  than  decisions     for  the  self.     Personality   and   Social   Psychology   Bulletin,   37,   492-­‐501,   doi:  

  10.1177/0146167211398362  

Preacher,   K.   J.,   &   Hayes,   A.   F.   (2008).     Asymptotic   and   resampling   strategies   for     assessing  and  comparing  indirect  effects  in  multiple  mediator  models.  Behavior  

  Research  Methods,  40,  879-­‐891.  doi:10.3758/BRM.40.3.879  

Proffitt,   D.   R.,   Stefanucci,   J.   K.,   Banton,   T.,   &   Epstein,   W.   (2003).   The   role   of   effort   in     perceiving  distance.    Psychological   Science,   14,   106–112.   doi:   10.1111/1467-­‐   9280.t01-­‐1-­‐01427  

Reder,  L.  M.  (1987).  Strategy  selection  in  question  answering.  Cognitive  Psychology,  19,     111-­‐138.  doi:  10.1016/0010-­‐0285(87)90005-­‐3  

Rietzschel,  E.  F.,  Nijstad,  B.  A.,  &  Stroebe,  W.  (2010),  The  selection  of  creative  ideas  after     individual   idea   generation:   Choosing   between   creativity   and   impact.   British  

  Journal  of  Psychology,  101,  47–68.  doi:  10.1348/000712609X414204  

Robertson,   L.   C.   (1996).   Attentional   persistence   for   features   of   hierarchical   patterns.  

  Journal   of   Experimental   Psychology:   General,   125,   227-­‐249.   doi:10.1037/0096-­‐

  3445.125.3.227  

Rosch,   E.   (1975).   Cognitive   representations   of   semantic   categories.   Journal   of  

  Experimental  Psychology:     General,   104,   192-­‐233.   doi:10.1037/0096-­‐3445.

  104.3.192  

Scherer,  K.   R.  (2001).  Appraisal   considered   as   a   process   of   multilevel   sequential     checking.  In  K.  R.  Scherer,    A.  Schorr,  &  T.  Johnstone  (Eds.),  Appraisal  processes  in  

  emotion:   Theory,   methods,   research   (pp.   92-­‐120).   New   York:   Oxford   University  

  Press.  

Scholer,  A.  A.  &  E.  T.  Higgins  (2009)  Exploring  the  complexities  of  value  creation:  The     role  of  engagement    strength.   Journal   of   Consumer   Psychology,   19,   137-­‐143.     doi:10.1016/j.jcps.2009.02.007  

(20)

Schooler,  J.  W.  (2002).  Verbalization  produces  a  transfer  inappropriate  processing  shift.  

  Applied  Cognitive  Psychology,  16,  989-­‐997.  doi:10.1002/acp.930  

Schooler,  J.  W.,  Fiore,  S.  M.,  &  Brandimonte,  M.  A.  (1997).  At  a  loss  from  words:  Verbal     overshadowing  of  perceptual  memories.  In  D.  L.  Medin  (Ed.),  The  psychology  of  

  learning  and  motivation  (pp.  293–334).    Hillsdale,  NJ:  Academic  Press.  

Schooler,  J.  W.  &  Melcher,  J.  (1995).  The  ineffability  of  insight.  In  S.  M.  Smith,  T.  B.  Ward   &  R.  A.  Finke  (Eds.),     The  creative  cognition  approach  (pp.  97-­‐   134).  

Cambridge,  MA:  MIT  Press.  

Schooler,   J.   W.,   Ohlsson,   S.,   &   Brooks,   K.   (1993).   Thoughts   beyond   words:   When     language  overshadows  insight.  Journal  of  Experimental  Psychology:  General,  122,     166-­‐183.  doi:10.1037/0096-­‐3445.122.2.166  

Schwarz,  N.,  &  Bless,  H.  (1991).  Happy  and  mindless,  but  sad  and  smart?  The  impact  of     affective  states  on     analytic   reasoning.   In   J.   P.   Forgas   (Ed.),   Emotion   and   social  

  judgments  (pp.  55–71).  Oxford,  England:  Pergamon  Press.  

Schwarz,  N.,  Bless,  H.,  Strack,  F.,  Klumpp,  G.,  Rittenauer-­‐Schatka,  H.,  &  Simons,  A.  (1991).     Ease  of  retrieval  as  information:  Another  look  at  the  availability  heuristic.  Journal  

  of   Personality   and   Social   Psychology,   61,   195-­‐202.   doi:10.1037/0022-­‐3514.

  61.2.195  

Schwarzer,  R.,  &  Jerusalem,  M.  (1995).  Generalized  Self-­‐Efficacy  scale.  In  J.  Weinman,  S.     Wright,  &  M.  Johnston,  Measures  in  health  psychology:  A  user’s  portfolio.  Causal  and  

  control  beliefs  (pp.  35-­‐  37).  Windsor,  England:  NFER-­‐NELSON.  

Simmons,  J.  P.,  &  Nelson,  L.  D.  (2006a).  Intuitive  confidence:  Choosing  between  intuitive     and   nonintuitive   alternatives.   Journal   of  Experimental  Psychology:  General,   135,     409-­‐428.  doi:10.1037/0096-­‐3445.135.3.409  

Stefanucci,   J.   K.,   Proffitt,   D.   R.,   Banton,   T.,   &   Epstein,   W.   (2005).   Distances   appear     different   on   hills.   Perception   &   Psychophysics,   67,   1052–1060.   doi:   10.3758/   BF03193631  

Stepper,   S.,   &   Strack,   F.   (1993).   Proprioceptive   determinants   of   emotional   and     nonemotional  feelings.  Journal  of  Personality  and  Social  Psychology,  64,  211–  220.     doi:10.1037/0022-­‐3514.64.2.211  

(21)

Stins,  J.  F.,  Roelofs,  K.,  Villan,  J.,  Kooijman,  K.,  Hagenaars,  M.  A.,  &  Beek,  P.  J.    (2011).  Walk     to  me  when  I  smile,    step  back  when  I'm  angry:  Emotional  faces  modulate  whole-­‐   body   approach-­‐avoidance   behaviors.  Experimental   Brain   Research,   4,   603-­‐611.     doi:10.1007/s00221-­‐011-­‐2767-­‐z  

Strack,  F.,  &  Neumann,  R.  (2000).  Furrowing  the  brow  may  undermine  perceived  fame:     The   role   of   facial   feedback   in   judgments   of   celebrity.   Personality   and   Social  

  Psychology  Bulletin,  26,  762-­‐768.    doi:10.1177/0146167200269002  

Stroop,   J.   R.   (1935).   Studies   of   interference   in   serial   verbal   reactions.   Journal   of  

  Experimental  Psychology,  18,  643-­‐662.  doi:10.1037/0096-­‐3445.121.1.15  

Strube,   M.   J.,   Turner,   C.   W.,   Cerro,   D.,   Stevens,   J.   &   Hinchey,   F.   (1984).   Interpersonal     aggression   and   the   Type   A   coronary-­‐prone   behavior   pattern:   A   theoretical     distinction   and   practical   implications.   Journal   of   Personality   and   Social  

  Psychology,  47,  839-­‐847.  doi:10.1037/0022-­‐3514.47.4.839  

Trope,  Y.,  &  Liberman,  N.  (2003).  Temporal  construal.  Psychological  Review,  110,  403-­‐   421.  doi:10.1037/0033-­‐295X.110.3.403  

Trope,   Y.,   &   Liberman,   N.   (2010).   Construal   level   theory   of   psychological   distance.  

  Psychological  Review,  117,  440–463.  doi:10.1037/a0018963  

Tulving,  E.,  &  Schacter,  D.  L.  (1990).  Priming  and  human  memory  systems.  Science,  247,     301-­‐306.  doi:10.1126/science.2296719  

Tversky,  A.,  &  Kahneman,  D.  (1973).  Availability:  A  heuristic  for  judging  frequency  and     probability.   Cognitive   Psychology,   5,   207-­‐232.   doi:10.1016/0010-­‐0285   (73)90033-­‐9  

Vallacher,   R.   R.,   &   Wegner,   D.   M.   (1987).   What   do   people   think   they’re   doing?   Action     identification  and     human   behavior.   Psychological   Review,   94,   3-­‐15.   doi:10.   1037/0033-­‐295X.94.1.3  

Vallacher,  R.  R.,  &  Wegner,  D.  M.  (1989).  Levels  of  personal  agency:  Individual  variation     in  action  identification.  Journal  of  Personality  and  Social  Psychology,  57,  660-­‐671.     doi:10.1037/0022-­‐  3514.57.4.660  

Van   Kleef,   G.   A.,   Anastasopoulou,   C.,   &   Nijstad,   B.   A.   (2010).   Can   expressions   of   anger     enhance   creativity?   A   test   of   the   emotions   as   social   information   (EASI)   model.  

(22)

  Journal  of  Experimental  Social  Psychology,  46,   1042-­‐1048.   doi:10.1016/j.jesp.   2010.05.015  

Van  Kleef,  G.  A.,  De  Dreu,  C.  K.  W.,  &  Manstead,  A.  S.  R.  (2004).  The  interpersonal  effects     of  emotions  in     negotiations:   A   motivated   information   processing   approach.  

  Journal   of   Personality   and   Social   Psychology,   87,   510-­‐528.   doi:   10.1037/0022-­‐

  3514.87.4.510  

Van   Koningsbruggen,   G.   M.,   Stroebe,   W.,   &   Aarts,   H.   (in   press).   Through   the   eyes   of   dieters:  Biased  size  perception  of  food  following  tempting  food  primes.  Journal  of  

  Experimental  Social  Psychology.  47,  293-­‐299.  doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2010.10.012  

Veltkamp,   M.,   Aarts,   H.,   Custers,   R.   (2008).   Perception   in   the   service   of   goal   pursuit:     Motivation  to  attain    goals   enhances   the   perceived   size   of   goal-­‐instrumental     objects.  Social  Cognition,  26,  720–736.  doi:  10.1521/soco.2008.26.6.720  

von  Hecker,  U.,  &  Meiser,  T.  (2005).  Defocused  attention  in  depressed  mood:  Evidence     from  source    monitoring.  Emotion,  5,  456-­‐463.  doi:10.1037/1528-­‐3542.5.4.456   Ward,   L.   M.   (1982).   Determinants   of   attention   to   local   and   global   features   of   visual     forms.   Journal   of   Experimental   Psychology:   Human   Perception   &   Performance,  

  8(4),  562-­‐581.  doi:10.1037/0096-­‐1523.8.4.562  

Ward,  T.  B.  (1994).  Structured  imagination:  The  role  of  category  structure  in  exemplar     generation.  Cognitive  Psychology,  27,  1-­‐40.  doi:10.1006/cogp.1994.1010    

Ward,  T.  B.  (1995).  What’s  old  about  new  ideas?  In  S.  M.  Smith,  T.  B.  Ward,  &  R.  A.  Finke     (Eds.),  The  creative  cognition  approach  (pp.157–178).  Cambridge,  MA:  MIT  Press.   Webster,  D.  M.,  &  Kruglanski,  A.  W.  (1994).  Individual  differences  in  need  for  cognitive     closure.   Journal   of   Personality   and   Social   Psychology,   67,   1049-­‐1062.     doi:10.1037/0022-­‐3514.67.6.1049  

Wegner,   D.   M.,   Connally,   D.,   Shearer,   D.,   &   Vallacher,   R.   R.   (1983).   [Disruption   and     identifications  of  the  act  of  eating.]  Unpublished  research  data.  

Wegner,  D.  M.,  &  Vallacher,  R.  R.  (1986).  Action  identification.  In  R.  M.  Sorrentino  &  E.  T.     Higgins   (Eds.),   Handbook   of   motivation   and   cognition:   Foundations   of   social  

Referenties

GERELATEERDE DOCUMENTEN

In my position as Science Librarian, I have been responsible for faculty liaison, collection development, reference and research and library instruction for my subject areas

If the number of surface species increases to three, for example CO(ads), OH(ads) and either free Pt sites or O(ads), two adsorption relaxations are needed, circuit 2L, in order

Against this complex contemporary social and cultural context, Tal-choom, as Korea’s popular theatre, exem­ plifies its current place and the future possibilities

But, above all, Bildung’s ideological force remains invisible (Gadamer’s &#34;atmosphere breathed”) so that individuals fieely consent to its demands; its subjects, that is,

Once the combined sets containing only good data points are identified, classical estimation methods such as the least-squares method and the maximum likelihood method can be applied

This thesis reduces the paucity of Canadian research on sexual minorities by showing lesbian and bisexual women (among a sample of residential treatment clients) use certain drugs

door Marjolein Veenendaal Op donderdag 25 oktober 2012 om 14.00 uur in de Agnietenkapel, Oudezijds Voorburgwal 231 te Amsterdam Receptie ter plaatse na afloop van de promotie.

Uiterwaal Faculteit der Geneeskunde Het onderzoek dat aan dit proefschrift ten grondslag ligt is mogelijk gemaakt door een subsidie van de Nederlandse Hartstichting