• No results found

References

Ahluwalia, R., Burnkrant, R. E., & Unnava, H. R. (2000). Consumer response to negative publicity: The moderating role of commitment. Journal of marketing research, 37(2), 203-214.

Aji, H. M., & Sutikno, B. (2015). The extended consequence of greenwashing: Perceived consumer skepticism. International Journal of Business and Information, 10(4), 433.

Akturan, U. (2018). How does greenwashing affect green branding equity and purchase intention? An empirical research. Marketing Intelligence & Planning.

Alhaddad, A. (2014). The effect of brand image and brand loyalty on brand equity.

International Journal of Business and Management Invention, 3(5), 28-32.

Atkinson, L., & Kim, Y. (2015). “I drink it anyway and I know I shouldn't”:

Understanding green consumers' positive evaluations of norm-violating non-green products and misleading green advertising. Environmental Communication, 9(1), 37-57.

Cheng, S. Y., White, T. B., & Chaplin, L. N. (2012). The effects of self-brand connections on responses to brand failure: A new look at the consumer–brand relationship. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 22(2), 280-288.

Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences. Hillsdle.

De Jong, M. D., Harkink, K. M., & Barth, S. (2018). Making green stuff? Effects of corporate greenwashing on consumers. Journal of business and technical communication, 32(1), 77-112.

Dean, D. H. (2004). Consumer reaction to negative publicity: Effects of corporate reputation, response, and responsibility for a crisis event. The Journal of Business Communication (1973), 41(2), 192-211.

Delmas, M. A., & Burbano, V. C. (2011). The drivers of greenwashing. California management review, 54(1), 64-87.

Escalas, J. E., & Bettman, J. R. (2003). You are what they eat: The influence of reference groups on consumers’ connections to brands. Journal of consumer psychology, 13(3), 339-348.

Ettinger, J. (2018). Coca-Cola’s ‘Sustainable’ PlantBottle Claims Full of Hot Air.

Organic Authority. Retrieved from

https://www.organicauthority.com/energetic-health/coca-colas-sustainable-plantbottle-claims-full-of-hot-air

Ewing, J. (2017). Engineering a Deception: What Led to Volkswagen’s Diesel Scandal.

New York Times. Retrieved from

https://www.nytimes.com/interactive/2017/business/volkswagen-diesel-emissions-timeline.html

Furlow, N. E. (2010). Greenwashing in the new millennium. The Journal of Applied Business and Economics, 10(6), 22.

Gelles, D. (2015). Social Responsibility That Rubs Right Off. New York Times. Retrieved from https://www.nytimes.com/2015/10/18/business/energy-environment/social-responsibility-that-rubs-right-off.html

Gorn, G., Pham, M. T., & Sin, L. Y. (2001). When arousal influences ad evaluation and valence does not (and vice versa). Journal of consumer Psychology, 11(1), 43-55.

Guo, R., Tao, L., Li, C. B., & Wang, T. (2017). A path analysis of greenwashing in a trust crisis among Chinese energy companies: The role of brand legitimacy and brand loyalty. Journal of Business Ethics, 140(3), 523-536.

Heitmann, M., Lehmann, D. R., & Herrmann, A. (2007). Choice goal attainment and decision and consumption satisfaction. Journal of marketing research, 44(2), 234-250.

Jeon, J. O., & Baeck, S. (2016). What drives consumer’s responses to brand crisis? The moderating roles of brand associations and brand-customer relationship strength.

Journal of Product & Brand Management.

Keller, K. L. (1993). Conceptualizing, measuring, and managing customer-based brand equity. Journal of marketing, 57(1), 1-22.

Kemp, E., Childers, C. Y., & Williams, K. H. (2012). Place branding: creating self‐brand connections and brand advocacy. Journal of Product & Brand Management.

Kwon, W. S., Englis, B., & Mann, M. (2016). Are third-party green–brown ratings believed?: The role of prior brand loyalty and environmental concern. Journal of Business Research, 69(2), 815-822.

Leonidou, C. N., & Skarmeas, D. (2017). Gray shades of green: Causes and

consequences of green skepticism. Journal of Business Ethics, 144(2), 401-415.

Lisjak, M., Lee, A. Y., & Gardner, W. L. (2012). When a threat to the brand is a threat to the self: The importance of brand identification and implicit self-esteem in

predicting defensiveness. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 38(9), 1120-1132.

Lyon, T. P., & Montgomery, A. W. (2015). The means and end of greenwash.

Organization & Environment, 28(2), 223-249.

Majláth, M. (2017). The effect of greenwashing information on ad evaluation. European Journal of Sustainable Development, 6(3), 92-92.

Matthes, J., & Wonneberger, A. (2014). The skeptical green consumer revisited: Testing the relationship between green consumerism and skepticism toward advertising.

Journal of advertising, 43(2), 115-127.

Mohr, L. A., Eroǧlu, D., & Ellen, P. S. (1998). The development and testing of a measure of skepticism toward environmental claims in marketers' communications. Journal of consumer affairs, 32(1), 30-55.

Nyilasy, G., Gangadharbatla, H., & Paladino, A. (2012). Greenwashing: A consumer perspective. Economics & Sociology, 5(2), 116.

Nyilasy, G., Gangadharbatla, H., & Paladino, A. (2014). Perceived greenwashing: The interactive effects of green advertising and corporate environmental performance on consumer reactions. Journal of Business Ethics, 125(4), 693-707.

Oliver, R. L. (1999). Whence consumer loyalty?. Journal of marketing, 63(4_suppl1), 33-44.

Parguel, B., Benoit-Moreau, F., & Russell, C. A. (2015). Can evoking nature in advertising mislead consumers? The power of ‘executional greenwashing'.

International Journal of Advertising, 34(1), 107-134.

Rea, B., Wang, Y. J., & Stoner, J. (2014). When a brand caught fire: the role of brand equity in product-harm crisis. Journal of Product & Brand Management.

Rindfleisch, A., Burroughs, J. E., & Wong, N. (2009). The safety of objects: Materialism, existential insecurity, and brand connection. Journal of Consumer Research, 36(1), 1-16.

Schmuck, D., Matthes, J., & Naderer, B. (2018). Misleading consumers with green advertising? An affect–reason–involvement account of greenwashing effects in environmental advertising. Journal of Advertising, 47(2), 127-145.

Siano, A., Vollero, A., Conte, F., & Amabile, S. (2017). “More than words”: Expanding the taxonomy of greenwashing after the Volkswagen scandal. Journal of Business Research, 71, 27-37.

Sweney, M. (2020). Ryanair accused of greenwash over carbon emissions claim. The Guardian. Retrieved from

https://www.theguardian.com/business/2020/feb/05/ryanair-accused-of-greenwash-over-carbon-emissions-claim

Thaler, V. S., Herbst, U., & Merz, M. A. (2018). A real product scandal’s impact on a high-equity brand: a new approach to assessing scandal impact. Journal of Product

& Brand Management.

van der Hoeven, D. (2013). Coca-Cola blundert met non-informatie over de plantbottle - laatste in een reeks incidenten. Bio Based Press. Retrieved from

https://www.biobasedpress.eu/nl/2013/09/coca-cola-blundert-met-non-informatie-over-de-plantbottle-laatste-in-een-reeks-incidenten/

Xiao, Z., Wang, Y., Ji, X., & Cai, L. (2021). Greenwash, moral decoupling, and brand loyalty. Social Behavior and Personality: an international journal, 49(4), 1-8.

Appendix

Appendix A: List of items for each variable Variable (Adopted from)

1. Perceived Consumer Skepticism (Aji & Sutikno, 2015)

• Most green claims in advertising are intended to mislead rather than to inform customers.

• I do not believe most green claims made in advertising.

• Because green claims are so exaggerated, consumers would be better off if such claims in advertising were eliminated.

2. Perceived Environmental Concern (Mohr, Eroǧlu & Ellen, 1998)

• I believe that environmental issues are unimportant - important

• I believe that environmental issues are something that does not really matter to me - … really matters to me

• I believe that environmental issues are not personally relevant - personally relevant

• I believe that environmental issues are uninvolving - involving

• I believe that environmental issues are of little concern to me - of great concern to me

3. Brand Loyalty (Heitmann, Lehmann, and Herrmann, 2007)

• It is very likely that I would purchase Coca-Cola again.

• I am willing to pay a price premium over competing products to be able to purchase Coca-Cola again.

• I would only consider purchasing Coca-Cola again, if it would be substantially cheaper. (R)

• Commercials regarding competing brands are not able to reduce my interest in buying Coca-Cola again.

• I would purchase Coca-Cola again, even if it receives bad evaluations by the media or other people.

4. Self-brand Connections (Escalas & Bettman, 2003)

• Coca-Cola reflects who I am

• I can identify with Coca-Cola.

• I feel a personal connection to Coca-Cola.

• I use Coca-Cola to communicate who I am to other people.

• I think Coca-Cola helps me become the type of person I want to be.

• I consider Coca-Cola to be “me” (it reflects who I consider myself to be or the way that I want to present myself to others)

• Coca-Cola suits me well.

5. Advertisement Evaluation (before greenwashing information) (Gorn, Pham &

Sin, 2001)

• The advertisement is unpleasant - pleasant

• The advertisement is bad - good

• I dislike - like the advertisement

• I react unfavorably - favorable to the advertisement

• I feel positive - negative to the advertisement (R)

• The advertisement is not fun to read - fun to read

6. Greenwashing information (Ettinger, 2018; van der Hoeven, 2013)

In 2013 the Danish consumer ombudsman declared Coca-Cola guilty of greenwashing its advertisements about its plant-based bottle, which debuted in 2009. The company claimed that the PlantBottle is environmental-friendly compared to traditional bottle packaging. Besides, Coca-Cola stated that it was reducing the companies carbon footprint with the adaptation of the PlantBottle. However, when asked for evidence of its green claims, Coca-Cola did not want to release the Life Cycle Assessment (LCA) of the PlantBottle. The Danish consumer ombudsman stated that Coca-Cola had exaggerated the environmental benefits of the PlantBottle as the company could not provide any proof. The company initially claimed that the PlantBottle saved up to 25 percent of CO2 emissions, but after the greenwashing scandal, they readjusted the percentage to 7.5.

7. Advertisement Evaluation (after greenwashing information) (Gorn, Pham &

Sin, 2001)

• The advertisement is unpleasant - pleasant

• The advertisement is bad - good

• I dislike - like the advertisement

• I react unfavorably - favorable to the advertisement

• I feel positive - negative to the advertisement (R)

• The advertisement is not fun to read - fun to read

*(R) = reversed score

Appendix B: Assumptions to test the hypotheses Assumptions hypothesis 1.

Assumption 1: Continuous dependent variable

This assumption holds since advertisement evaluation is measured at the interval level.

Assumption 2: Independent observations

Each participant completed the online survey without interacting with the other participants. Thus, the assumption of independent observations is met.

Assumption 3: The independent variable consists of related pairs

This assumption also holds because participants evaluated the same Coca-Cola advertisement on two occasions.

Assumption 4: Normality

The Normal Q-Q plot shows that the difference in advertisement evaluation is normally distributed. In addition, the skewness is 0.53 and the kurtosis is -0.70. Suggesting a normal distribution since the skewness lies between -2 and 2 and the kurtosis is smaller than 4.

Assumption 5: No outliers

The boxplot shows that the variable, difference in advertisement evaluation, does not contain any outliers.

All in all, the five assumptions are all met. Hence, the data can be used for the performance of a paired-samples t-test.

Assumptions hypothesis 2.

Assumption 1: The assumption of linearity

The scatterplot shows a positive linear relationship between environmental concern and the difference in advertisement evaluation.

Assumption 2: The independence of residuals

This assumption is not a concern for cross-sectional surveys. Thus, the independence of the residuals assumption is met.

Assumption 3: Normality of residuals

The Normal P-P Plot shows that the residuals are approximately normally distributed.

Assumption 4: Homoscedasticity of residuals

The scatterplot indicates that the residuals are approximately equally spread. Therefore, the assumption holds since there is homoscedasticity.

Assumption 5: No influential outliers

Three outliers were identified with the standardized residuals method using a cutoff value of two. The linear regressions results with the outliers differed considerably compared to the results when we remove the three outliers. For this reason, the three outliers are removed.

Regression results with outliers

B SE B t p

Environmental concern 0.50 .22 2.29 .025

N = 63.

R Square = .08.

Regression results without outliers

B SE B t p

Environmental concern 0.60 .28 2.10 .04

N = 60.

R Square = 0.07.

Assumption 6: No presence of multicollinearity

The VIF scores of environmental concern, brand loyalty, and self-brand connections indicate no presence of multicollinearity since the VIF scores are well below five. Namely, the VIF score of environmental concern is 1.02, the VIF score of brand loyalty is 1.37, and the VIF score of self-brand connections is 1.37.

All in all, the six assumptions are all met after the removal of three outliers.

Assumptions hypothesis 3.

Assumption 1: Independent observations

Each participant has completed the online survey individually. Thus, there was no interaction between the participants while they were making the online questionnaire.

Therefore, the assumption of independent observations has been met.

Assumption 2: Normality

This assumption presumes a normal distribution of the dependent variable. The Normal Q-Q plot indicates that the dependent variable, the difference in advertisement evaluation, is normally distributed. In addition, the skewness is 0.53, and the kurtosis is -0.70. Thus, the dependent variable, the difference in advertisement evaluation, is normally distributed since the skewness lies between -2 and 2, and the kurtosis is smaller than 4.

Assumption 3: Homogeneity

Levene’s test indicates the confirmation of the homogeneity assumption. Namely, Levene’s test is non-significant as the p-value .503 exceeds the significance level of .05.

Consequently, the null hypothesis, which suggests homogenous variances, is not rejected.

Assumption 4: Homogeneity of regression slopes

The interaction effect between environmental concern and self-brand connections has a p-value of 0.92. Since the p-p-value exceeds the significance level of .05, it can be assumed that there is homogeneity of regression slopes.

Assumption 5: Linearity

This assumption entails that the relationship between the covariate and the dependent variable must be linear. The scatterplot demonstrates a positive linear relationship between environmental concern and the difference in advertisement evaluation.

All in all, the five assumptions have been verified.

Assumptions hypothesis 4.

Assumption 1: Independent observations

Participants conducted the survey individually. Hence, the independent observations assumption holds for this particular dataset.

Assumption 2: Normality

The Normal Q-Q plot verifies the normal distribution of the dependent variable, the difference in advertisement evaluation. The skewness (0.53) and the kurtosis (-0.70) also indicate a normal distribution since the skewness lies between -2 and 2, and the kurtosis is smaller than 4.

Assumption 3: Homogeneity

Levene’s test indicates the verification of the homogeneity assumption. Namely, the p-value of .079 is bigger than the significant p-value of .05. Thus, the null hypothesis, which states that the variances are homogenous, is not rejected. Therefore, the assumption of homogeneity is met.

Assumption 4: Homogeneity of regression slopes

The homogeneity of regression slopes assumption is met because the interaction effect between environmental concern and brand loyalty exceeds the significance level of 0.05.

Namely, the p-value of the interaction effect is .79.

GERELATEERDE DOCUMENTEN